During the COVID-19 pandemic participants prefer settings with a face mask, no interaction and at a closer distance

Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulate...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inScientific reports Vol. 12; no. 1; pp. 12777 - 12
Main Authors Kühne, K., Fischer, M. H., Jeglinski-Mende, M. A.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published London Nature Publishing Group UK 27.07.2022
Nature Publishing Group
Nature Portfolio
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.
AbstractList Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers' task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers' task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.
ArticleNumber 12777
Author Kühne, K.
Jeglinski-Mende, M. A.
Fischer, M. H.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: K.
  surname: Kühne
  fullname: Kühne, K.
  email: kkuehne@uni-potsdam.de
  organization: Cognitive Sciences Division, University of Potsdam
– sequence: 2
  givenname: M. H.
  surname: Fischer
  fullname: Fischer, M. H.
  organization: Cognitive Sciences Division, University of Potsdam
– sequence: 3
  givenname: M. A.
  surname: Jeglinski-Mende
  fullname: Jeglinski-Mende, M. A.
  organization: Cognitive Sciences Division, University of Potsdam
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35896701$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kktv1DAURiNUREvpH2CBLLFhQcCP2Ik3SNWUx0iVugG2luPczHjI2IPtAfHvuTMppWVBNrGdc49z7e9pdRJigKp6zugbRkX3NjdM6q6mnNdMtYLW7FF1xmkjay44P7k3Pq0uct5QfCTXDdNPqlMhO61ays6qfLVPPqxIWQNZ3HxdXtVMk50NA2y9w0Eq3nmcl0x2CUZIJEMpWJHJT1_WxJLROiBbm7-9JiESHwok64qPgaCF2IKIm2LGysHnYoODZ9Xj0U4ZLm7f59WXD-8_Lz7V1zcfl4vL69rJhpaa9brRToMatGvoALKnclDgrMIm6Mgk7Ts-yhGEkOMgRy0dAqyxSvOOWybOq-XsHaLdmF3yW5t-mWi9OS7EtDLH_iYwCm3Ssr4VwJqWDbiHoFxzXLFUtT263s2u3b7fwuAglGSnB9KHX4Jfm1X8YbTgiokOBa9uBSl-30MuZuuzg2myAeI-G660whtqOUf05T_oJu5TwKM6UFIoxARSfKZcijnj3dz9DKPmEBEzR8RgRMwxIuZwJC_ut3FX8icQCIgZyLtDLiD93fs_2t-fM8dQ
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1007_s10339_024_01179_z
crossref_primary_10_1007_s44202_023_00066_6
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_34667_x
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_55218_y
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_024_00570_w
crossref_primary_10_3390_bs14010020
crossref_primary_10_1002_ijop_12921
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_023_00528_4
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychsport_2023_102509
crossref_primary_10_3390_healthcare11121711
crossref_primary_10_4236_pp_2023_145011
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijchp_2024_100476
Cites_doi 10.1037/a0028347
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.001
10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.006
10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01412
10.1016/j.cub.2021.06.001
10.48550/ARXIV.1406.5823
10.1007/s00429-005-0039-z
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0607-12.2012
10.1038/s41598-021-84806-5
10.1007/s00221-021-06080-1
10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008
10.1371/journal.pone.0243023
10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x
10.2307/2786027
10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045
10.9758/cpn.2018.16.1.18
10.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X
10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.004
10.3758/s13428-016-0783-4
10.1037/a0017387
10.1371/journal.pone.0049162
10.1007/s00426-003-0157-2
10.1038/s41598-022-06086-x
10.1002/ejsp.2082
10.2139/ssrn.3641367
10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14
10.1037/0021-843X.95.2.131
10.1177/0022022117698039
10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092
10.1007/s00221-021-06040-9
10.1007/s00426-019-01242-x
10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00657
10.1007/s00221-017-5115-0
10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024
10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004
10.1038/s41598-021-90714-5
10.1038/s41598-021-81780-w
10.1371/journal.pone.0255598
10.1371/journal.pone.0192753
10.1007/s10339-012-0470-z
10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008
10.5334/joc.72
10.1007/s00426-016-0782-1
10.3389/fnhum.2021.636124
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x
10.1101/2021.04.25.441303
10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043
10.1017/dmp.2020.295
10.1007/s00426-016-0806-x
10.1177/0031512520973515
10.1371/journal.pone.0249792
10.1038/s41598-019-45224-w
10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00959.x
10.1038/s41562-020-00977-7
10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565
10.31234/osf.io/jfeca
10.1177/0018720820956858
10.1177/0146167205275613
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright The Author(s) 2022
2022. The Author(s).
The Author(s) 2022. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Copyright_xml – notice: The Author(s) 2022
– notice: 2022. The Author(s).
– notice: The Author(s) 2022. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
DBID C6C
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
3V.
7X7
7XB
88A
88E
88I
8FE
8FH
8FI
8FJ
8FK
ABUWG
AFKRA
AZQEC
BBNVY
BENPR
BHPHI
CCPQU
DWQXO
FYUFA
GHDGH
GNUQQ
HCIFZ
K9.
LK8
M0S
M1P
M2P
M7P
PIMPY
PQEST
PQQKQ
PQUKI
Q9U
7X8
5PM
DOA
DOI 10.1038/s41598-022-16730-1
DatabaseName Springer Nature OA Free Journals
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
CrossRef
ProQuest Central (Corporate)
Health & Medical Collection (Proquest)
ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)
Biology Database (Alumni Edition)
Medical Database (Alumni Edition)
Science Database (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
Hospital Premium Collection
Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
ProQuest Central Essentials
Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Central
Health Research Premium Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Central Student
SciTech Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Biological Sciences
Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)
PML(ProQuest Medical Library)
ProQuest Science Journals
Biological Science Database
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central Basic
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
DatabaseTitle MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
CrossRef
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
SciTech Premium Collection
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Biology Journals (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central
Health Research Premium Collection
Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition)
Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Central Korea
Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni)
ProQuest Science Journals (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central Basic
ProQuest Science Journals
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
ProQuest Hospital Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
Biological Science Database
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete
ProQuest Medical Library
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList
Publicly Available Content Database

CrossRef
MEDLINE

MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: C6C
  name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals
  url: http://www.springeropen.com/
  sourceTypes: Publisher
– sequence: 2
  dbid: DOA
  name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 3
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 4
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 5
  dbid: BENPR
  name: ProQuest Central
  url: https://www.proquest.com/central
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Biology
EISSN 2045-2322
EndPage 12
ExternalDocumentID oai_doaj_org_article_61505a1b73e1471da64302921b7a067b
10_1038_s41598_022_16730_1
35896701
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Universität Potsdam (1031)
– fundername: ;
GroupedDBID 0R~
3V.
4.4
53G
5VS
7X7
88A
88E
88I
8FE
8FH
8FI
8FJ
AAFWJ
AAJSJ
AAKDD
ABDBF
ABUWG
ACGFS
ACSMW
ADBBV
ADRAZ
AENEX
AFKRA
AJTQC
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
AZQEC
BAWUL
BBNVY
BCNDV
BENPR
BHPHI
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C6C
CCPQU
DIK
DWQXO
EBD
EBLON
EBS
ESX
FYUFA
GNUQQ
GROUPED_DOAJ
GX1
HCIFZ
HH5
HMCUK
HYE
KQ8
LK8
M0L
M1P
M2P
M48
M7P
M~E
NAO
OK1
PIMPY
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
RIG
RNT
RNTTT
RPM
SNYQT
UKHRP
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
AAYXX
AFPKN
CITATION
7XB
8FK
K9.
PQEST
PQUKI
Q9U
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-1b949c9e6d9c40de5b05d6eca64190f150b82f5fe335fd5f95c05d14a69282a13
IEDL.DBID RPM
ISSN 2045-2322
IngestDate Tue Oct 22 15:14:34 EDT 2024
Tue Sep 17 21:17:03 EDT 2024
Sat Oct 26 04:08:24 EDT 2024
Sun Oct 27 16:55:05 EDT 2024
Fri Aug 23 02:47:42 EDT 2024
Sat Nov 02 12:30:23 EDT 2024
Fri Oct 11 20:53:32 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 1
Language English
License 2022. The Author(s).
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c540t-1b949c9e6d9c40de5b05d6eca64190f150b82f5fe335fd5f95c05d14a69282a13
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9326138/
PMID 35896701
PQID 2695360573
PQPubID 2041939
PageCount 12
ParticipantIDs doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_61505a1b73e1471da64302921b7a067b
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9326138
proquest_miscellaneous_2696005722
proquest_journals_2695360573
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_16730_1
pubmed_primary_35896701
springer_journals_10_1038_s41598_022_16730_1
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2022-07-27
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2022-07-27
PublicationDate_xml – month: 07
  year: 2022
  text: 2022-07-27
  day: 27
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace London
PublicationPlace_xml – name: London
– name: England
PublicationTitle Scientific reports
PublicationTitleAbbrev Sci Rep
PublicationTitleAlternate Sci Rep
PublicationYear 2022
Publisher Nature Publishing Group UK
Nature Publishing Group
Nature Portfolio
Publisher_xml – name: Nature Publishing Group UK
– name: Nature Publishing Group
– name: Nature Portfolio
References NosekBAModerators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluationJ. Exp. Psychol. Gen.200513456558416316292144067610.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565
IachiniTRuggieroGCan I put myself in your shoes? Sharing peripersonal space reveals the simulation of the action possibilities of othersExp. Brain Res.2021239103510453352323910.1007/s00221-021-06040-9
LisiMPScattolinMFusaroMAgliotiSMA Bayesian approach to reveal the key role of mask wearing in modulating projected interpersonal distance during the first COVID-19 outbreakPLoS ONE2021161:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhvVWnsLbL34375361835447110.1371/journal.pone.0255598
LeDoux, J. E. & & Armony, J. L. How danger is encoded: towards a systems, cellular, and computational understanding of cognitive-emotional interactions in fear circuits. in The cognitive neurosciences 1067–1079 (Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press, 1999).
JeonHLeeS-HFrom Neurons to Social Beings: Short Review of the Mirror Neuron System Research and Its Socio-Psychological and Psychiatric ImplicationsClin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci.201816183129397663581045610.9758/cpn.2018.16.1.18
SakumaNIkedaKEffects of modulated emotion and facial masking encounter on personal distancePercept. Mot. Skills20211281781903320218010.1177/0031512520973515
Inmagine Lab Pte Ltd. 123rf.
HolmesNPCalvertGASpenceCExtending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of toolsNeurosci. Lett.200437262671:CAS:528:DC%2BD2cXpsFWhsbo%3D1553108910.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024
WelschRHechtHChuangLvon CastellCInterpersonal Distance in the SARS-CoV-2 CrisisHum. Factors J. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc.2020621095110110.1177/0018720820956858
MathewsAMacLeodCDiscrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety statesJ. Abnorm. Psychol.1986951311381:STN:280:DyaL283isV2htg%3D%3D371143610.1037/0021-843X.95.2.131
FerriFTajadura-JiménezAVäljamäeAVastanoRCostantiniMEmotion-inducing approaching sounds shape the boundaries of multisensory peripersonal spaceNeuropsychologia2015704684752574486910.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.001
VagnoniELewisJTajadura-JiménezACardiniFListening to a conversation with aggressive content expands the interpersonal spacePLoS ONE20181329590110587393610.1371/journal.pone.01927531:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXit1aju73E
BourgeoisJFarnèACoelloYCosts and benefits of tool-use on the perception of reachable spaceActa Psychol. (Amst.)2014148919510.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008
JuddCMWestfallJKennyDATreating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problemJ. Pers. Soc. Psychol.201210354692261266710.1037/a0028347
SerinoABassolinoMFarnèALàdavasEExtended multisensory space in blind cane usersPsychol. Sci.2007186426481761487410.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x
NeumannRHülsenbeckKSeibtBAttitudes towards people with AIDS and avoidance behavior: Automatic and reflective bases of behaviorJ. Exp. Soc. Psychol.20044054355010.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.006
IachiniTPeripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: Effects of gender and ageJ. Environ. Psychol.20164515416410.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004
KroczekLOHBöhmeSMühlbergerAFace masks reduce interpersonal distance in virtual realitySci. Rep.20221222132022NatSR..12.2213K1:CAS:528:DC%2BB38Xjt1Cltr0%3D35140279882885010.1038/s41598-022-06086-x
RuggieroGRapuanoMIachiniTPerceived temperature modulates peripersonal and interpersonal spaces differently in men and womenJ. Environ. Psychol.201963525910.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.004
GrundmannFEpstudeKScheibeSFace masks reduce emotion-recognition accuracy and perceived closenessPLoS ONE2021161:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhtVWms7jL33891614806459010.1371/journal.pone.0249792
RizzolattiGThe mirror neuron system and its function in humansAnat. Embryol. (Berl.)200521041942110.1007/s00429-005-0039-z
CartaudARuggieroGOttLIachiniTCoelloYPhysiological response to facial expressions in peripersonal space determines interpersonal distance in a social interaction contextFront. Psychol.2018965729867639594986510.3389/fpsyg.2018.00657
TeneggiCCanzoneriEdi PellegrinoGSerinoASocial modulation of peripersonal space boundariesCurr. Biol.2013234064111:CAS:528:DC%2BC3sXit12jtbw%3D2339483110.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043
HofmannWGawronskiBGschwendnerTLeHSchmittMA meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measuresPers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.200531136913851614366910.1177/0146167205275613
PinkhamAEGriffinMBaronRSassonNJGurRCThe face in the crowd effect: Anger superiority when using real faces and multiple identitiesEmotion2010101411462014131110.1037/a0017387
RuggieroGThe effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spacesPsychol. Res.201781123212402778556710.1007/s00426-016-0806-x
NosekBASmythFLA multitrait-multimethod validation of the implicit association testExp. Psychol.20075414291734101110.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14
SpitzerRLKroenkeKWilliamsJBWLöweBA brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7Arch. Intern. Med.200616610921671717110.1001/archinte.166.10.1092
MariniMAnsaniAPaglieriFCaruanaFViolaMThe impact of facemasks on emotion recognition, trust attribution and re-identificationSci. Rep.20211155772021NatSR..11.5577M1:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXmsVClsbg%3D33692417797093710.1038/s41598-021-84806-5
VealeJFEdinburgh handedness inventory—Short form: A revised version based on confirmatory factor analysisLateral. Asymmet. Body Brain Cogn.20141916417710.1080/1357650X.2013.783045
WelschRWesselsMBernhardCThönesSvon CastellCPhysical distancing and the perception of interpersonal distance in the COVID-19 crisisSci. Rep.202111114852021NatSR..1111485W1:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXht12jsL3K34075094816967410.1038/s41598-021-90714-5
MooneyKMCohnESSwiftMBPhysical Distance and AIDS: Too close for comfort?J. Appl. Soc. Psychol.1992221442145210.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00959.x
MathewsKECanonLKAlexanderKRThe influence of level of empathy and ambient noise on body buffer zoneProc. Div. Personal. Soc. Psychol.19741367369
ToppenbergHLBosAERRuiterRACWigboldusDHJPryorJBHIV-related stigma in social interactions: Approach and avoidance behaviour in a virtual environment: HIV-related stigma in social interactionsEur. J. Soc. Psychol.20154516917910.1002/ejsp.2082
Valdés-ConroyBRománFJHinojosaJAShorkeySPSo far so good: Emotion in the peripersonal/extrapersonal spacePLoS ONE201272012PLoSO...749162V23185304350403410.1371/journal.pone.00491621:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XhvVagsrvK
Gimp Team. GIMP - GNU Image Manipulation Pogramm.
GokmenYTurenUErdemHTokmakİNational preferred interpersonal distance curbs the spread of COVID-19: A cross-country analysisDisast. Med. Public Health Prep.202115e20e2610.1017/dmp.2020.2951:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXhslenur7I
SeresGBalleyerAHCeruttiNFriedrichsenJSüerMFace mask use and physical distancing before and after mandatory masking: Evidence from public waiting linesSSRN Electron. J.202010.2139/ssrn.3641367
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models using lme4. (2014). https://doi.org/10.48550/ARXIV.1406.5823.
GollwitzerAPartisan differences in physical distancing are linked to health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemicNat. Hum. Behav.20204118611973313989710.1038/s41562-020-00977-7
StoneKDKandulaMKeizerADijkermanHCPeripersonal space boundaries around the lower limbsExp. Brain Res.20182361611731:STN:280:DC%2BC1M7nt1yiuw%3D%3D2909831510.1007/s00221-017-5115-0
SerinoSSharpening of peripersonal space during the COVID-19 pandemicCurr. Biol.202131R889R8901:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhs1eqt7zM34314711818830310.1016/j.cub.2021.06.001
Anwyl-IrvineALMassonniéJFlittonAKirkhamNEvershedJKGorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builderBehav. Res. Methods2020523884073101668410.3758/s13428-019-01237-x
Hall, E. T. The hidden dimension. (Anchor Books, 1990).
Ellena, G. et al. Acute stress affects peripersonal space representation. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.04.25.441303 (2021).
Anwyl-Irvine, A. L., Dalmaijer, E. S., Hodges, N. & Evershed, J. Online Timing Accuracy and Precision: A comparison of platforms, browsers, and participant’s deviceshttps://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jfeca (2020).
RizzolattiGScandolaraCMatelliMGentilucciMAfferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responsesBehav. Brain Res.198121471631:STN:280:DyaL3M3jt1Cntg%3D%3D724805510.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X
GigliottiMFSoares CoelhoPCoutinhoJCoelloYPeripersonal space in social context is modulated by action reward, but differently in males and femalesPsychol. Res.2021851811943149304910.1007/s00426-019-01242-x
Adobe Inc. Adobe Stock.
Pixabay GmbH Pixabay.
CoelloYBourgeoisJIachiniTEmbodied perception of reachable space: how do we manage threatening objects?Cogn. Process.20121313113510.1007/s10339-012-0470-z
SamboCFForsterBWilliamsSCIannettiGDTo blink or not to blink: Fine cognitive tuning of the defensive peripersonal spaceJ. Neurosci.20123212921129271:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XhsVSlsbfF22973016670381310.1523/JNEUROSCI.0607-12.2012
SemmelmannKWeigeltSOnline psychophysics: Reaction time effects in cognitive experimentsBehav. Res. Methods201749124112602749617110.3758/s13428-016-0783-4
QuesqueFKeeping you at arm’s length: Modifying peripersonal space influences interpersonal distancePsychol. Res.2017817097201:STN:280:DC%2BC2s7lsVSksQ%3D%3D2736461210.1007/s00426-016-0782-1
MassonCTestosterone administration in women increases the size of their peripersonal spaceExp. Brain Res.2021239163916491:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhtlKnt73E3377021910.1007/s00221-021-06080-1
CartaudAQuesqueFCoelloYWearing a face mask against Covid-19 results in a reduction of social distancingPLoS ONE2020151:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXis1emt7nE33284812772116910.1371/journal.pone.0243023
CalbiMThe consequences of COVID-19 on social interactions: an online study on face coveringSci. Rep.20211126011:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXivVWjt7o%3D33510195784400210.1038/s41598-021-81780-w
BrysbaertMHow many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tablesJ. Cogn.201921631517234664
16730_CR55
A Maravita (16730_CR14) 2004; 8
16730_CR54
NP Holmes (16730_CR13) 2004; 372
16730_CR53
BA Nosek (16730_CR45) 2005; 134
KM Mooney (16730_CR33) 1992; 22
E Vagnoni (16730_CR31) 2018; 13
A Cartaud (16730_CR2) 2018; 9
F Quesque (16730_CR4) 2017; 81
HL Toppenberg (16730_CR35) 2015; 45
M Argyle (16730_CR30) 1965; 28
F Ferri (16730_CR21) 2015; 70
AL Anwyl-Irvine (16730_CR52) 2020; 52
G Seres (16730_CR37) 2020
A Mathews (16730_CR60) 1986; 95
16730_CR1
H Jeon (16730_CR66) 2018; 16
B Valdés-Conroy (16730_CR22) 2012; 7
MP Lisi (16730_CR40) 2021; 16
16730_CR47
M Brysbaert (16730_CR51) 2019; 2
R Neumann (16730_CR34) 2004; 40
KE Mathews (16730_CR32) 1974; 1
K Semmelmann (16730_CR50) 2017; 49
CM Judd (16730_CR62) 2012; 103
W Hofmann (16730_CR43) 2005; 31
A Cartaud (16730_CR23) 2020; 15
MF Gigliotti (16730_CR10) 2021; 85
16730_CR36
JM Leppänen (16730_CR46) 2004; 69
LOH Kroczek (16730_CR41) 2022; 12
M Minoura (16730_CR12) 2021; 8
A Gollwitzer (16730_CR42) 2020; 4
A Dijksterhuis (16730_CR48) 2003; 14
A Serino (16730_CR15) 2007; 18
T Iachini (16730_CR24) 2021; 239
JF Veale (16730_CR58) 2014; 19
Y Coello (16730_CR5) 2021; 15
G Rizzolatti (16730_CR67) 2005; 210
M Marini (16730_CR64) 2021; 11
R Welsch (16730_CR38) 2021; 11
M Calbi (16730_CR39) 2021; 11
AE Pinkham (16730_CR49) 2010; 10
16730_CR26
C Teneggi (16730_CR18) 2013; 23
16730_CR69
16730_CR68
G Ruggiero (16730_CR11) 2019; 63
R Welsch (16730_CR28) 2020; 62
G Ruggiero (16730_CR3) 2017; 81
A Sorokowska (16730_CR63) 2017; 48
16730_CR61
S Serino (16730_CR25) 2021; 31
G Rizzolatti (16730_CR7) 1981; 2
BA Nosek (16730_CR44) 2007; 54
CF Sambo (16730_CR19) 2012; 32
F Grundmann (16730_CR65) 2021; 16
F Cardini (16730_CR9) 2019; 9
KD Stone (16730_CR8) 2018; 236
N Sakuma (16730_CR27) 2021; 128
Y Coello (16730_CR20) 2012; 13
J Bourgeois (16730_CR6) 2014; 148
Y Gokmen (16730_CR17) 2021; 15
C Masson (16730_CR16) 2021; 239
T Iachini (16730_CR29) 2016; 45
RL Spitzer (16730_CR59) 2006; 166
16730_CR57
16730_CR56
References_xml – volume: 103
  start-page: 54
  year: 2012
  ident: 16730_CR62
  publication-title: J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1037/a0028347
  contributor:
    fullname: CM Judd
– volume: 70
  start-page: 468
  year: 2015
  ident: 16730_CR21
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.001
  contributor:
    fullname: F Ferri
– volume: 40
  start-page: 543
  year: 2004
  ident: 16730_CR34
  publication-title: J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.006
  contributor:
    fullname: R Neumann
– volume: 14
  start-page: 14
  year: 2003
  ident: 16730_CR48
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01412
  contributor:
    fullname: A Dijksterhuis
– ident: 16730_CR1
– volume: 31
  start-page: R889
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR25
  publication-title: Curr. Biol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.06.001
  contributor:
    fullname: S Serino
– ident: 16730_CR61
  doi: 10.48550/ARXIV.1406.5823
– ident: 16730_CR57
– volume: 210
  start-page: 419
  year: 2005
  ident: 16730_CR67
  publication-title: Anat. Embryol. (Berl.)
  doi: 10.1007/s00429-005-0039-z
  contributor:
    fullname: G Rizzolatti
– volume: 32
  start-page: 12921
  year: 2012
  ident: 16730_CR19
  publication-title: J. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0607-12.2012
  contributor:
    fullname: CF Sambo
– volume: 11
  start-page: 5577
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR64
  publication-title: Sci. Rep.
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84806-5
  contributor:
    fullname: M Marini
– volume: 239
  start-page: 1639
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR16
  publication-title: Exp. Brain Res.
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06080-1
  contributor:
    fullname: C Masson
– volume: 8
  start-page: 79
  year: 2004
  ident: 16730_CR14
  publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci.
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008
  contributor:
    fullname: A Maravita
– volume: 15
  year: 2020
  ident: 16730_CR23
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243023
  contributor:
    fullname: A Cartaud
– ident: 16730_CR53
– volume: 52
  start-page: 388
  year: 2020
  ident: 16730_CR52
  publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x
  contributor:
    fullname: AL Anwyl-Irvine
– volume: 28
  start-page: 289
  year: 1965
  ident: 16730_CR30
  publication-title: Sociometry
  doi: 10.2307/2786027
  contributor:
    fullname: M Argyle
– volume: 19
  start-page: 164
  year: 2014
  ident: 16730_CR58
  publication-title: Lateral. Asymmet. Body Brain Cogn.
  doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045
  contributor:
    fullname: JF Veale
– volume: 16
  start-page: 18
  year: 2018
  ident: 16730_CR66
  publication-title: Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.9758/cpn.2018.16.1.18
  contributor:
    fullname: H Jeon
– volume: 2
  start-page: 147
  year: 1981
  ident: 16730_CR7
  publication-title: Behav. Brain Res.
  doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X
  contributor:
    fullname: G Rizzolatti
– volume: 63
  start-page: 52
  year: 2019
  ident: 16730_CR11
  publication-title: J. Environ. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.004
  contributor:
    fullname: G Ruggiero
– volume: 49
  start-page: 1241
  year: 2017
  ident: 16730_CR50
  publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0783-4
  contributor:
    fullname: K Semmelmann
– volume: 10
  start-page: 141
  year: 2010
  ident: 16730_CR49
  publication-title: Emotion
  doi: 10.1037/a0017387
  contributor:
    fullname: AE Pinkham
– volume: 7
  year: 2012
  ident: 16730_CR22
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049162
  contributor:
    fullname: B Valdés-Conroy
– volume: 69
  start-page: 22
  year: 2004
  ident: 16730_CR46
  publication-title: Psychol. Res. Psychol. Forsch.
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0157-2
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Leppänen
– volume: 12
  start-page: 2213
  year: 2022
  ident: 16730_CR41
  publication-title: Sci. Rep.
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-06086-x
  contributor:
    fullname: LOH Kroczek
– ident: 16730_CR56
– volume: 45
  start-page: 169
  year: 2015
  ident: 16730_CR35
  publication-title: Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2082
  contributor:
    fullname: HL Toppenberg
– year: 2020
  ident: 16730_CR37
  publication-title: SSRN Electron. J.
  doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3641367
  contributor:
    fullname: G Seres
– volume: 54
  start-page: 14
  year: 2007
  ident: 16730_CR44
  publication-title: Exp. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14
  contributor:
    fullname: BA Nosek
– volume: 95
  start-page: 131
  year: 1986
  ident: 16730_CR60
  publication-title: J. Abnorm. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.2.131
  contributor:
    fullname: A Mathews
– volume: 48
  start-page: 577
  year: 2017
  ident: 16730_CR63
  publication-title: J. Cross-Cult. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1177/0022022117698039
  contributor:
    fullname: A Sorokowska
– volume: 166
  start-page: 1092
  year: 2006
  ident: 16730_CR59
  publication-title: Arch. Intern. Med.
  doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092
  contributor:
    fullname: RL Spitzer
– volume: 239
  start-page: 1035
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR24
  publication-title: Exp. Brain Res.
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06040-9
  contributor:
    fullname: T Iachini
– volume: 85
  start-page: 181
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR10
  publication-title: Psychol. Res.
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01242-x
  contributor:
    fullname: MF Gigliotti
– volume: 9
  start-page: 657
  year: 2018
  ident: 16730_CR2
  publication-title: Front. Psychol.
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00657
  contributor:
    fullname: A Cartaud
– volume: 236
  start-page: 161
  year: 2018
  ident: 16730_CR8
  publication-title: Exp. Brain Res.
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5115-0
  contributor:
    fullname: KD Stone
– volume: 372
  start-page: 62
  year: 2004
  ident: 16730_CR13
  publication-title: Neurosci. Lett.
  doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024
  contributor:
    fullname: NP Holmes
– volume: 45
  start-page: 154
  year: 2016
  ident: 16730_CR29
  publication-title: J. Environ. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004
  contributor:
    fullname: T Iachini
– volume: 1
  start-page: 367
  year: 1974
  ident: 16730_CR32
  publication-title: Proc. Div. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
  contributor:
    fullname: KE Mathews
– ident: 16730_CR36
– volume: 11
  start-page: 11485
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR38
  publication-title: Sci. Rep.
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-90714-5
  contributor:
    fullname: R Welsch
– volume: 11
  start-page: 2601
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR39
  publication-title: Sci. Rep.
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81780-w
  contributor:
    fullname: M Calbi
– volume: 16
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR40
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255598
  contributor:
    fullname: MP Lisi
– volume: 13
  year: 2018
  ident: 16730_CR31
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192753
  contributor:
    fullname: E Vagnoni
– volume: 13
  start-page: 131
  year: 2012
  ident: 16730_CR20
  publication-title: Cogn. Process.
  doi: 10.1007/s10339-012-0470-z
  contributor:
    fullname: Y Coello
– volume: 148
  start-page: 91
  year: 2014
  ident: 16730_CR6
  publication-title: Acta Psychol. (Amst.)
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008
  contributor:
    fullname: J Bourgeois
– volume: 8
  start-page: 248
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR12
  publication-title: Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract.
  contributor:
    fullname: M Minoura
– volume: 2
  start-page: 16
  year: 2019
  ident: 16730_CR51
  publication-title: J. Cogn.
  doi: 10.5334/joc.72
  contributor:
    fullname: M Brysbaert
– ident: 16730_CR55
– volume: 81
  start-page: 709
  year: 2017
  ident: 16730_CR4
  publication-title: Psychol. Res.
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0782-1
  contributor:
    fullname: F Quesque
– volume: 15
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR5
  publication-title: Front. Hum. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.636124
  contributor:
    fullname: Y Coello
– volume: 18
  start-page: 642
  year: 2007
  ident: 16730_CR15
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x
  contributor:
    fullname: A Serino
– ident: 16730_CR26
  doi: 10.1101/2021.04.25.441303
– volume: 23
  start-page: 406
  year: 2013
  ident: 16730_CR18
  publication-title: Curr. Biol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043
  contributor:
    fullname: C Teneggi
– volume: 15
  start-page: e20
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR17
  publication-title: Disast. Med. Public Health Prep.
  doi: 10.1017/dmp.2020.295
  contributor:
    fullname: Y Gokmen
– volume: 81
  start-page: 1232
  year: 2017
  ident: 16730_CR3
  publication-title: Psychol. Res.
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0806-x
  contributor:
    fullname: G Ruggiero
– volume: 128
  start-page: 178
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR27
  publication-title: Percept. Mot. Skills
  doi: 10.1177/0031512520973515
  contributor:
    fullname: N Sakuma
– volume: 16
  year: 2021
  ident: 16730_CR65
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249792
  contributor:
    fullname: F Grundmann
– volume: 9
  start-page: 8606
  year: 2019
  ident: 16730_CR9
  publication-title: Sci. Rep.
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-45224-w
  contributor:
    fullname: F Cardini
– ident: 16730_CR54
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1442
  year: 1992
  ident: 16730_CR33
  publication-title: J. Appl. Soc. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00959.x
  contributor:
    fullname: KM Mooney
– volume: 4
  start-page: 1186
  year: 2020
  ident: 16730_CR42
  publication-title: Nat. Hum. Behav.
  doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00977-7
  contributor:
    fullname: A Gollwitzer
– volume: 134
  start-page: 565
  year: 2005
  ident: 16730_CR45
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565
  contributor:
    fullname: BA Nosek
– ident: 16730_CR47
– ident: 16730_CR69
  doi: 10.31234/osf.io/jfeca
– ident: 16730_CR68
– volume: 62
  start-page: 1095
  year: 2020
  ident: 16730_CR28
  publication-title: Hum. Factors J. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc.
  doi: 10.1177/0018720820956858
  contributor:
    fullname: R Welsch
– volume: 31
  start-page: 1369
  year: 2005
  ident: 16730_CR43
  publication-title: Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.
  doi: 10.1177/0146167205275613
  contributor:
    fullname: W Hofmann
SSID ssj0000529419
Score 2.4654193
Snippet Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space...
Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of...
Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of...
SourceID doaj
pubmedcentral
proquest
crossref
pubmed
springer
SourceType Open Website
Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Publisher
StartPage 12777
SubjectTerms 631/477
631/477/2811
Coronaviruses
COVID-19
COVID-19 - epidemiology
COVID-19 - prevention & control
Face
Humanities and Social Sciences
Humans
Masks
multidisciplinary
Pandemics
Pandemics - prevention & control
Personal Space
Physical Distancing
Protective equipment
Science
Science (multidisciplinary)
Sensory integration
Social behavior
Social distancing
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  dbid: DOA
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1LbxUhFCamiYkbY32O1gYTd5aUx8DAsrY21YVurOmOAAOx0c696UwX_fc9wNzbXh9x427CkMnJOR_wMZzzgdBbSoNIWiUC5JiS1rWSaNUrYkLsjXMhhVASZD-rk9P205k8u3PVV84Jq_LA1XH7WbBcOuY7ERlMpL2DJZRyw6HFwUzry-xLzZ3NVFX15qZlZq6SoULvj7BS5Woy2HsxBbAmbGMlKoL9f2KZvydL_nJiWhai40fo4cwg8UG1fBvdi8NjdL_eKXn9BI1HpfAQA7HDh1--fTwizOBl_lV8cR7gYU6jHqYRL8sdI3iMJfd5xPmfLHY4uRDxhRt_7OFhgbOexGWtfsDwFewm6BJ-LgC6uM_kE1DzFJ0ef_h6eELmmxVIAIY2EeZNa4KJqjehpX2UnspexQDOBYKQwOVe8yRTFEKmXiYjA3RgrVMGtmiOiWdoa1gM8QXCQSehW51CFpoTofPeawesyAF30MHHBr1bedkuq4CGLQffQtsaEwsxsSUmljXofQ7EumcWvy4NAAk7Q8L-CxIN2lmF0c4jcrRc5YPqLP_YoDfr1zCW8gGJG-LiqvRRuTqX8wY9r1FfWyKkNqqjYGG3gYcNUzffDOffi153pshM6AbtrZBza9bfXfHyf7jiFXrAM-RpR3i3g7amy6v4GljU5HfLgLkBBMMV8g
  priority: 102
  providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals
– databaseName: ProQuest Central
  dbid: BENPR
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELZgKyQuiDeBgozEjVqN7dixT4i-VDgUhCjqzbIdu1TQZNlsD_x7xo53q-V1ixwrmnjGns_zROhVXXselYwEwHFNGtsIomQnifah09b66H0OkD2Rx6fN-zNxVgxuYwmrXJ2J-aDuBp9s5LtMJkdjKt_3Zv6DpK5RybtaWmjcRFsMbgpshrb2Dk8-flpbWZIfq6G6ZMvUXO2OoLFSVhncwagE8SZ0QyPlwv1_Q5t_Bk3-5jnNCunoLrpTkCR-O7H-HroR-vvo1tRb8ucDNB7kBEQMAA_vf_jy7oBQjefJZHx54eGhhFP3yxHPc68RPIYcAz3iZJvFFkfrA76047cd3A841ZVYTFkQGL6C7RKm-O8DiDDuEggF6XmITo8OP-8fk9JhgXhAaktCnW6010F22jd1F4SrRSeDtxIWrY4AFp1iUcTAuYidiFp4mEAbKzVc1Szlj9CsH_rwBGGvIleNij4VnOO-dc4pC-jIAoZQ3oUKvV6tsplPhTRMdoBzZSaeGOCJyTwxtEJ7iRHrmakIdh4YFuem7CmTatkLS13LAwUd2wHVvGaawYgFJewqtL1ioyk7czTXclShl-vXsKeSo8T2YbjKc2TK0mWsQo8nrq8p4UJp2dZAYbshDxukbr7pL77mut0JKlOuKrSzkpxrsv69FE___xfP0G2WhLluCWu30Wy5uArPASct3YuyGX4B65EQbA
  priority: 102
  providerName: ProQuest
– databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access
  dbid: M48
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3Pj9UgECabNSZejL-trgYTby5aoFA4GKO7blYPevGZvRGgsG7cbZ-v3cT97x1o-8zTp7cGaDNlhswHzHyD0POy9DwqGQmA45JUthJEyUYS7UOjrfXR-xwg-0keL6qPJ-JkB83ljqYJ7Ldu7VI9qcXq_OXPH1dvYMG_HlPG1asenFBKFINtFZVgsQR2Q9dYIuZKoXwT3B-5vpmuqJ5yZ7a_uuGfMo3_Nuz5dwjlH_eo2T0d3UI3J1yJ346GcBvthPYOuj5Wmry6i_rDnI6IAe7hg89fPxwSqvEyHSBfnHl4mIKr26HHy1x5BPchR0T3OJ3UYouj9QFf2P77Pm47nFgmVmNOBIavYDvAEH_egUHjJkFSsKV7aHH0_svBMZnqLRAPuG0g1OlKex1ko31VNkG4UjQyeCth0soI0NEpFkUMnIvYiKiFhwG0slLDxs1Sfh_ttl0bHiLsVeSqUtEn-jnua-ecsoCVLCAK5V0o0It5ls1ypNUw-TqcKzPqxIBOTNaJoQV6lxSxHpkosXNDtzo10wozidleWOpqHih43Aak5iXTDFosuGRXoL1ZjWY2M8Nkur5OpJAFerbuhhWWrk1sG7rLPEamnF3GCvRg1PpaEi6UlnUJEtYb9rAh6mZPe_Yts3gn4Ey5KtD-bDm_xfr3VDz6_188RjdYMuayJqzeQ7vD6jI8AdQ0uKd5KfwC610SNw
  priority: 102
  providerName: Scholars Portal
– databaseName: Springer Nature OA Free Journals
  dbid: C6C
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3Pb9UwDI7QEBIXxG_KBgoSNxaRNE2aHOGNaXCAC0O7RUmaiAnW97R2B_772WnfQ4Vx4FalbmXFjvwltr8Q8przKLPRmQE45qzxjWJGd5rZmDrrfcwxlgLZz_rktPl0ps5mmhzshVnk76V5O0CAwSYw2DIJDd7IYKdzG2MwXtOw0qvdeQpmrBph576Ymz9dxJ5C0X8Trvy7PPKPHGkJPcf3yb0ZM9J3k5EfkFupf0juTLdI_npEhqPSakgBytHVl28fj5iwdIOHwxfnER7mwul-HOim3CpCh1SqnQeKp7DU0-xjohd--HFI-zVFBonLqd-Bwl-oH0Ek_lyDs9IO4Sb4yWNyevzh6-qEzXcpsAiYbGQi2MZGm3RnY8O7pAJXnU7Ra5g0ngEWBlNnlZOUKncqWxVBQDReW9iUeSGfkL1-3adnhEaTpWlMjkgtJ2MbQjAecJAHtGBiSBV5s51lt5koM1xJdUvjJps4sIkrNnGiIu_REDtJpLsuA-AFbl49DlnrlRehlUlANO1Aa8lrW8OIh3AbKnKwNaOb1-Dgao2paSR8rMir3WtYPZgS8X1aXxUZjf24dV2Rp5PVd5pIZaxuOWjYLvxhoeryTX_-vTB0IygW0lTkcOs5v9X691Q8_z_xfXK3RufmLavbA7I3Xl6lF4CQxvCyLI1rKPEH5Q
  priority: 102
  providerName: Springer Nature
Title During the COVID-19 pandemic participants prefer settings with a face mask, no interaction and at a closer distance
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.1038/s41598-022-16730-1
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35896701
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2695360573
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2696005722
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9326138
https://doaj.org/article/61505a1b73e1471da64302921b7a067b
Volume 12
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Nb9MwFH_ahpC4IL4JjMpI3FjWJI4d-8i6TQNpY0IM9RbZjj0q1qRqsgP_Pc9OUigfFy5Vmry0T34_6_1svw-AN0liqBPcxUiOkzhXOYsFr3gsja2kUsYZEwJkL_jZVf5hzuY7wMZcmBC0b_TisL5ZHtaLryG2crU00zFObHp5PvOcI6Viugu7CNBfluh9Qe9M5qkcEmQSFGvRSflEMlx2pRxfiH17GMqE5MXQC2b0R6Fs_9-45p8hk7-dmwZ3dPoA7g88krzr9X0IO7Z-BHf7zpLfH0N7HNIPCdI7Mvv45f1xnEqy8hvGy4XBiyGYuu5asgqdRkhrQwR0S_zOLFHEKWPJUrXfDkjdEF9VYt3nQBD8FaI6FDE3DQKYVJ6CInaewNXpyefZWTz0V4gN8rQuTrXMpZGWV9LkSWWZTljFrVEcxy9xSBW1yBxzllLmKuYkMyiQ5opLXKiplD6Fvbqp7XMgRjgqcuGMLzdHTaG1Fgq5kUIGIYy2EbwdR7lc9WU0ynD8TUXZm6dE85TBPGUawZE3xEbSl8AON5r1dTkAofSV7JlKdUFtih62Qq1pkskM7yh0wTqC_dGM5TAv2zLj_rjaF4GM4PXmMc4of0yiatvcBhnuc3SzLIJnvdU3moyoiaDYwsOWqttPEMShavcA2ggORuT8VOvfQ_Hiv__oJdzLPOSTIs6Kfdjr1rf2FRKoTk9w2syLCdw5Orm4_ITfZnw2CZsR-Hmei0mYUD8A8gwdLw
link.rule.ids 230,315,730,783,787,867,888,2109,2228,12070,21402,24332,27938,27939,31733,31734,33758,33759,41134,42203,43324,43819,51590,53806,53808,74081,74638
linkProvider National Library of Medicine
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELagCMEF8SZQwEjcqFU7Thz7hKCl2kIplxbtzXIcGyposmzSA_-eGSe71fK6rRIrmvWMM1_m8Q0hLzn3MmoVGYBjzgpXlEyrRjHjQ2Oc89H7VCB7rGanxft5OZ8Cbv1UVrl6J6YXddN5jJHv5goTjUjf93rxg-HUKMyuTiM0rpJryMOF3PnVvFrHWDCLVQgz9cpwqXd78FfYUwZfYEKBcTOx4Y8Sbf_fsOafJZO_5U2TOzq4TW5NOJK-GRV_h1wJ7V1yfZws-fMe6fdT-yEFeEf3Pn0-3GfC0AUGjM_PPPyYiqnboaeLNGmE9iFVQPcUI7PU0eh8oOeu_7ZD244iq8Ry7IGg8BTqBljiv3dgwLRBCAq2c5-cHrw72Zuxab4C84DTBiZqUxhvgmqML3gTypqXjQreKdg0HgEq1jqPZQxSlrEpoyk9LBCFUwY-1JyQD8hW27XhEaFeR6kLHT3SzUlf1XWtHWAjBwhC-zpk5NVql-1ipNGwKf0ttR11YkEnNunEioy8RUWsVyIFdrrQLb_Y6URZZLIvnagrGQR42Aakljw3OVxx4ILrjGyv1Ginc9nbSyvKyIv1bThRmCZxbegu0hqFPbp5npGHo9bXkshSG1VxkLDasIcNUTfvtGdfE2s3AmUhdUZ2VpZzKda_t-Lx___Fc3JjdvLxyB4dHn94Qm7maNi8Ynm1TbaG5UV4CohpqJ-lY_ELSm8R9w
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELagCNQL4t1AASNxo9bGcezYJwRdVi2gwoGivVm2Y0MFTZbN9sC_Z-x4t1pet1ViRbOebzJfPC-EnpelY0GKQIAcl6Q2NSdStIIo51tljAvOpQTZE3F0Wr-d83nOfxpyWuX6nZhe1G3v4hn5pBIx0Bjb901CTov4OJ29XPwgcYJUjLTmcRpX0bWmBkcH2G7mzea8JUa0aqpy3UzJ5GQA3xXry-BrjAoAOqFbvim18P8b7_wzffK3GGpyTbNb6GbmlPjVCILb6Irv7qDr45TJn3fRME2liBioHj788Pl4SqjCi3h4fH7m4EdOrO5WA16kqSN48CkbesDxlBYbHIzz-NwM3w5w1-PYYWI51kNgeAo2K1jivvcAZtxGOgo4uodOZ28-HR6RPGuBOOBsK0KtqpVTXrTK1WXruS15K7wzAjatDEAbrawCD54xHloeFHewgNZGKPhoM5TdRztd3_k9hJ0MTNYyuNh6jrnGWisN8CQDbEI66wv0Yr3LejG21NApFM6kHnWiQSc66UTTAr2OitisjO2w04V--UVn69Kxqz031DbMU_C2LUjNykpVcMWAO7YF2l-rUWcbHfQlogr0bHMbrCuGTEzn-4u0RsR63aoq0INR6xtJGJdKNCVI2GzhYUvU7Tvd2dfUwTuSZspkgQ7WyLkU699b8fD__-IpugEWod8fn7x7hHariOuyIVWzj3ZWywv_GMjTyj5JVvELT4cWLA
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=During+the+COVID-19+pandemic+participants+prefer+settings+with+a+face+mask%2C+no+interaction+and+at+a+closer+distance&rft.jtitle=Scientific+reports&rft.au=K%C3%BChne%2C+K&rft.au=Fischer%2C+M.+H&rft.au=Jeglinski-Mende%2C+M.+A&rft.date=2022-07-27&rft.pub=Nature+Publishing+Group&rft.eissn=2045-2322&rft.volume=12&rft.issue=1&rft_id=info:doi/10.1038%2Fs41598-022-16730-1&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2045-2322&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2045-2322&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2045-2322&client=summon