During the COVID-19 pandemic participants prefer settings with a face mask, no interaction and at a closer distance
Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulate...
Saved in:
Published in | Scientific reports Vol. 12; no. 1; pp. 12777 - 12 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
London
Nature Publishing Group UK
27.07.2022
Nature Publishing Group Nature Portfolio |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed. Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed. Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed. Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers’ task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed. Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers' task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers' task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed. |
ArticleNumber | 12777 |
Author | Kühne, K. Jeglinski-Mende, M. A. Fischer, M. H. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: K. surname: Kühne fullname: Kühne, K. email: kkuehne@uni-potsdam.de organization: Cognitive Sciences Division, University of Potsdam – sequence: 2 givenname: M. H. surname: Fischer fullname: Fischer, M. H. organization: Cognitive Sciences Division, University of Potsdam – sequence: 3 givenname: M. A. surname: Jeglinski-Mende fullname: Jeglinski-Mende, M. A. organization: Cognitive Sciences Division, University of Potsdam |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35896701$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kktv1DAURiNUREvpH2CBLLFhQcCP2Ik3SNWUx0iVugG2luPczHjI2IPtAfHvuTMppWVBNrGdc49z7e9pdRJigKp6zugbRkX3NjdM6q6mnNdMtYLW7FF1xmkjay44P7k3Pq0uct5QfCTXDdNPqlMhO61ays6qfLVPPqxIWQNZ3HxdXtVMk50NA2y9w0Eq3nmcl0x2CUZIJEMpWJHJT1_WxJLROiBbm7-9JiESHwok64qPgaCF2IKIm2LGysHnYoODZ9Xj0U4ZLm7f59WXD-8_Lz7V1zcfl4vL69rJhpaa9brRToMatGvoALKnclDgrMIm6Mgk7Ts-yhGEkOMgRy0dAqyxSvOOWybOq-XsHaLdmF3yW5t-mWi9OS7EtDLH_iYwCm3Ssr4VwJqWDbiHoFxzXLFUtT263s2u3b7fwuAglGSnB9KHX4Jfm1X8YbTgiokOBa9uBSl-30MuZuuzg2myAeI-G660whtqOUf05T_oJu5TwKM6UFIoxARSfKZcijnj3dz9DKPmEBEzR8RgRMwxIuZwJC_ut3FX8icQCIgZyLtDLiD93fs_2t-fM8dQ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s10339_024_01179_z crossref_primary_10_1007_s44202_023_00066_6 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_34667_x crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_55218_y crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_024_00570_w crossref_primary_10_3390_bs14010020 crossref_primary_10_1002_ijop_12921 crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_023_00528_4 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychsport_2023_102509 crossref_primary_10_3390_healthcare11121711 crossref_primary_10_4236_pp_2023_145011 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijchp_2024_100476 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/a0028347 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.001 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.006 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01412 10.1016/j.cub.2021.06.001 10.48550/ARXIV.1406.5823 10.1007/s00429-005-0039-z 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0607-12.2012 10.1038/s41598-021-84806-5 10.1007/s00221-021-06080-1 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008 10.1371/journal.pone.0243023 10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x 10.2307/2786027 10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045 10.9758/cpn.2018.16.1.18 10.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.004 10.3758/s13428-016-0783-4 10.1037/a0017387 10.1371/journal.pone.0049162 10.1007/s00426-003-0157-2 10.1038/s41598-022-06086-x 10.1002/ejsp.2082 10.2139/ssrn.3641367 10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14 10.1037/0021-843X.95.2.131 10.1177/0022022117698039 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092 10.1007/s00221-021-06040-9 10.1007/s00426-019-01242-x 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00657 10.1007/s00221-017-5115-0 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004 10.1038/s41598-021-90714-5 10.1038/s41598-021-81780-w 10.1371/journal.pone.0255598 10.1371/journal.pone.0192753 10.1007/s10339-012-0470-z 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008 10.5334/joc.72 10.1007/s00426-016-0782-1 10.3389/fnhum.2021.636124 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x 10.1101/2021.04.25.441303 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043 10.1017/dmp.2020.295 10.1007/s00426-016-0806-x 10.1177/0031512520973515 10.1371/journal.pone.0249792 10.1038/s41598-019-45224-w 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00959.x 10.1038/s41562-020-00977-7 10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565 10.31234/osf.io/jfeca 10.1177/0018720820956858 10.1177/0146167205275613 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author(s) 2022 2022. The Author(s). The Author(s) 2022. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author(s) 2022 – notice: 2022. The Author(s). – notice: The Author(s) 2022. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
DBID | C6C CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM AAYXX CITATION 3V. 7X7 7XB 88A 88E 88I 8FE 8FH 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABUWG AFKRA AZQEC BBNVY BENPR BHPHI CCPQU DWQXO FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ HCIFZ K9. LK8 M0S M1P M2P M7P PIMPY PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI Q9U 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.1038/s41598-022-16730-1 |
DatabaseName | Springer Nature OA Free Journals Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed CrossRef ProQuest Central (Corporate) Health & Medical Collection (Proquest) ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Biology Database (Alumni Edition) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) Science Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Natural Science Collection Hospital Premium Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland ProQuest Central Essentials Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student SciTech Premium Collection (Proquest) (PQ_SDU_P3) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Biological Sciences Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition) PML(ProQuest Medical Library) ProQuest Science Journals Biological Science Database Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central Basic MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) CrossRef Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Biology Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Health Research Premium Collection Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) Natural Science Collection ProQuest Central Korea Biological Science Collection ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) ProQuest Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest Science Journals ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) Biological Science Database ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest Medical Library ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest One Academic ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | Publicly Available Content Database CrossRef MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: C6C name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals url: http://www.springeropen.com/ sourceTypes: Publisher – sequence: 2 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 3 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 4 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 5 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Biology |
EISSN | 2045-2322 |
EndPage | 12 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_61505a1b73e1471da64302921b7a067b 10_1038_s41598_022_16730_1 35896701 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Universität Potsdam (1031) – fundername: ; |
GroupedDBID | 0R~ 3V. 4.4 53G 5VS 7X7 88A 88E 88I 8FE 8FH 8FI 8FJ AAFWJ AAJSJ AAKDD ABDBF ABUWG ACGFS ACSMW ADBBV ADRAZ AENEX AFKRA AJTQC ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS AZQEC BAWUL BBNVY BCNDV BENPR BHPHI BPHCQ BVXVI C6C CCPQU DIK DWQXO EBD EBLON EBS ESX FYUFA GNUQQ GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HCIFZ HH5 HMCUK HYE KQ8 LK8 M0L M1P M2P M48 M7P M~E NAO OK1 PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO RIG RNT RNTTT RPM SNYQT UKHRP CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM AAYXX AFPKN CITATION 7XB 8FK K9. PQEST PQUKI Q9U 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-1b949c9e6d9c40de5b05d6eca64190f150b82f5fe335fd5f95c05d14a69282a13 |
IEDL.DBID | RPM |
ISSN | 2045-2322 |
IngestDate | Tue Oct 22 15:14:34 EDT 2024 Tue Sep 17 21:17:03 EDT 2024 Sat Oct 26 04:08:24 EDT 2024 Sun Oct 27 16:55:05 EDT 2024 Fri Aug 23 02:47:42 EDT 2024 Sat Nov 02 12:30:23 EDT 2024 Fri Oct 11 20:53:32 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Language | English |
License | 2022. The Author(s). Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c540t-1b949c9e6d9c40de5b05d6eca64190f150b82f5fe335fd5f95c05d14a69282a13 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9326138/ |
PMID | 35896701 |
PQID | 2695360573 |
PQPubID | 2041939 |
PageCount | 12 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_61505a1b73e1471da64302921b7a067b pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9326138 proquest_miscellaneous_2696005722 proquest_journals_2695360573 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_16730_1 pubmed_primary_35896701 springer_journals_10_1038_s41598_022_16730_1 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2022-07-27 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2022-07-27 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 07 year: 2022 text: 2022-07-27 day: 27 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | London |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: London – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Scientific reports |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Sci Rep |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Sci Rep |
PublicationYear | 2022 |
Publisher | Nature Publishing Group UK Nature Publishing Group Nature Portfolio |
Publisher_xml | – name: Nature Publishing Group UK – name: Nature Publishing Group – name: Nature Portfolio |
References | NosekBAModerators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluationJ. Exp. Psychol. Gen.200513456558416316292144067610.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565 IachiniTRuggieroGCan I put myself in your shoes? Sharing peripersonal space reveals the simulation of the action possibilities of othersExp. Brain Res.2021239103510453352323910.1007/s00221-021-06040-9 LisiMPScattolinMFusaroMAgliotiSMA Bayesian approach to reveal the key role of mask wearing in modulating projected interpersonal distance during the first COVID-19 outbreakPLoS ONE2021161:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhvVWnsLbL34375361835447110.1371/journal.pone.0255598 LeDoux, J. E. & & Armony, J. L. How danger is encoded: towards a systems, cellular, and computational understanding of cognitive-emotional interactions in fear circuits. in The cognitive neurosciences 1067–1079 (Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press, 1999). JeonHLeeS-HFrom Neurons to Social Beings: Short Review of the Mirror Neuron System Research and Its Socio-Psychological and Psychiatric ImplicationsClin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci.201816183129397663581045610.9758/cpn.2018.16.1.18 SakumaNIkedaKEffects of modulated emotion and facial masking encounter on personal distancePercept. Mot. Skills20211281781903320218010.1177/0031512520973515 Inmagine Lab Pte Ltd. 123rf. HolmesNPCalvertGASpenceCExtending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of toolsNeurosci. Lett.200437262671:CAS:528:DC%2BD2cXpsFWhsbo%3D1553108910.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024 WelschRHechtHChuangLvon CastellCInterpersonal Distance in the SARS-CoV-2 CrisisHum. Factors J. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc.2020621095110110.1177/0018720820956858 MathewsAMacLeodCDiscrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety statesJ. Abnorm. Psychol.1986951311381:STN:280:DyaL283isV2htg%3D%3D371143610.1037/0021-843X.95.2.131 FerriFTajadura-JiménezAVäljamäeAVastanoRCostantiniMEmotion-inducing approaching sounds shape the boundaries of multisensory peripersonal spaceNeuropsychologia2015704684752574486910.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.001 VagnoniELewisJTajadura-JiménezACardiniFListening to a conversation with aggressive content expands the interpersonal spacePLoS ONE20181329590110587393610.1371/journal.pone.01927531:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXit1aju73E BourgeoisJFarnèACoelloYCosts and benefits of tool-use on the perception of reachable spaceActa Psychol. (Amst.)2014148919510.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008 JuddCMWestfallJKennyDATreating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problemJ. Pers. Soc. Psychol.201210354692261266710.1037/a0028347 SerinoABassolinoMFarnèALàdavasEExtended multisensory space in blind cane usersPsychol. Sci.2007186426481761487410.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x NeumannRHülsenbeckKSeibtBAttitudes towards people with AIDS and avoidance behavior: Automatic and reflective bases of behaviorJ. Exp. Soc. Psychol.20044054355010.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.006 IachiniTPeripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: Effects of gender and ageJ. Environ. Psychol.20164515416410.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004 KroczekLOHBöhmeSMühlbergerAFace masks reduce interpersonal distance in virtual realitySci. Rep.20221222132022NatSR..12.2213K1:CAS:528:DC%2BB38Xjt1Cltr0%3D35140279882885010.1038/s41598-022-06086-x RuggieroGRapuanoMIachiniTPerceived temperature modulates peripersonal and interpersonal spaces differently in men and womenJ. Environ. Psychol.201963525910.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.004 GrundmannFEpstudeKScheibeSFace masks reduce emotion-recognition accuracy and perceived closenessPLoS ONE2021161:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhtVWms7jL33891614806459010.1371/journal.pone.0249792 RizzolattiGThe mirror neuron system and its function in humansAnat. Embryol. (Berl.)200521041942110.1007/s00429-005-0039-z CartaudARuggieroGOttLIachiniTCoelloYPhysiological response to facial expressions in peripersonal space determines interpersonal distance in a social interaction contextFront. Psychol.2018965729867639594986510.3389/fpsyg.2018.00657 TeneggiCCanzoneriEdi PellegrinoGSerinoASocial modulation of peripersonal space boundariesCurr. Biol.2013234064111:CAS:528:DC%2BC3sXit12jtbw%3D2339483110.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043 HofmannWGawronskiBGschwendnerTLeHSchmittMA meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measuresPers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.200531136913851614366910.1177/0146167205275613 PinkhamAEGriffinMBaronRSassonNJGurRCThe face in the crowd effect: Anger superiority when using real faces and multiple identitiesEmotion2010101411462014131110.1037/a0017387 RuggieroGThe effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spacesPsychol. Res.201781123212402778556710.1007/s00426-016-0806-x NosekBASmythFLA multitrait-multimethod validation of the implicit association testExp. Psychol.20075414291734101110.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14 SpitzerRLKroenkeKWilliamsJBWLöweBA brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7Arch. Intern. Med.200616610921671717110.1001/archinte.166.10.1092 MariniMAnsaniAPaglieriFCaruanaFViolaMThe impact of facemasks on emotion recognition, trust attribution and re-identificationSci. Rep.20211155772021NatSR..11.5577M1:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXmsVClsbg%3D33692417797093710.1038/s41598-021-84806-5 VealeJFEdinburgh handedness inventory—Short form: A revised version based on confirmatory factor analysisLateral. Asymmet. Body Brain Cogn.20141916417710.1080/1357650X.2013.783045 WelschRWesselsMBernhardCThönesSvon CastellCPhysical distancing and the perception of interpersonal distance in the COVID-19 crisisSci. Rep.202111114852021NatSR..1111485W1:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXht12jsL3K34075094816967410.1038/s41598-021-90714-5 MooneyKMCohnESSwiftMBPhysical Distance and AIDS: Too close for comfort?J. Appl. Soc. Psychol.1992221442145210.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00959.x MathewsKECanonLKAlexanderKRThe influence of level of empathy and ambient noise on body buffer zoneProc. Div. Personal. Soc. Psychol.19741367369 ToppenbergHLBosAERRuiterRACWigboldusDHJPryorJBHIV-related stigma in social interactions: Approach and avoidance behaviour in a virtual environment: HIV-related stigma in social interactionsEur. J. Soc. Psychol.20154516917910.1002/ejsp.2082 Valdés-ConroyBRománFJHinojosaJAShorkeySPSo far so good: Emotion in the peripersonal/extrapersonal spacePLoS ONE201272012PLoSO...749162V23185304350403410.1371/journal.pone.00491621:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XhvVagsrvK Gimp Team. GIMP - GNU Image Manipulation Pogramm. GokmenYTurenUErdemHTokmakİNational preferred interpersonal distance curbs the spread of COVID-19: A cross-country analysisDisast. Med. Public Health Prep.202115e20e2610.1017/dmp.2020.2951:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXhslenur7I SeresGBalleyerAHCeruttiNFriedrichsenJSüerMFace mask use and physical distancing before and after mandatory masking: Evidence from public waiting linesSSRN Electron. J.202010.2139/ssrn.3641367 Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models using lme4. (2014). https://doi.org/10.48550/ARXIV.1406.5823. GollwitzerAPartisan differences in physical distancing are linked to health outcomes during the COVID-19 pandemicNat. Hum. Behav.20204118611973313989710.1038/s41562-020-00977-7 StoneKDKandulaMKeizerADijkermanHCPeripersonal space boundaries around the lower limbsExp. Brain Res.20182361611731:STN:280:DC%2BC1M7nt1yiuw%3D%3D2909831510.1007/s00221-017-5115-0 SerinoSSharpening of peripersonal space during the COVID-19 pandemicCurr. Biol.202131R889R8901:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhs1eqt7zM34314711818830310.1016/j.cub.2021.06.001 Anwyl-IrvineALMassonniéJFlittonAKirkhamNEvershedJKGorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builderBehav. Res. Methods2020523884073101668410.3758/s13428-019-01237-x Hall, E. T. The hidden dimension. (Anchor Books, 1990). Ellena, G. et al. Acute stress affects peripersonal space representation. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.04.25.441303 (2021). Anwyl-Irvine, A. L., Dalmaijer, E. S., Hodges, N. & Evershed, J. Online Timing Accuracy and Precision: A comparison of platforms, browsers, and participant’s deviceshttps://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jfeca (2020). RizzolattiGScandolaraCMatelliMGentilucciMAfferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responsesBehav. Brain Res.198121471631:STN:280:DyaL3M3jt1Cntg%3D%3D724805510.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X GigliottiMFSoares CoelhoPCoutinhoJCoelloYPeripersonal space in social context is modulated by action reward, but differently in males and femalesPsychol. Res.2021851811943149304910.1007/s00426-019-01242-x Adobe Inc. Adobe Stock. Pixabay GmbH Pixabay. CoelloYBourgeoisJIachiniTEmbodied perception of reachable space: how do we manage threatening objects?Cogn. Process.20121313113510.1007/s10339-012-0470-z SamboCFForsterBWilliamsSCIannettiGDTo blink or not to blink: Fine cognitive tuning of the defensive peripersonal spaceJ. Neurosci.20123212921129271:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XhsVSlsbfF22973016670381310.1523/JNEUROSCI.0607-12.2012 SemmelmannKWeigeltSOnline psychophysics: Reaction time effects in cognitive experimentsBehav. Res. Methods201749124112602749617110.3758/s13428-016-0783-4 QuesqueFKeeping you at arm’s length: Modifying peripersonal space influences interpersonal distancePsychol. Res.2017817097201:STN:280:DC%2BC2s7lsVSksQ%3D%3D2736461210.1007/s00426-016-0782-1 MassonCTestosterone administration in women increases the size of their peripersonal spaceExp. Brain Res.2021239163916491:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhtlKnt73E3377021910.1007/s00221-021-06080-1 CartaudAQuesqueFCoelloYWearing a face mask against Covid-19 results in a reduction of social distancingPLoS ONE2020151:CAS:528:DC%2BB3cXis1emt7nE33284812772116910.1371/journal.pone.0243023 CalbiMThe consequences of COVID-19 on social interactions: an online study on face coveringSci. Rep.20211126011:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXivVWjt7o%3D33510195784400210.1038/s41598-021-81780-w BrysbaertMHow many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tablesJ. Cogn.201921631517234664 16730_CR55 A Maravita (16730_CR14) 2004; 8 16730_CR54 NP Holmes (16730_CR13) 2004; 372 16730_CR53 BA Nosek (16730_CR45) 2005; 134 KM Mooney (16730_CR33) 1992; 22 E Vagnoni (16730_CR31) 2018; 13 A Cartaud (16730_CR2) 2018; 9 F Quesque (16730_CR4) 2017; 81 HL Toppenberg (16730_CR35) 2015; 45 M Argyle (16730_CR30) 1965; 28 F Ferri (16730_CR21) 2015; 70 AL Anwyl-Irvine (16730_CR52) 2020; 52 G Seres (16730_CR37) 2020 A Mathews (16730_CR60) 1986; 95 16730_CR1 H Jeon (16730_CR66) 2018; 16 B Valdés-Conroy (16730_CR22) 2012; 7 MP Lisi (16730_CR40) 2021; 16 16730_CR47 M Brysbaert (16730_CR51) 2019; 2 R Neumann (16730_CR34) 2004; 40 KE Mathews (16730_CR32) 1974; 1 K Semmelmann (16730_CR50) 2017; 49 CM Judd (16730_CR62) 2012; 103 W Hofmann (16730_CR43) 2005; 31 A Cartaud (16730_CR23) 2020; 15 MF Gigliotti (16730_CR10) 2021; 85 16730_CR36 JM Leppänen (16730_CR46) 2004; 69 LOH Kroczek (16730_CR41) 2022; 12 M Minoura (16730_CR12) 2021; 8 A Gollwitzer (16730_CR42) 2020; 4 A Dijksterhuis (16730_CR48) 2003; 14 A Serino (16730_CR15) 2007; 18 T Iachini (16730_CR24) 2021; 239 JF Veale (16730_CR58) 2014; 19 Y Coello (16730_CR5) 2021; 15 G Rizzolatti (16730_CR67) 2005; 210 M Marini (16730_CR64) 2021; 11 R Welsch (16730_CR38) 2021; 11 M Calbi (16730_CR39) 2021; 11 AE Pinkham (16730_CR49) 2010; 10 16730_CR26 C Teneggi (16730_CR18) 2013; 23 16730_CR69 16730_CR68 G Ruggiero (16730_CR11) 2019; 63 R Welsch (16730_CR28) 2020; 62 G Ruggiero (16730_CR3) 2017; 81 A Sorokowska (16730_CR63) 2017; 48 16730_CR61 S Serino (16730_CR25) 2021; 31 G Rizzolatti (16730_CR7) 1981; 2 BA Nosek (16730_CR44) 2007; 54 CF Sambo (16730_CR19) 2012; 32 F Grundmann (16730_CR65) 2021; 16 F Cardini (16730_CR9) 2019; 9 KD Stone (16730_CR8) 2018; 236 N Sakuma (16730_CR27) 2021; 128 Y Coello (16730_CR20) 2012; 13 J Bourgeois (16730_CR6) 2014; 148 Y Gokmen (16730_CR17) 2021; 15 C Masson (16730_CR16) 2021; 239 T Iachini (16730_CR29) 2016; 45 RL Spitzer (16730_CR59) 2006; 166 16730_CR57 16730_CR56 |
References_xml | – volume: 103 start-page: 54 year: 2012 ident: 16730_CR62 publication-title: J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/a0028347 contributor: fullname: CM Judd – volume: 70 start-page: 468 year: 2015 ident: 16730_CR21 publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.001 contributor: fullname: F Ferri – volume: 40 start-page: 543 year: 2004 ident: 16730_CR34 publication-title: J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.006 contributor: fullname: R Neumann – volume: 14 start-page: 14 year: 2003 ident: 16730_CR48 publication-title: Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01412 contributor: fullname: A Dijksterhuis – ident: 16730_CR1 – volume: 31 start-page: R889 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR25 publication-title: Curr. Biol. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.06.001 contributor: fullname: S Serino – ident: 16730_CR61 doi: 10.48550/ARXIV.1406.5823 – ident: 16730_CR57 – volume: 210 start-page: 419 year: 2005 ident: 16730_CR67 publication-title: Anat. Embryol. (Berl.) doi: 10.1007/s00429-005-0039-z contributor: fullname: G Rizzolatti – volume: 32 start-page: 12921 year: 2012 ident: 16730_CR19 publication-title: J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0607-12.2012 contributor: fullname: CF Sambo – volume: 11 start-page: 5577 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR64 publication-title: Sci. Rep. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84806-5 contributor: fullname: M Marini – volume: 239 start-page: 1639 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR16 publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06080-1 contributor: fullname: C Masson – volume: 8 start-page: 79 year: 2004 ident: 16730_CR14 publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008 contributor: fullname: A Maravita – volume: 15 year: 2020 ident: 16730_CR23 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243023 contributor: fullname: A Cartaud – ident: 16730_CR53 – volume: 52 start-page: 388 year: 2020 ident: 16730_CR52 publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x contributor: fullname: AL Anwyl-Irvine – volume: 28 start-page: 289 year: 1965 ident: 16730_CR30 publication-title: Sociometry doi: 10.2307/2786027 contributor: fullname: M Argyle – volume: 19 start-page: 164 year: 2014 ident: 16730_CR58 publication-title: Lateral. Asymmet. Body Brain Cogn. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045 contributor: fullname: JF Veale – volume: 16 start-page: 18 year: 2018 ident: 16730_CR66 publication-title: Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. doi: 10.9758/cpn.2018.16.1.18 contributor: fullname: H Jeon – volume: 2 start-page: 147 year: 1981 ident: 16730_CR7 publication-title: Behav. Brain Res. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(81)90053-X contributor: fullname: G Rizzolatti – volume: 63 start-page: 52 year: 2019 ident: 16730_CR11 publication-title: J. Environ. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.04.004 contributor: fullname: G Ruggiero – volume: 49 start-page: 1241 year: 2017 ident: 16730_CR50 publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0783-4 contributor: fullname: K Semmelmann – volume: 10 start-page: 141 year: 2010 ident: 16730_CR49 publication-title: Emotion doi: 10.1037/a0017387 contributor: fullname: AE Pinkham – volume: 7 year: 2012 ident: 16730_CR22 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049162 contributor: fullname: B Valdés-Conroy – volume: 69 start-page: 22 year: 2004 ident: 16730_CR46 publication-title: Psychol. Res. Psychol. Forsch. doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0157-2 contributor: fullname: JM Leppänen – volume: 12 start-page: 2213 year: 2022 ident: 16730_CR41 publication-title: Sci. Rep. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-06086-x contributor: fullname: LOH Kroczek – ident: 16730_CR56 – volume: 45 start-page: 169 year: 2015 ident: 16730_CR35 publication-title: Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2082 contributor: fullname: HL Toppenberg – year: 2020 ident: 16730_CR37 publication-title: SSRN Electron. J. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3641367 contributor: fullname: G Seres – volume: 54 start-page: 14 year: 2007 ident: 16730_CR44 publication-title: Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.14 contributor: fullname: BA Nosek – volume: 95 start-page: 131 year: 1986 ident: 16730_CR60 publication-title: J. Abnorm. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.95.2.131 contributor: fullname: A Mathews – volume: 48 start-page: 577 year: 2017 ident: 16730_CR63 publication-title: J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. doi: 10.1177/0022022117698039 contributor: fullname: A Sorokowska – volume: 166 start-page: 1092 year: 2006 ident: 16730_CR59 publication-title: Arch. Intern. Med. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092 contributor: fullname: RL Spitzer – volume: 239 start-page: 1035 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR24 publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. doi: 10.1007/s00221-021-06040-9 contributor: fullname: T Iachini – volume: 85 start-page: 181 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR10 publication-title: Psychol. Res. doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01242-x contributor: fullname: MF Gigliotti – volume: 9 start-page: 657 year: 2018 ident: 16730_CR2 publication-title: Front. Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00657 contributor: fullname: A Cartaud – volume: 236 start-page: 161 year: 2018 ident: 16730_CR8 publication-title: Exp. Brain Res. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5115-0 contributor: fullname: KD Stone – volume: 372 start-page: 62 year: 2004 ident: 16730_CR13 publication-title: Neurosci. Lett. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024 contributor: fullname: NP Holmes – volume: 45 start-page: 154 year: 2016 ident: 16730_CR29 publication-title: J. Environ. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004 contributor: fullname: T Iachini – volume: 1 start-page: 367 year: 1974 ident: 16730_CR32 publication-title: Proc. Div. Personal. Soc. Psychol. contributor: fullname: KE Mathews – ident: 16730_CR36 – volume: 11 start-page: 11485 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR38 publication-title: Sci. Rep. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-90714-5 contributor: fullname: R Welsch – volume: 11 start-page: 2601 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR39 publication-title: Sci. Rep. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81780-w contributor: fullname: M Calbi – volume: 16 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR40 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255598 contributor: fullname: MP Lisi – volume: 13 year: 2018 ident: 16730_CR31 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192753 contributor: fullname: E Vagnoni – volume: 13 start-page: 131 year: 2012 ident: 16730_CR20 publication-title: Cogn. Process. doi: 10.1007/s10339-012-0470-z contributor: fullname: Y Coello – volume: 148 start-page: 91 year: 2014 ident: 16730_CR6 publication-title: Acta Psychol. (Amst.) doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.008 contributor: fullname: J Bourgeois – volume: 8 start-page: 248 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR12 publication-title: Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. contributor: fullname: M Minoura – volume: 2 start-page: 16 year: 2019 ident: 16730_CR51 publication-title: J. Cogn. doi: 10.5334/joc.72 contributor: fullname: M Brysbaert – ident: 16730_CR55 – volume: 81 start-page: 709 year: 2017 ident: 16730_CR4 publication-title: Psychol. Res. doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0782-1 contributor: fullname: F Quesque – volume: 15 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR5 publication-title: Front. Hum. Neurosci. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.636124 contributor: fullname: Y Coello – volume: 18 start-page: 642 year: 2007 ident: 16730_CR15 publication-title: Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01952.x contributor: fullname: A Serino – ident: 16730_CR26 doi: 10.1101/2021.04.25.441303 – volume: 23 start-page: 406 year: 2013 ident: 16730_CR18 publication-title: Curr. Biol. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043 contributor: fullname: C Teneggi – volume: 15 start-page: e20 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR17 publication-title: Disast. Med. Public Health Prep. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2020.295 contributor: fullname: Y Gokmen – volume: 81 start-page: 1232 year: 2017 ident: 16730_CR3 publication-title: Psychol. Res. doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0806-x contributor: fullname: G Ruggiero – volume: 128 start-page: 178 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR27 publication-title: Percept. Mot. Skills doi: 10.1177/0031512520973515 contributor: fullname: N Sakuma – volume: 16 year: 2021 ident: 16730_CR65 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249792 contributor: fullname: F Grundmann – volume: 9 start-page: 8606 year: 2019 ident: 16730_CR9 publication-title: Sci. Rep. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-45224-w contributor: fullname: F Cardini – ident: 16730_CR54 – volume: 22 start-page: 1442 year: 1992 ident: 16730_CR33 publication-title: J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb00959.x contributor: fullname: KM Mooney – volume: 4 start-page: 1186 year: 2020 ident: 16730_CR42 publication-title: Nat. Hum. Behav. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00977-7 contributor: fullname: A Gollwitzer – volume: 134 start-page: 565 year: 2005 ident: 16730_CR45 publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565 contributor: fullname: BA Nosek – ident: 16730_CR47 – ident: 16730_CR69 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/jfeca – ident: 16730_CR68 – volume: 62 start-page: 1095 year: 2020 ident: 16730_CR28 publication-title: Hum. Factors J. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. doi: 10.1177/0018720820956858 contributor: fullname: R Welsch – volume: 31 start-page: 1369 year: 2005 ident: 16730_CR43 publication-title: Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. doi: 10.1177/0146167205275613 contributor: fullname: W Hofmann |
SSID | ssj0000529419 |
Score | 2.4654193 |
Snippet | Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space... Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of... Abstract Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest crossref pubmed springer |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Publisher |
StartPage | 12777 |
SubjectTerms | 631/477 631/477/2811 Coronaviruses COVID-19 COVID-19 - epidemiology COVID-19 - prevention & control Face Humanities and Social Sciences Humans Masks multidisciplinary Pandemics Pandemics - prevention & control Personal Space Physical Distancing Protective equipment Science Science (multidisciplinary) Sensory integration Social behavior Social distancing |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1LbxUhFCamiYkbY32O1gYTd5aUx8DAsrY21YVurOmOAAOx0c696UwX_fc9wNzbXh9x427CkMnJOR_wMZzzgdBbSoNIWiUC5JiS1rWSaNUrYkLsjXMhhVASZD-rk9P205k8u3PVV84Jq_LA1XH7WbBcOuY7ERlMpL2DJZRyw6HFwUzry-xLzZ3NVFX15qZlZq6SoULvj7BS5Woy2HsxBbAmbGMlKoL9f2KZvydL_nJiWhai40fo4cwg8UG1fBvdi8NjdL_eKXn9BI1HpfAQA7HDh1--fTwizOBl_lV8cR7gYU6jHqYRL8sdI3iMJfd5xPmfLHY4uRDxhRt_7OFhgbOexGWtfsDwFewm6BJ-LgC6uM_kE1DzFJ0ef_h6eELmmxVIAIY2EeZNa4KJqjehpX2UnspexQDOBYKQwOVe8yRTFEKmXiYjA3RgrVMGtmiOiWdoa1gM8QXCQSehW51CFpoTofPeawesyAF30MHHBr1bedkuq4CGLQffQtsaEwsxsSUmljXofQ7EumcWvy4NAAk7Q8L-CxIN2lmF0c4jcrRc5YPqLP_YoDfr1zCW8gGJG-LiqvRRuTqX8wY9r1FfWyKkNqqjYGG3gYcNUzffDOffi153pshM6AbtrZBza9bfXfHyf7jiFXrAM-RpR3i3g7amy6v4GljU5HfLgLkBBMMV8g priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – databaseName: ProQuest Central dbid: BENPR link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELZgKyQuiDeBgozEjVqN7dixT4i-VDgUhCjqzbIdu1TQZNlsD_x7xo53q-V1ixwrmnjGns_zROhVXXselYwEwHFNGtsIomQnifah09b66H0OkD2Rx6fN-zNxVgxuYwmrXJ2J-aDuBp9s5LtMJkdjKt_3Zv6DpK5RybtaWmjcRFsMbgpshrb2Dk8-flpbWZIfq6G6ZMvUXO2OoLFSVhncwagE8SZ0QyPlwv1_Q5t_Bk3-5jnNCunoLrpTkCR-O7H-HroR-vvo1tRb8ucDNB7kBEQMAA_vf_jy7oBQjefJZHx54eGhhFP3yxHPc68RPIYcAz3iZJvFFkfrA76047cd3A841ZVYTFkQGL6C7RKm-O8DiDDuEggF6XmITo8OP-8fk9JhgXhAaktCnW6010F22jd1F4SrRSeDtxIWrY4AFp1iUcTAuYidiFp4mEAbKzVc1Szlj9CsH_rwBGGvIleNij4VnOO-dc4pC-jIAoZQ3oUKvV6tsplPhTRMdoBzZSaeGOCJyTwxtEJ7iRHrmakIdh4YFuem7CmTatkLS13LAwUd2wHVvGaawYgFJewqtL1ioyk7czTXclShl-vXsKeSo8T2YbjKc2TK0mWsQo8nrq8p4UJp2dZAYbshDxukbr7pL77mut0JKlOuKrSzkpxrsv69FE___xfP0G2WhLluCWu30Wy5uArPASct3YuyGX4B65EQbA priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest – databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access dbid: M48 link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3Pj9UgECabNSZejL-trgYTby5aoFA4GKO7blYPevGZvRGgsG7cbZ-v3cT97x1o-8zTp7cGaDNlhswHzHyD0POy9DwqGQmA45JUthJEyUYS7UOjrfXR-xwg-0keL6qPJ-JkB83ljqYJ7Ldu7VI9qcXq_OXPH1dvYMG_HlPG1asenFBKFINtFZVgsQR2Q9dYIuZKoXwT3B-5vpmuqJ5yZ7a_uuGfMo3_Nuz5dwjlH_eo2T0d3UI3J1yJ346GcBvthPYOuj5Wmry6i_rDnI6IAe7hg89fPxwSqvEyHSBfnHl4mIKr26HHy1x5BPchR0T3OJ3UYouj9QFf2P77Pm47nFgmVmNOBIavYDvAEH_egUHjJkFSsKV7aHH0_svBMZnqLRAPuG0g1OlKex1ko31VNkG4UjQyeCth0soI0NEpFkUMnIvYiKiFhwG0slLDxs1Sfh_ttl0bHiLsVeSqUtEn-jnua-ecsoCVLCAK5V0o0It5ls1ypNUw-TqcKzPqxIBOTNaJoQV6lxSxHpkosXNDtzo10wozidleWOpqHih43Aak5iXTDFosuGRXoL1ZjWY2M8Nkur5OpJAFerbuhhWWrk1sG7rLPEamnF3GCvRg1PpaEi6UlnUJEtYb9rAh6mZPe_Yts3gn4Ey5KtD-bDm_xfr3VDz6_188RjdYMuayJqzeQ7vD6jI8AdQ0uKd5KfwC610SNw priority: 102 providerName: Scholars Portal – databaseName: Springer Nature OA Free Journals dbid: C6C link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3Pb9UwDI7QEBIXxG_KBgoSNxaRNE2aHOGNaXCAC0O7RUmaiAnW97R2B_772WnfQ4Vx4FalbmXFjvwltr8Q8przKLPRmQE45qzxjWJGd5rZmDrrfcwxlgLZz_rktPl0ps5mmhzshVnk76V5O0CAwSYw2DIJDd7IYKdzG2MwXtOw0qvdeQpmrBph576Ymz9dxJ5C0X8Trvy7PPKPHGkJPcf3yb0ZM9J3k5EfkFupf0juTLdI_npEhqPSakgBytHVl28fj5iwdIOHwxfnER7mwul-HOim3CpCh1SqnQeKp7DU0-xjohd--HFI-zVFBonLqd-Bwl-oH0Ek_lyDs9IO4Sb4yWNyevzh6-qEzXcpsAiYbGQi2MZGm3RnY8O7pAJXnU7Ra5g0ngEWBlNnlZOUKncqWxVBQDReW9iUeSGfkL1-3adnhEaTpWlMjkgtJ2MbQjAecJAHtGBiSBV5s51lt5koM1xJdUvjJps4sIkrNnGiIu_REDtJpLsuA-AFbl49DlnrlRehlUlANO1Aa8lrW8OIh3AbKnKwNaOb1-Dgao2paSR8rMir3WtYPZgS8X1aXxUZjf24dV2Rp5PVd5pIZaxuOWjYLvxhoeryTX_-vTB0IygW0lTkcOs5v9X691Q8_z_xfXK3RufmLavbA7I3Xl6lF4CQxvCyLI1rKPEH5Q priority: 102 providerName: Springer Nature |
Title | During the COVID-19 pandemic participants prefer settings with a face mask, no interaction and at a closer distance |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.1038/s41598-022-16730-1 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35896701 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2695360573 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2696005722 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9326138 https://doaj.org/article/61505a1b73e1471da64302921b7a067b |
Volume | 12 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Nb9MwFH_ahpC4IL4JjMpI3FjWJI4d-8i6TQNpY0IM9RbZjj0q1qRqsgP_Pc9OUigfFy5Vmry0T34_6_1svw-AN0liqBPcxUiOkzhXOYsFr3gsja2kUsYZEwJkL_jZVf5hzuY7wMZcmBC0b_TisL5ZHtaLryG2crU00zFObHp5PvOcI6Viugu7CNBfluh9Qe9M5qkcEmQSFGvRSflEMlx2pRxfiH17GMqE5MXQC2b0R6Fs_9-45p8hk7-dmwZ3dPoA7g88krzr9X0IO7Z-BHf7zpLfH0N7HNIPCdI7Mvv45f1xnEqy8hvGy4XBiyGYuu5asgqdRkhrQwR0S_zOLFHEKWPJUrXfDkjdEF9VYt3nQBD8FaI6FDE3DQKYVJ6CInaewNXpyefZWTz0V4gN8rQuTrXMpZGWV9LkSWWZTljFrVEcxy9xSBW1yBxzllLmKuYkMyiQ5opLXKiplD6Fvbqp7XMgRjgqcuGMLzdHTaG1Fgq5kUIGIYy2EbwdR7lc9WU0ynD8TUXZm6dE85TBPGUawZE3xEbSl8AON5r1dTkAofSV7JlKdUFtih62Qq1pkskM7yh0wTqC_dGM5TAv2zLj_rjaF4GM4PXmMc4of0yiatvcBhnuc3SzLIJnvdU3moyoiaDYwsOWqttPEMShavcA2ggORuT8VOvfQ_Hiv__oJdzLPOSTIs6Kfdjr1rf2FRKoTk9w2syLCdw5Orm4_ITfZnw2CZsR-Hmei0mYUD8A8gwdLw |
link.rule.ids | 230,315,730,783,787,867,888,2109,2228,12070,21402,24332,27938,27939,31733,31734,33758,33759,41134,42203,43324,43819,51590,53806,53808,74081,74638 |
linkProvider | National Library of Medicine |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELagCMEF8SZQwEjcqFU7Thz7hKCl2kIplxbtzXIcGyposmzSA_-eGSe71fK6rRIrmvWMM1_m8Q0hLzn3MmoVGYBjzgpXlEyrRjHjQ2Oc89H7VCB7rGanxft5OZ8Cbv1UVrl6J6YXddN5jJHv5goTjUjf93rxg-HUKMyuTiM0rpJryMOF3PnVvFrHWDCLVQgz9cpwqXd78FfYUwZfYEKBcTOx4Y8Sbf_fsOafJZO_5U2TOzq4TW5NOJK-GRV_h1wJ7V1yfZws-fMe6fdT-yEFeEf3Pn0-3GfC0AUGjM_PPPyYiqnboaeLNGmE9iFVQPcUI7PU0eh8oOeu_7ZD244iq8Ry7IGg8BTqBljiv3dgwLRBCAq2c5-cHrw72Zuxab4C84DTBiZqUxhvgmqML3gTypqXjQreKdg0HgEq1jqPZQxSlrEpoyk9LBCFUwY-1JyQD8hW27XhEaFeR6kLHT3SzUlf1XWtHWAjBwhC-zpk5NVql-1ipNGwKf0ttR11YkEnNunEioy8RUWsVyIFdrrQLb_Y6URZZLIvnagrGQR42Aakljw3OVxx4ILrjGyv1Ginc9nbSyvKyIv1bThRmCZxbegu0hqFPbp5npGHo9bXkshSG1VxkLDasIcNUTfvtGdfE2s3AmUhdUZ2VpZzKda_t-Lx___Fc3JjdvLxyB4dHn94Qm7maNi8Ynm1TbaG5UV4CohpqJ-lY_ELSm8R9w |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELagCNQL4t1AASNxo9bGcezYJwRdVi2gwoGivVm2Y0MFTZbN9sC_Z-x4t1pet1ViRbOebzJfPC-EnpelY0GKQIAcl6Q2NSdStIIo51tljAvOpQTZE3F0Wr-d83nOfxpyWuX6nZhe1G3v4hn5pBIx0Bjb901CTov4OJ29XPwgcYJUjLTmcRpX0bWmBkcH2G7mzea8JUa0aqpy3UzJ5GQA3xXry-BrjAoAOqFbvim18P8b7_wzffK3GGpyTbNb6GbmlPjVCILb6Irv7qDr45TJn3fRME2liBioHj788Pl4SqjCi3h4fH7m4EdOrO5WA16kqSN48CkbesDxlBYbHIzz-NwM3w5w1-PYYWI51kNgeAo2K1jivvcAZtxGOgo4uodOZ28-HR6RPGuBOOBsK0KtqpVTXrTK1WXruS15K7wzAjatDEAbrawCD54xHloeFHewgNZGKPhoM5TdRztd3_k9hJ0MTNYyuNh6jrnGWisN8CQDbEI66wv0Yr3LejG21NApFM6kHnWiQSc66UTTAr2OitisjO2w04V--UVn69Kxqz031DbMU_C2LUjNykpVcMWAO7YF2l-rUWcbHfQlogr0bHMbrCuGTEzn-4u0RsR63aoq0INR6xtJGJdKNCVI2GzhYUvU7Tvd2dfUwTuSZspkgQ7WyLkU699b8fD__-IpugEWod8fn7x7hHariOuyIVWzj3ZWywv_GMjTyj5JVvELT4cWLA |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=During+the+COVID-19+pandemic+participants+prefer+settings+with+a+face+mask%2C+no+interaction+and+at+a+closer+distance&rft.jtitle=Scientific+reports&rft.au=K%C3%BChne%2C+K&rft.au=Fischer%2C+M.+H&rft.au=Jeglinski-Mende%2C+M.+A&rft.date=2022-07-27&rft.pub=Nature+Publishing+Group&rft.eissn=2045-2322&rft.volume=12&rft.issue=1&rft_id=info:doi/10.1038%2Fs41598-022-16730-1&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2045-2322&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2045-2322&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2045-2322&client=summon |