Optimizing the mnemonic similarity task for efficient, widespread use

Introduction: The Mnemonic Similarity Task (MST) has become a popular test of memory and, in particular, of hippocampal function. It has been heavily used in research settings and is currently included as an alternate outcome measure on a number of clinical trials. However, as it typically requires...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inFrontiers in behavioral neuroscience Vol. 17; p. 1080366
Main Authors Stark, Craig E. L., Noche, Jessica A., Ebersberger, Jarrett R., Mayer, Lizabeth, Stark, Shauna M.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Switzerland Frontiers Research Foundation 26.01.2023
Frontiers Media S.A
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1662-5153
1662-5153
DOI10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1080366

Cover

More Information
Summary:Introduction: The Mnemonic Similarity Task (MST) has become a popular test of memory and, in particular, of hippocampal function. It has been heavily used in research settings and is currently included as an alternate outcome measure on a number of clinical trials. However, as it typically requires ~15 min to administer and benefits substantially from an experienced test administrator to ensure the instructions are well-understood, its use in trials and in other settings is somewhat restricted. Several different variants of the MST are in common use that alter the task format (study-test vs. continuous) and the response prompt given to participants (old/similar/new vs. old/new). Methods: In eight online experiments, we sought to address three main goals: (1) To determine whether a robust version of the task could be created that could be conducted in half the traditional time; (2) To determine whether the test format or response prompt choice significantly impacted the MST’s results; and (3) To determine how robust the MST is to repeat testing. In Experiments 1–7, participants received both the traditional and alternate forms of the MST to determine how well the alternate version captured the traditional task’s performance. In Experiment 8, participants were given the MST four times over approximately 4 weeks. Results: In Experiments 1–7, we found that test format had no effect on the reliability of the MST, but that shifting to the two-choice response format significantly reduced its ability to reflect the traditional MST’s score. We also found that the full running time could be cut it half or less without appreciable reduction in reliability. We confirmed the efficacy of this reduced task in older adults as well. Here, and in Experiment 8, we found that while there often are no effects of repeat-testing, small effects are possible, but appear limited to the initial testing session. Discussion: The optimized version of the task developed here (oMST) is freely available for web-based experiment delivery and provides an accurate estimate of the same memory ability as the classic MST in less than half the time.
Bibliography:ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
Reviewed by: Brianne Alyssia Kent, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom; Oliver Hardt, McGill University, Canada
Specialty section: This article was submitted to Learning and Memory, a section of the journal Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
Edited by: Pedro Bekinschtein, CONICET Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), Argentina
ISSN:1662-5153
1662-5153
DOI:10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1080366