Action representations in perception, motor control and learning: implications for medical education

Medical Education 2011: 45: 119–131 Objectives  The motor behaviours or ‘actions’ that provide the basis for precision limb control, including the performance of complex medical procedures, are represented at different levels in the central nervous system. This review focuses on how these representa...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inMedical education Vol. 45; no. 2; pp. 119 - 131
Main Authors Elliott, Digby, Grierson, Lawrence E M, Hayes, Spencer J, Lyons, James
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Oxford, UK Blackwell Publishing Ltd 01.02.2011
Wiley-Blackwell
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
More Information
Summary:Medical Education 2011: 45: 119–131 Objectives  The motor behaviours or ‘actions’ that provide the basis for precision limb control, including the performance of complex medical procedures, are represented at different levels in the central nervous system. This review focuses on how these representations influence the way people perceive, execute and learn goal‐directed movements. Perception and attention  The neural processes associated with paying attention to an object are part and particle of the same processes engaged to physically interact with that object. The automatic way in which specific actions are engaged makes it important that we structure perceptual motor environments in a manner that facilitates goal actions and minimises the likelihood of unwanted actions. Motor control  Most actions are organised to optimise speed, accuracy and energy expenditure while avoiding worst‐case outcomes. To achieve a good outcome on movements, the performer must have the opportunity to experiment with the way specific actions are executed. Early in the discovery process, errors are necessary if the performer is to determine his or her performance boundaries. Motor learning  As learning progresses, representations of action become predictive. For example, if rapid corrective processes are to operate, the performer needs to anticipate sensorimotor consequences of movement. Thus, practice should be specific to the conditions under which actions are performed, and the performer. Although nothing can replace physical practice, complex representations of action can develop by observing both expert performers and learners. In many cases, practice scenarios that include both physical practice and observations of other learners can be the most efficient use of time and resources. Conclusions  Although most of the experiments reviewed here involved laboratory tasks such as rapid aiming and movement sequencing, the majority of the principles apply to motor control and learning in more complex situations. Thus, they should be considered when developing methods to train medical personnel to perform perceptual motor procedures with precision.
Bibliography:istex:004B07B8B28B19B6265A7A4E143834CD7C6536F6
ArticleID:MEDU3851
ark:/67375/WNG-ZPFNDJJN-B
ObjectType-Article-2
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-3
content type line 23
ObjectType-Review-1
ISSN:0308-0110
1365-2923
DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2923.2010.03851.x