The Relationship Between Experiential Avoidance and Impulsiveness in a Nonclinical Sample

Experiential avoidance (EA) has been connected to various behavioural indicators of psychological distress, implicated in the etiology and maintenance of psychological disorders, and is the target of prevalent psychological treatments. However, the reasons that individuals engage in dysfunctional EA...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBehaviour change Vol. 29; no. 1; pp. 25 - 35
Main Authors Berghoff, Christopher R., Pomerantz, Andrew M., Pettibone, Jonathan C., Segrist, Daniel J., Bedwell, David R.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Cambridge, UK Cambridge University Press 01.03.2012
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
More Information
Summary:Experiential avoidance (EA) has been connected to various behavioural indicators of psychological distress, implicated in the etiology and maintenance of psychological disorders, and is the target of prevalent psychological treatments. However, the reasons that individuals engage in dysfunctional EA are little understood. One hypothesis focuses on the preference for small, immediate rewards above larger, delayed rewards — in other words, impulsiveness. We examined the relationship of impulsiveness, measured both by self-report and behaviourally, to EA, while statistically controlling for possible confounding variables (i.e., intellectual functioning, gender, ethnicity), in a sample of normal undergraduate participants. Regression analyses suggest a significant relationship exists between EA and self-reported (but not behaviourally measured) impulsiveness. Exploratory analyses indicate nonplanning-type impulsiveness might be the best predictor of EA. Possible confounding variables did not account for a significant amount of variance within either model examined. Thus, support is provided for a theoretically proposed relationship within the Acceptance and Commitment Therapy model of psychopathology between EA and impulsiveness.
Bibliography:Behaviour Change, Vol. 29, No. 1, 2012: 25-35
BehCha_c.jpg
ISSN:0813-4839
2049-7768
DOI:10.1017/bec.2012.6