The replication crisis
The problem is easily stated and well known. Research findings that cannot be reproduced or replicated lack trustworthiness and that's why, in part, reports detail the methods employed in finding derivation. Stating methods, including methods of statistical analysis, enables researchers to reru...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of advanced nursing Vol. 77; no. 2; pp. 501 - 503 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
01.02.2021
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Summary: | The problem is easily stated and well known. Research findings that cannot be reproduced or replicated lack trustworthiness and that's why, in part, reports detail the methods employed in finding derivation. Stating methods, including methods of statistical analysis, enables researchers to rerun and thereby test (confirm or not) the conclusions claimed in studies. However, it is now realized that many findings in, for example, psychology, social science, and some branches of medicine cannot be replicated (Stupple, Singerman, & Celi, 2019; Yong, 2018). The significance and ramifications of this realization are difficult to underplay. Findings that refuse replication undercut what we think we know and although replication might, as a topic, seem arcane, the subject is not simply of concern to those entangled in academic or research methods disputes. Popular science texts such as Tim Harford's How to Make the World Add Up (2020), and – in the UK – radio programmes such as Analysis’ The Replication Crisis (2018) have brought the issue to public attention. The subject even has its own Wikipedia page (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Replication_crisis). |
---|---|
Bibliography: | SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 content type line 14 ObjectType-Editorial-2 ObjectType-Commentary-1 content type line 23 |
ISSN: | 0309-2402 1365-2648 1365-2648 |
DOI: | 10.1111/jan.14697 |