Study of non-visually induced smooth pursuit eye movements and their predictability using pseudorandom target motion

It has been found that the smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are elicited by not only visual stimuli but also non-visual information such as the subject's fingertip movement and a moving sound source. We have already reported the quantitative analysis of SPEM which were induced by somatosenso...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inNippon Jibi Inkoka Gakkai Kaiho Vol. 100; no. 12; p. 1450
Main Authors Watanabe, N, Hashiba, M
Format Journal Article
LanguageJapanese
Published Japan 01.12.1997
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0030-6622
DOI10.3950/jibiinkoka.100.1450

Cover

Abstract It has been found that the smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are elicited by not only visual stimuli but also non-visual information such as the subject's fingertip movement and a moving sound source. We have already reported the quantitative analysis of SPEM which were induced by somatosensory and acoustic information. In the previous study, we used a sinusoidal waveform that could be highly predictable. Since it is wellknown that predictive control has an important role in the normal SPEM, we expect the predictive control to function in non-visually induced SPEM (NVSPEM). We quantitatively analyzed NVSPEM and normal SPEM evoked by pseudorandom target motion in ten human subjects who had no ocular, oculomotor or vestibular disorders. NVSPEM were induced by the following two non-visual targets: 1, subjects' fingertip motion as a somatosensory target ("Somato"), 2, a small loudspeaker (3-cm diameter.) generating white noise with an intensity of about 60 dB (A) as an acoustic target ("Acoustic"). A servo-controlled swing arm of 50cm was used to drive the subject's fingertip and the acoustic target of the small loudspeaker. The horizontal motion of the swing arm was controlled by a personal computer. The pseudorandom target motion was generated by mixing four sinusoids (0.1, 0.2, 0.4, 0.8 Hz) of which the phases were randomly selected and the peak velocities were equally set at 19 deg/s. The mean peak velocity of the target was 26.2 deg/s and the amplitude was limited within 15 deg. Horizontal eye movements were recorded by DC electro-oculography and on an analogue datatape. The experiment was performed for 30 s in complete darkness so that the subjects' fingertip and loudspeaker as such remain invisible to the subject. Signals from the data recorder were smoothed by a low pass analogue filter of 20Hz, after digitization with a sampling frequency of 200 Hz and precision of 12 bits, and stored on a computer. The slow and quick eye movement components, both of which were present in each class of horizontal eye movement investigated, were identified and separated by a computer. Then we developed a method of automatic quantitative analysis of ocular tracking eye movement. Gain and phase values for the smooth pursuit eye movements were obtained in each condition. In the lower frequency area, the gain elicited by the pseudorandom stimulation was lower than the smooth pursuit gain for sinusoidal (predictable) stimulation in all conditions. In the highest frequency, gain values did not differ significantly among the three. For the sinusoidal stimulation, the phase of the smooth component of "Visual" always had a lag and that of "Somato" and "Acoustic" had a lead in lower frequencies. All conditions had a phase shift, decreasing with increasing frequency. For the pseudorandom stimulation the phase of the SPEM had a lead only in the lowest frequency (0.1 Hz). On the other hand, in the NVSPEM the phases of the three lower frequencies had a lead which had a tendency of a larger phase lead with decreasingly frequency. In the highest frequency (0.8 Hz), we could see a short phase lag. These findings support the idea that SPEM and NVSPEM have a mutual or similar physiologic system and overlap part of the anatomical pathway.
AbstractList It has been found that the smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are elicited by not only visual stimuli but also non-visual information such as the subject's fingertip movement and a moving sound source. We have already reported the quantitative analysis of SPEM which were induced by somatosensory and acoustic information. In the previous study, we used a sinusoidal waveform that could be highly predictable. Since it is wellknown that predictive control has an important role in the normal SPEM, we expect the predictive control to function in non-visually induced SPEM (NVSPEM). We quantitatively analyzed NVSPEM and normal SPEM evoked by pseudorandom target motion in ten human subjects who had no ocular, oculomotor or vestibular disorders. NVSPEM were induced by the following two non-visual targets: 1, subjects' fingertip motion as a somatosensory target ("Somato"), 2, a small loudspeaker (3-cm diameter.) generating white noise with an intensity of about 60 dB (A) as an acoustic target ("Acoustic"). A servo-controlled swing arm of 50cm was used to drive the subject's fingertip and the acoustic target of the small loudspeaker. The horizontal motion of the swing arm was controlled by a personal computer. The pseudorandom target motion was generated by mixing four sinusoids (0.1, 0.2, 0.4, 0.8 Hz) of which the phases were randomly selected and the peak velocities were equally set at 19 deg/s. The mean peak velocity of the target was 26.2 deg/s and the amplitude was limited within 15 deg. Horizontal eye movements were recorded by DC electro-oculography and on an analogue datatape. The experiment was performed for 30 s in complete darkness so that the subjects' fingertip and loudspeaker as such remain invisible to the subject. Signals from the data recorder were smoothed by a low pass analogue filter of 20Hz, after digitization with a sampling frequency of 200 Hz and precision of 12 bits, and stored on a computer. The slow and quick eye movement components, both of which were present in each class of horizontal eye movement investigated, were identified and separated by a computer. Then we developed a method of automatic quantitative analysis of ocular tracking eye movement. Gain and phase values for the smooth pursuit eye movements were obtained in each condition. In the lower frequency area, the gain elicited by the pseudorandom stimulation was lower than the smooth pursuit gain for sinusoidal (predictable) stimulation in all conditions. In the highest frequency, gain values did not differ significantly among the three. For the sinusoidal stimulation, the phase of the smooth component of "Visual" always had a lag and that of "Somato" and "Acoustic" had a lead in lower frequencies. All conditions had a phase shift, decreasing with increasing frequency. For the pseudorandom stimulation the phase of the SPEM had a lead only in the lowest frequency (0.1 Hz). On the other hand, in the NVSPEM the phases of the three lower frequencies had a lead which had a tendency of a larger phase lead with decreasingly frequency. In the highest frequency (0.8 Hz), we could see a short phase lag. These findings support the idea that SPEM and NVSPEM have a mutual or similar physiologic system and overlap part of the anatomical pathway.It has been found that the smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are elicited by not only visual stimuli but also non-visual information such as the subject's fingertip movement and a moving sound source. We have already reported the quantitative analysis of SPEM which were induced by somatosensory and acoustic information. In the previous study, we used a sinusoidal waveform that could be highly predictable. Since it is wellknown that predictive control has an important role in the normal SPEM, we expect the predictive control to function in non-visually induced SPEM (NVSPEM). We quantitatively analyzed NVSPEM and normal SPEM evoked by pseudorandom target motion in ten human subjects who had no ocular, oculomotor or vestibular disorders. NVSPEM were induced by the following two non-visual targets: 1, subjects' fingertip motion as a somatosensory target ("Somato"), 2, a small loudspeaker (3-cm diameter.) generating white noise with an intensity of about 60 dB (A) as an acoustic target ("Acoustic"). A servo-controlled swing arm of 50cm was used to drive the subject's fingertip and the acoustic target of the small loudspeaker. The horizontal motion of the swing arm was controlled by a personal computer. The pseudorandom target motion was generated by mixing four sinusoids (0.1, 0.2, 0.4, 0.8 Hz) of which the phases were randomly selected and the peak velocities were equally set at 19 deg/s. The mean peak velocity of the target was 26.2 deg/s and the amplitude was limited within 15 deg. Horizontal eye movements were recorded by DC electro-oculography and on an analogue datatape. The experiment was performed for 30 s in complete darkness so that the subjects' fingertip and loudspeaker as such remain invisible to the subject. Signals from the data recorder were smoothed by a low pass analogue filter of 20Hz, after digitization with a sampling frequency of 200 Hz and precision of 12 bits, and stored on a computer. The slow and quick eye movement components, both of which were present in each class of horizontal eye movement investigated, were identified and separated by a computer. Then we developed a method of automatic quantitative analysis of ocular tracking eye movement. Gain and phase values for the smooth pursuit eye movements were obtained in each condition. In the lower frequency area, the gain elicited by the pseudorandom stimulation was lower than the smooth pursuit gain for sinusoidal (predictable) stimulation in all conditions. In the highest frequency, gain values did not differ significantly among the three. For the sinusoidal stimulation, the phase of the smooth component of "Visual" always had a lag and that of "Somato" and "Acoustic" had a lead in lower frequencies. All conditions had a phase shift, decreasing with increasing frequency. For the pseudorandom stimulation the phase of the SPEM had a lead only in the lowest frequency (0.1 Hz). On the other hand, in the NVSPEM the phases of the three lower frequencies had a lead which had a tendency of a larger phase lead with decreasingly frequency. In the highest frequency (0.8 Hz), we could see a short phase lag. These findings support the idea that SPEM and NVSPEM have a mutual or similar physiologic system and overlap part of the anatomical pathway.
It has been found that the smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are elicited by not only visual stimuli but also non-visual information such as the subject's fingertip movement and a moving sound source. We have already reported the quantitative analysis of SPEM which were induced by somatosensory and acoustic information. In the previous study, we used a sinusoidal waveform that could be highly predictable. Since it is wellknown that predictive control has an important role in the normal SPEM, we expect the predictive control to function in non-visually induced SPEM (NVSPEM). We quantitatively analyzed NVSPEM and normal SPEM evoked by pseudorandom target motion in ten human subjects who had no ocular, oculomotor or vestibular disorders. NVSPEM were induced by the following two non-visual targets: 1, subjects' fingertip motion as a somatosensory target ("Somato"), 2, a small loudspeaker (3-cm diameter.) generating white noise with an intensity of about 60 dB (A) as an acoustic target ("Acoustic"). A servo-controlled swing arm of 50cm was used to drive the subject's fingertip and the acoustic target of the small loudspeaker. The horizontal motion of the swing arm was controlled by a personal computer. The pseudorandom target motion was generated by mixing four sinusoids (0.1, 0.2, 0.4, 0.8 Hz) of which the phases were randomly selected and the peak velocities were equally set at 19 deg/s. The mean peak velocity of the target was 26.2 deg/s and the amplitude was limited within 15 deg. Horizontal eye movements were recorded by DC electro-oculography and on an analogue datatape. The experiment was performed for 30 s in complete darkness so that the subjects' fingertip and loudspeaker as such remain invisible to the subject. Signals from the data recorder were smoothed by a low pass analogue filter of 20Hz, after digitization with a sampling frequency of 200 Hz and precision of 12 bits, and stored on a computer. The slow and quick eye movement components, both of which were present in each class of horizontal eye movement investigated, were identified and separated by a computer. Then we developed a method of automatic quantitative analysis of ocular tracking eye movement. Gain and phase values for the smooth pursuit eye movements were obtained in each condition. In the lower frequency area, the gain elicited by the pseudorandom stimulation was lower than the smooth pursuit gain for sinusoidal (predictable) stimulation in all conditions. In the highest frequency, gain values did not differ significantly among the three. For the sinusoidal stimulation, the phase of the smooth component of "Visual" always had a lag and that of "Somato" and "Acoustic" had a lead in lower frequencies. All conditions had a phase shift, decreasing with increasing frequency. For the pseudorandom stimulation the phase of the SPEM had a lead only in the lowest frequency (0.1 Hz). On the other hand, in the NVSPEM the phases of the three lower frequencies had a lead which had a tendency of a larger phase lead with decreasingly frequency. In the highest frequency (0.8 Hz), we could see a short phase lag. These findings support the idea that SPEM and NVSPEM have a mutual or similar physiologic system and overlap part of the anatomical pathway.
Author Hashiba, M
Watanabe, N
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: N
  surname: Watanabe
  fullname: Watanabe, N
  organization: Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Nagoya City University Medical School
– sequence: 2
  givenname: M
  surname: Hashiba
  fullname: Hashiba, M
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9465609$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNotkLtOwzAYRj0UlVL6BAjJE1uKHcdJPKKKm1SJAZgjJ_7duk3s4AtS3p5IdPqW853h3KCFdRYQuqNkywQnjyfTGmPP7iy3lJAtLThZoBUhjGRlmefXaBOCaQkhoiAsJ0u0FEXJSyJWKH7GpCbsNJ6d2a8JSfb9hI1VqQOFw-BcPOIx-ZBMxDABHtwvDGBjwNIqHI9gPB49KNNF2ZrexAmnYOwBjwGScn6m3ICj9AeI8zkaZ2_RlZZ9gM1l1-j75flr95btP17fd0_77ESrOmYadCGoZkwUlLK65V2rBS9qVolOFcAoqeoKaiVlrVXHS6E1l6JgmudUKmBsjR7-vaN3PwlCbAYTOuh7acGl0FSCc0poPYP3FzC1A6hm9GaQfmoumdgfxIlwaQ
ContentType Journal Article
DBID CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
DOI 10.3950/jibiinkoka.100.1450
DatabaseName Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic
MEDLINE
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
ExternalDocumentID 9465609
Genre English Abstract
Journal Article
GroupedDBID .GJ
2WC
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
F5P
JSF
KQ8
NPM
OK1
RJT
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-j178t-fef491f33941138b5cbf9548379cd4e310787e8daa8fdc569ff5a943f521ade33
ISSN 0030-6622
IngestDate Fri Jul 11 11:11:16 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:32:07 EST 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 12
Language Japanese
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-j178t-fef491f33941138b5cbf9548379cd4e310787e8daa8fdc569ff5a943f521ade33
Notes ObjectType-Article-2
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 23
PMID 9465609
PQID 79551018
PQPubID 23479
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_79551018
pubmed_primary_9465609
PublicationCentury 1900
PublicationDate 1997-Dec
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 1997-12-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 12
  year: 1997
  text: 1997-Dec
PublicationDecade 1990
PublicationPlace Japan
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Japan
PublicationTitle Nippon Jibi Inkoka Gakkai Kaiho
PublicationTitleAlternate Nihon Jibiinkoka Gakkai Kaiho
PublicationYear 1997
SSID ssib000940320
ssib007762623
ssj0066768
ssib000959836
ssib002252301
ssib001174089
ssib022575415
ssib058493820
ssib005879730
ssib001174090
Score 1.4781256
Snippet It has been found that the smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are elicited by not only visual stimuli but also non-visual information such as the subject's...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
StartPage 1450
SubjectTerms Adult
Auditory Perception
Female
Humans
Male
Pursuit, Smooth - physiology
Touch
Title Study of non-visually induced smooth pursuit eye movements and their predictability using pseudorandom target motion
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9465609
https://www.proquest.com/docview/79551018
Volume 100
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lj5swELbarVT1UvW16vbpQ2-ILWCI8bGqdhvtI1XVRMoNGduoTnYDClBp--s7tkniVFv1cUEEQYQ9H-Nv7JnPCL0jMC7AwKfCnPEyTKuYh0wleUhJGZM8EoxxU5x8ORmNZ-nZPJvvdiq01SVdeSx-3FpX8j9WhWtgV1Ml-w-W3f4pXIBzsC8cwcJw_Csbf90qQter8Ltue351Zer4ZG9W9dvrGswQNP267XUXqBsVXNdWHrxrN4mTem1UAqQWndPrvgl6O3nQtKqXgI6VNNUnNls8cBv--Gx2opsG0HOmSw2OZlkvefCJL5dcB-dcf6t3k_XAQHmp9lZ-xmYXp5LvJmTlUItHvTSOwZ0SiD1Hyb47jSIfN4nnHePUicz-6rYJy0ye4wLeVtuXNbkbx5u790WyJ5-L09nFRTE9mU_vonsJpXZ1_vyLxypZaraF91kky4mvkkfTyBPZt7_3yoHNVLmnWpZTRr0oi8IA4rFGuJ1mXlQKlI4Rq0LoCIHJInaEYOgtJ35lGv3-lib_PtSxlGf6CD0cYhX8wQHvMbqz4E_Q_cshG-Mp6iz-cF1hH394wB92-MMD_jDgD2_xhwFZ2OIP7-MPW_xhH3_Y4Q87_D1Ds9OT6cdxOOziES5imndhpaqUxRUhLI3h8y8zUVZGZZBQJmSqILyAMUPlkvO8kiIbsarKOEtJBcSSS0XIITqAVqjnCEshEwEUOGJAPMssYSKW0J8iF5ESwEuP0NtN1xXgJc3SF1-pum8LyjKrTXeEDl2PFo0TcymYEQyM2Is_PvoSPdh9A6_QQbfu1WsgpF35xsLvJ3uYhJE
linkProvider Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Study+of+non-visually+induced+smooth+pursuit+eye+movements+and+their+predictability+using+pseudorandom+target+motion&rft.jtitle=Nippon+Jibi+Inkoka+Gakkai+Kaiho&rft.au=Watanabe%2C+N&rft.au=Hashiba%2C+M&rft.date=1997-12-01&rft.issn=0030-6622&rft.volume=100&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=1450&rft_id=info:doi/10.3950%2Fjibiinkoka.100.1450&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0030-6622&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0030-6622&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0030-6622&client=summon