The Neural Correlates of Religious and Nonreligious Belief
While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has u...
Saved in:
Published in | PloS one Vol. 4; no. 10; p. e7272 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Public Library of Science
01.10.2009
Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1], and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2]. However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly.
We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjects-fifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelievers-as they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of "true" vs judgments of "false") was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3], [4], [5], [6], emotional associations [7], reward [8], [9], [10], and goal-driven behavior [11]. This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks.
While religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world. |
---|---|
AbstractList | While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1], and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2]. However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjects-fifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelievers-as they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of "true" vs judgments of "false") was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3], [4], [5], [6], emotional associations [7], reward [8], [9], [10], and goal-driven behavior [11]. This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks. While religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world. While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1], and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2]. However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly.BACKGROUNDWhile religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1], and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2]. However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly.We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjects-fifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelievers-as they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of "true" vs judgments of "false") was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3], [4], [5], [6], emotional associations [7], reward [8], [9], [10], and goal-driven behavior [11]. This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks.METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGSWe used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjects-fifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelievers-as they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of "true" vs judgments of "false") was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3], [4], [5], [6], emotional associations [7], reward [8], [9], [10], and goal-driven behavior [11]. This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks.While religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world.CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCEWhile religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world. Background While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1] , and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2] . However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly. Methodology/Principal Findings We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjectsafifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelieversaas they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of atruea vs judgments of afalsea) was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3] , [4] , [5] , [6] , emotional associations [7] , reward [8] , [9] , [10] , and goal-driven behavior [11] . This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks. Conclusions/Significance While religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world. Background While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1], and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2]. However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly. Methodology/Principal Findings We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjects—fifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelievers—as they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of “true” vs judgments of “false”) was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3], [4], [5], [6], emotional associations [7], reward [8], [9], [10], and goal-driven behavior [11]. This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks. Conclusions/Significance While religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world. Background While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1], and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2]. However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly. Methodology/Principal Findings We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjects—fifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelievers—as they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of “true” vs judgments of “false”) was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3], [4], [5], [6], emotional associations [7], reward [8], [9], [10], and goal-driven behavior [11]. This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks. Conclusions/Significance While religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world. While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the brain. Nor is it known whether religious believers and nonbelievers differ in how they evaluate statements of fact. Our lab previously has used functional neuroimaging to study belief as a general mode of cognition [1], and others have looked specifically at religious belief [2]. However, no research has compared these two states of mind directly. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure signal changes in the brains of thirty subjects-fifteen committed Christians and fifteen nonbelievers-as they evaluated the truth and falsity of religious and nonreligious propositions. For both groups, and in both categories of stimuli, belief (judgments of "true" vs judgments of "false") was associated with greater signal in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area important for self-representation [3], [4], [5], [6], emotional associations [7], reward [8], [9], [10], and goal-driven behavior [11]. This region showed greater signal whether subjects believed statements about God, the Virgin Birth, etc. or statements about ordinary facts. A comparison of both stimulus categories suggests that religious thinking is more associated with brain regions that govern emotion, self-representation, and cognitive conflict, while thinking about ordinary facts is more reliant upon memory retrieval networks. While religious and nonreligious thinking differentially engage broad regions of the frontal, parietal, and medial temporal lobes, the difference between belief and disbelief appears to be content-independent. Our study compares religious thinking with ordinary cognition and, as such, constitutes a step toward developing a neuropsychology of religion. However, these findings may also further our understanding of how the brain accepts statements of all kinds to be valid descriptions of the world. |
Audience | Academic |
Author | Kaplan, Jonas T. Bookheimer, Susan Y. Cohen, Mark S. Curiel, Ashley Harris, Sam Iacoboni, Marco |
AuthorAffiliation | 3 Department of Clinical Psychology, Pepperdine University, Los Angeles, California, United States of America 7 The Brain Research Institute, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America 1 UCLA Ahmanson-Lovelace Brain Mapping Center, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America 9 Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America 10 The Reason Project, Santa Monica, California, United States of America 5 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America Indiana University, United States of America 2 Brain and Creativity Institute and Department of Psychology, University of Southern California (USC), Los Angeles, California, United States of America 8 Departments of Neurology, Radiological Sciences, Biom |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 4 Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: 3 Department of Clinical Psychology, Pepperdine University, Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: 5 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: 1 UCLA Ahmanson-Lovelace Brain Mapping Center, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: 2 Brain and Creativity Institute and Department of Psychology, University of Southern California (USC), Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: 7 The Brain Research Institute, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: 9 Department of Psychology, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: 8 Departments of Neurology, Radiological Sciences, Biomedical Engineering, and Biomedical Physics, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America – name: Indiana University, United States of America – name: 10 The Reason Project, Santa Monica, California, United States of America – name: 6 Departments of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, California, United States of America |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Sam surname: Harris fullname: Harris, Sam – sequence: 2 givenname: Jonas T. surname: Kaplan fullname: Kaplan, Jonas T. – sequence: 3 givenname: Ashley surname: Curiel fullname: Curiel, Ashley – sequence: 4 givenname: Susan Y. surname: Bookheimer fullname: Bookheimer, Susan Y. – sequence: 5 givenname: Marco surname: Iacoboni fullname: Iacoboni, Marco – sequence: 6 givenname: Mark S. surname: Cohen fullname: Cohen, Mark S. |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19794914$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNk2tr2zAUhs3oWC_bPxibYdCxD8l0tax-GLRhl0Bpoev2VRzLcqKgWJlkj-7fT17SkJRRikASR8_7inOkc5wdtL41WfYaozGmAn9c-D604MarFB4jhAQR5Fl2hCUlo4IgerCzP8yOY1wgxGlZFC-yQyyFZBKzo-zsdm7yK9MHcPnEh2AcdCbmvslvjLMz6_uYQ1vnV74N28BF2pnmZfa8ARfNq816kv348vl28m10ef11Ojm_HGkhcTeSiAJoIwuMoQScBkhcVZRUFdGyLjjTGHNMyxK45KA1khokDBOWkA5Osrdr35XzUW3SjgoTSSjCjJFETNdE7WGhVsEuIfxRHqz6F_BhpiB0VjujqhrxhkCFaG0YN1KKhhFtmOQlq6CWyevT5ra-Wppam7ZLtdkz3T9p7VzN_G9FBCsFLpPB-41B8L96Ezu1tFEb56A1qXhKUFrIkguRyNNHSYJZQZlATwE5o3xwfPcA_H-5xmtqBqkitm18SkSnUZul1ek7NTbFz5kgJadC0iT4sCdITGfuuhn0Marp95uns9c_99nTHXZuwHXz6F3fWd_GffDN7qNsX-P-HyfgbA3o4GMMplHadjD4pNSsUxipoWnuy6GGplGbpkli9kC89X9M9he17RpR |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_22299_6 crossref_primary_10_1177_2514183X17714097 crossref_primary_10_7202_1019197ar crossref_primary_10_1007_s10943_017_0433_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2014_04_011 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01441 crossref_primary_10_1057_s41292_016_0019_y crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14561 crossref_primary_10_1177_0735275118759697 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compbiomed_2023_107604 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neures_2014_10_019 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01964 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_yebeh_2020_107326 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_1062535 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2017_10_003 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jpsychires_2021_03_030 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_socscimed_2021_114580 crossref_primary_10_1080_09515089_2015_1110852 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_knosys_2020_105932 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0124596 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40894_020_00145_y crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_84445_6 crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_66043 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0273174 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2016_1257437 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nrl_2016_04_010 crossref_primary_10_3390_rel11090468 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40750_020_00131_7 crossref_primary_10_1038_news_2010_66 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_018_0563_y crossref_primary_10_1080_13674676_2012_735226 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2011_641228 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0149989 crossref_primary_10_3390_rel12111032 crossref_primary_10_12688_f1000research_9773_1 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2016_12_031 crossref_primary_10_12688_f1000research_9773_2 crossref_primary_10_1186_2211_1522_1_3 crossref_primary_10_1080_2153599X_2011_647854 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10943_013_9768_0 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci4020396 crossref_primary_10_1177_0963721419898183 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11757_011_0134_3 crossref_primary_10_1159_000485221 crossref_primary_10_3390_e12092045 crossref_primary_10_1038_srep39589 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnbeh_2020_548856 crossref_primary_10_1002_brb3_1209 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_envres_2014_10_001 crossref_primary_10_3389_fsoc_2017_00022 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2025_1557796 crossref_primary_10_3390_rel15030290 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2011_06_012 crossref_primary_10_1080_13869795_2017_1287294 crossref_primary_10_1080_2153599X_2022_2143398 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2010_11_073 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2014_01_023 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nrleng_2016_04_012 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_rcp_2023_01_006 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_encep_2013_12_005 crossref_primary_10_3390_rel12010023 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1174 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2015_02_016 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40806_016_0059_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_mehy_2015_04_004 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11042_023_15588_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pscychresns_2014_03_010 |
Cites_doi | 10.1038/nrn2277 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00388-4 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.068 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005 10.1196/annals.1440.011 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 10.1002/ana.21301 10.1126/science.1093535 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1965 10.1089/acm.2007.0675 10.1093/scan/nsn050 10.1371/journal.pone.0001897 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004 10.2466/pms.2003.97.2.625 10.1162/089892904322926791 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295 10.1126/science.280.5364.747 10.1073/pnas.0811717106 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.005 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00145-8 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6 10.1073/pnas.0407401101 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 10.1162/08989290260138672 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02305.x 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01527.x 10.1006/cogp.2001.0749 10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.009 10.1126/science.1093065 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001 10.1002/ana.20742 10.1016/S0925-4927(01)00074-9 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.020 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00031-7 10.1073/pnas.0606552104 10.1038/82959 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.08.014 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1586 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01419-9 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00046-0 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.07.001 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2 10.1093/scan/nsn020 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.005 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | COPYRIGHT 2009 Public Library of Science 2009 Harris et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. Harris et al. 2009 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: COPYRIGHT 2009 Public Library of Science – notice: 2009 Harris et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. – notice: Harris et al. 2009 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM IOV ISR 3V. 7QG 7QL 7QO 7RV 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TG 7TM 7U9 7X2 7X7 7XB 88E 8AO 8C1 8FD 8FE 8FG 8FH 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABJCF ABUWG AEUYN AFKRA ARAPS ATCPS AZQEC BBNVY BENPR BGLVJ BHPHI C1K CCPQU D1I DWQXO FR3 FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ H94 HCIFZ K9. KB. KB0 KL. L6V LK8 M0K M0S M1P M7N M7P M7S NAPCQ P5Z P62 P64 PATMY PDBOC PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PJZUB PKEHL PPXIY PQEST PQGLB PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PTHSS PYCSY RC3 7TK 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.1371/journal.pone.0007272 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Gale in Context: Opposing Viewpoints Gale in Context: Science ProQuest Central (Corporate) Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Biotechnology Research Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Database Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Immunology Abstracts Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts Virology and AIDS Abstracts Agricultural Science Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Pharma Collection Public Health Database Technology Research Database ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Technology Collection ProQuest Natural Science Collection Hospital Premium Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) Materials Science & Engineering Collection ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest One Sustainability (subscription) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection ProQuest Central Essentials Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central Technology Collection Natural Science Collection Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest One ProQuest Materials Science Collection ProQuest Central Korea Engineering Research Database Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Materials Science Database Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition) Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic ProQuest Engineering Collection Biological Sciences Agricultural Science Database Health & Medical Collection (Alumni) Medical Database Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Biological Science Database ProQuest Engineering Database Nursing & Allied Health Premium Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Science Database Materials Science Collection ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China Engineering collection Environmental Science Collection Genetics Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Agricultural Science Database Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection ProQuest Central Essentials Nucleic Acids Abstracts SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest Central China Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences ProQuest One Sustainability Health Research Premium Collection Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts Natural Science Collection Health & Medical Research Collection Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) Engineering Collection Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection Engineering Database Virology and AIDS Abstracts ProQuest Biological Science Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition Agricultural Science Collection ProQuest Hospital Collection ProQuest Technology Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) Biological Science Database Ecology Abstracts ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Science Collection Entomology Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Premium ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition Environmental Science Database ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source (Alumni) Engineering Research Database ProQuest One Academic Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) Technology Collection Technology Research Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) Materials Science Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Pharma Collection ProQuest Central ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection Genetics Abstracts ProQuest Engineering Collection Biotechnology Research Abstracts Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Materials Science Database ProQuest Materials Science Collection ProQuest Public Health ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source ProQuest SciTech Collection Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database ProQuest Medical Library Animal Behavior Abstracts Materials Science & Engineering Collection Immunology Abstracts ProQuest Central (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts Agricultural Science Database Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Open Access Full Text url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 4 dbid: 8FG name: ProQuest Technology Collection url: https://search.proquest.com/technologycollection1 sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Sciences (General) Religion |
DocumentTitleAlternate | Belief |
EISSN | 1932-6203 |
EndPage | e7272 |
ExternalDocumentID | 1292301442 oai_doaj_org_article_bd05f2ab03de45e997f42ce49584bad9 PMC2748718 2898411151 A472853793 19794914 10_1371_journal_pone_0007272 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GeographicLocations | Los Angeles California United States--US California |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: Los Angeles California – name: United States--US – name: California |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NCRR NIH HHS grantid: P41 RR013642 – fundername: NCRR NIH HHS grantid: RR13642 – fundername: NIDA NIH HHS grantid: R90 DA023422 – fundername: NCRR NIH HHS grantid: RR00865 – fundername: NCRR NIH HHS grantid: C06 RR012169 – fundername: NCRR NIH HHS grantid: M01 RR000865 |
GroupedDBID | --- 123 29O 2WC 53G 5VS 7RV 7X2 7X7 7XC 88E 8AO 8C1 8CJ 8FE 8FG 8FH 8FI 8FJ A8Z AAFWJ AAUCC AAWOE AAYXX ABDBF ABIVO ABJCF ABUWG ACGFO ACIHN ACIWK ACPRK ACUHS ADBBV ADRAZ AEAQA AENEX AEUYN AFKRA AFPKN AFRAH AHMBA ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS APEBS ARAPS ATCPS BAWUL BBNVY BCNDV BENPR BGLVJ BHPHI BKEYQ BPHCQ BVXVI BWKFM CCPQU CITATION CS3 D1I D1J D1K DIK DU5 E3Z EAP EAS EBD EMOBN ESX EX3 F5P FPL FYUFA GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HCIFZ HH5 HMCUK HYE IAO IEA IGS IHR IHW INH INR IOV IPY ISE ISR ITC K6- KB. KQ8 L6V LK5 LK8 M0K M1P M48 M7P M7R M7S M~E NAPCQ O5R O5S OK1 OVT P2P P62 PATMY PDBOC PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PTHSS PYCSY RNS RPM SV3 TR2 UKHRP WOQ WOW ~02 ~KM CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PJZUB PPXIY PQGLB BBORY PMFND 3V. 7QG 7QL 7QO 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TG 7TM 7U9 7XB 8FD 8FK AZQEC C1K DWQXO FR3 GNUQQ H94 K9. KL. M7N P64 PKEHL PQEST PQUKI PRINS RC3 7TK 7X8 5PM PUEGO - 02 AAPBV ABPTK ADACO BBAFP KM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c791t-903aace9611a8a1a1aa91bb32bb2c9d654c1151388a595acc09ca9a9ca919a513 |
IEDL.DBID | M48 |
ISSN | 1932-6203 |
IngestDate | Fri Nov 26 17:13:33 EST 2021 Wed Aug 27 00:45:24 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 18:23:50 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 20:37:15 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 23:58:49 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 10:57:16 EDT 2025 Sat Aug 16 21:21:51 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 10 21:31:25 EDT 2025 Fri Jun 27 03:40:30 EDT 2025 Fri Jun 27 04:02:25 EDT 2025 Thu May 22 21:23:11 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:06:37 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:50:36 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:55:14 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 10 |
Language | English |
License | This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited. Creative Commons Attribution License |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c791t-903aace9611a8a1a1aa91bb32bb2c9d654c1151388a595acc09ca9a9ca919a513 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Conceived and designed the experiments: SH JTK MI MSC. Performed the experiments: JTK. Analyzed the data: SH JTK MI MSC. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MI MSC. Wrote the paper: SH JTK. Performed all subject recruitment, telephone screenings, and psychometric assessments prior to scanning: AC. Supervised our psychological assessment procedures and consulted on subject exclusions: SB. Gave extensive notes on the manuscript: MSC MI. |
OpenAccessLink | http://journals.scholarsportal.info/openUrl.xqy?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0007272 |
PMID | 19794914 |
PQID | 1292301442 |
PQPubID | 23462 |
PageCount | e7272 |
ParticipantIDs | plos_journals_1292301442 doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_bd05f2ab03de45e997f42ce49584bad9 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2748718 proquest_miscellaneous_733698577 proquest_miscellaneous_21463470 proquest_miscellaneous_21454357 proquest_journals_1292301442 gale_infotracacademiconefile_A472853793 gale_incontextgauss_ISR_A472853793 gale_incontextgauss_IOV_A472853793 gale_healthsolutions_A472853793 pubmed_primary_19794914 crossref_citationtrail_10_1371_journal_pone_0007272 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0007272 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2009-10-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2009-10-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 10 year: 2009 text: 2009-10-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: San Francisco – name: San Francisco, USA |
PublicationTitle | PloS one |
PublicationTitleAlternate | PLoS One |
PublicationYear | 2009 |
Publisher | Public Library of Science Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
Publisher_xml | – name: Public Library of Science – name: Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
References | M Inzlicht (ref41) 2009; 20 EM Evans (ref48) 2001; 42 M Jenkinson (ref56) 2002; 17 JLR Andersson (ref60) 2007 P Boyer (ref31) 2001 J O'Doherty (ref10) 2003; 41 DC Dennett (ref33) 2006 U Schjoedt (ref26) 2009; 4 MW Anastasi (ref29) 2008; 14 JL Barrett (ref50) 2000; 4 CS Carter (ref40) 1998; 280 A Bechara (ref7) 2000; 10 P Zuckerman (ref16) 2008 S Freud (ref13) 1930 M Weber (ref15) 1922 WM Kelley (ref53) 2002; 14 AB Newberg (ref30) 2006; 148 G Northoff (ref3) 2006; 31 J Hornak (ref8) 2004; 16 TD Wager (ref34) 2004; 303 B Wicker (ref36) 2003; 40 K Matsumoto (ref11) 2004; 14 J O'Doherty (ref54) 2001; 4 K Izuma (ref38) 2008; 58 D Kapogiannis (ref2) 2009; 106 RA Diana (ref42) 2007; 11 M Brooks (ref47) 2009 S Freud (ref14) 1927 A Lutz (ref21) 2008; 12 ET Rolls (ref9) 2008; 41 K Patterson (ref43) 2007; 8 P Boyer (ref45) 2003; 7 MW Woolrich (ref58) 2001; 14 P Norris (ref17) 2004 A Lutz (ref23) 2004; 101 A Newberg (ref28) 2003; 97 K Marx (ref12) 1843 F Schneider (ref6) 2008; 157 BM Hood (ref49) 2009 J Borg (ref19) 2003; 160 NP Azari (ref25) 2001; 13 ME Raichle (ref52) 2001; 98 RL Buckner (ref44) 2008; 1124 FH Previc (ref18) 2006; 15 T Singer (ref35) 2004; 303 A Lutz (ref20) 2008; 3 JM Moran (ref5) 2006; 18 JA Brefczynski-Lewis (ref22) 2007; 104 E Durkheim (ref32) 2001 S Harris (ref1) 2008; 63 RA Poldrack (ref51) 2006; 10 U Schjoedt (ref27) 2008; 443 TD Wager (ref55) 2003; 18 M Jenkinson (ref57) 2001; 5 A Newberg (ref24) 2001; 106 A D'Argembeau (ref4) 2008; 3 JLR Andersson (ref59) 2007 JP Royet (ref37) 2003; 20 O Monchi (ref39) 2006; 59 P Bloom (ref46) 2004 16466680 - Neuroimage. 2006 May 15;31(1):440-57 11135651 - Nat Neurosci. 2001 Jan;4(1):95-102 18307389 - J Altern Complement Med. 2008 Mar;14(2):163-5 18468458 - Neuroimage. 2008 Jul 15;41(4):1504-13 11306250 - Psychiatry Res. 2001 Apr 10;106(2):113-22 12167262 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 Jul 1;14(5):785-94 16439158 - Conscious Cogn. 2006 Sep;15(3):500-39 16989558 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2006 Sep;18(9):1586-94 12459213 - Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(2):147-55 18400922 - Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008 Mar;1124:1-38 18329323 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Apr;12(4):163-9 12595184 - Neuroimage. 2003 Feb;18(2):293-309 10637620 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2000 Jan;4(1):29-34 17707683 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2007 Sep;11(9):379-86 15534199 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 16;101(46):16369-73 18365029 - PLoS One. 2008;3(3):e1897 11209064 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001 Jan 16;98(2):676-82 14568446 - Neuroimage. 2003 Oct;20(2):713-28 14620252 - Percept Mot Skills. 2003 Oct;97(2):625-30 14976306 - Science. 2004 Feb 20;303(5661):1162-7 11305883 - Cogn Psychol. 2001 May;42(3):217-66 17046214 - Psychiatry Res. 2006 Nov 22;148(1):67-71 16406760 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 Feb;10(2):59-63 15082322 - Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004 Apr;14(2):178-85 11516708 - Med Image Anal. 2001 Jun;5(2):143-56 12377157 - Neuroimage. 2002 Oct;17(2):825-41 18793699 - Neuroscience. 2008 Nov 11;157(1):120-31 19273839 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 Mar 24;106(12):4876-81 10731224 - Cereb Cortex. 2000 Mar;10(3):295-307 17596341 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007 Jul 3;104(27):11483-8 19246473 - Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009 Jun;4(2):199-207 19015116 - Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008 Sep;3(3):244-52 12639693 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Mar;7(3):119-124 18682275 - Neurosci Lett. 2008 Oct 10;443(3):165-8 14642287 - Neuron. 2003 Oct 30;40(3):655-64 18026167 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007 Dec;8(12):976-87 18072236 - Ann Neurol. 2008 Feb;63(2):141-7 14976305 - Science. 2004 Feb 20;303(5661):1157-62 11707093 - Neuroimage. 2001 Dec;14(6):1370-86 16437582 - Ann Neurol. 2006 Feb;59(2):257-64 19291205 - Psychol Sci. 2009 Mar;20(3):385-92 18439412 - Neuron. 2008 Apr 24;58(2):284-94 14594742 - Am J Psychiatry. 2003 Nov;160(11):1965-9 11328359 - Eur J Neurosci. 2001 Apr;13(8):1649-52 9563953 - Science. 1998 May 1;280(5364):747-9 15072681 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2004 Apr;16(3):463-78 |
References_xml | – volume: 8 start-page: 976 year: 2007 ident: ref43 article-title: Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. publication-title: Nat Rev Neurosci doi: 10.1038/nrn2277 – start-page: xv year: 2004 ident: ref46 article-title: Descartes' baby: how the science of child development explains what makes us human – volume: 20 start-page: 713 year: 2003 ident: ref37 article-title: fMRI of emotional responses to odors: influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender. publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00388-4 – volume: 443 start-page: 165 year: 2008 ident: ref27 article-title: Rewarding prayers. publication-title: Neurosci Lett doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.068 – volume: 12 start-page: 163 year: 2008 ident: ref21 article-title: Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005 – volume: 1124 start-page: 1 year: 2008 ident: ref44 article-title: The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. publication-title: Ann N Y Acad Sci doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011 – volume: 98 start-page: 676 year: 2001 ident: ref52 article-title: A default mode of brain function. publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 – volume: 63 start-page: 141 year: 2008 ident: ref1 article-title: Functional neuroimaging of belief, disbelief, and uncertainty. publication-title: Ann Neurol doi: 10.1002/ana.21301 – volume: 303 start-page: 1157 year: 2004 ident: ref35 article-title: Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.1093535 – volume: 160 start-page: 1965 year: 2003 ident: ref19 article-title: The serotonin system and spiritual experiences. publication-title: Am J Psychiatry doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1965 – volume: 14 start-page: 163 year: 2008 ident: ref29 article-title: A preliminary study of the acute effects of religious ritual on anxiety. publication-title: J Altern Complement Med doi: 10.1089/acm.2007.0675 – volume: 4 start-page: 199 year: 2009 ident: ref26 article-title: Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. publication-title: Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn050 – volume: 3 start-page: e1897 year: 2008 ident: ref20 article-title: Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: effects of meditative expertise. publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001897 – volume: 10 start-page: 59 year: 2006 ident: ref51 article-title: Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004 – year: 2008 ident: ref16 article-title: Society Without God – volume: 97 start-page: 625 year: 2003 ident: ref28 article-title: Cerebral blood flow during meditative prayer: preliminary findings and methodological issues. publication-title: Percept Mot Skills doi: 10.2466/pms.2003.97.2.625 – start-page: v year: 1930 ident: ref13 article-title: Civilization and its discontents – volume: 16 start-page: 463 year: 2004 ident: ref8 article-title: Reward-related reversal learning after surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in humans. publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci doi: 10.1162/089892904322926791 – volume: 10 start-page: 295 year: 2000 ident: ref7 article-title: Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. publication-title: Cereb Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295 – start-page: xli year: 2001 ident: ref32 article-title: The elementary forms of religious life – volume: 280 start-page: 747 year: 1998 ident: ref40 article-title: Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.280.5364.747 – year: 2009 ident: ref47 article-title: Born believers: How your brain creates God. publication-title: New Scientist – volume: 106 start-page: 4876 year: 2009 ident: ref2 article-title: Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief. publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811717106 – volume: 41 start-page: 1504 year: 2008 ident: ref9 article-title: Warm pleasant feelings in the brain. publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.005 – volume: 41 start-page: 147 year: 2003 ident: ref10 article-title: Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00145-8 – year: 2009 ident: ref49 article-title: Supersense: Why we believe in the unbelievable – volume: 5 start-page: 143 year: 2001 ident: ref57 article-title: A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. publication-title: Med Image Anal doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6 – year: 1927 ident: ref14 article-title: The future of an illusion – volume: 101 start-page: 16369 year: 2004 ident: ref23 article-title: Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407401101 – start-page: lxxvii year: 1922 ident: ref15 article-title: The sociology of religion – volume: 17 start-page: 825 year: 2002 ident: ref56 article-title: Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 – volume: 14 start-page: 785 year: 2002 ident: ref53 article-title: Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci doi: 10.1162/08989290260138672 – volume: 14 start-page: 1370 year: 2001 ident: ref58 article-title: Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931 – volume: 20 start-page: 385 year: 2009 ident: ref41 article-title: Neural markers of religious conviction. publication-title: Psychol Sci doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02305.x – volume: 31 start-page: 440 year: 2006 ident: ref3 article-title: Self-referential processing in our brain–a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002 – volume: 13 start-page: 1649 year: 2001 ident: ref25 article-title: Neural correlates of religious experience. publication-title: Eur J Neurosci doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01527.x – volume: 42 start-page: 217 year: 2001 ident: ref48 article-title: Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief systems: creation versus evolution. publication-title: Cogn Psychol doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0749 – volume: 15 start-page: 500 year: 2006 ident: ref18 article-title: The role of the extrapersonal brain systems in religious activity. publication-title: Conscious Cogn doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.009 – start-page: xiv year: 2006 ident: ref33 article-title: Breaking the spell: religion as a natural phenomenon – volume: 303 start-page: 1162 year: 2004 ident: ref34 article-title: Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.1093065 – volume: 11 start-page: 379 year: 2007 ident: ref42 article-title: Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001 – start-page: xv year: 2004 ident: ref17 article-title: Sacred and secular : religion and politics worldwide – volume: 59 start-page: 257 year: 2006 ident: ref39 article-title: Functional role of the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of actions. publication-title: Ann Neurol doi: 10.1002/ana.20742 – year: 2007 ident: ref60 article-title: Non-linear optimisation. publication-title: FMRIB technical report TR07JA1 – volume: 106 start-page: 113 year: 2001 ident: ref24 article-title: The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. publication-title: Psychiatry Res doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(01)00074-9 – volume: 58 start-page: 284 year: 2008 ident: ref38 article-title: Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. publication-title: Neuron doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.020 – volume: 7 start-page: 119 year: 2003 ident: ref45 article-title: Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00031-7 – volume: 104 start-page: 11483 year: 2007 ident: ref22 article-title: Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.0606552104 – volume: 4 start-page: 95 year: 2001 ident: ref54 article-title: Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. publication-title: Nat Neurosci doi: 10.1038/82959 – volume: 157 start-page: 120 year: 2008 ident: ref6 article-title: The resting brain and our self: self-relatedness modulates resting state neural activity in cortical midline structures. publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.08.014 – volume: 18 start-page: 1586 year: 2006 ident: ref5 article-title: Neuroanatomical evidence for distinct cognitive and affective components of self. publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1586 – volume: 4 start-page: 29 year: 2000 ident: ref50 article-title: Exploring the natural foundations of religion. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01419-9 – year: 1843 ident: ref12 article-title: Critique of Hegel's Philosophy of Right O'Malley AJaJ, translator; – year: 2001 ident: ref31 article-title: Religion explained: The evolutionary orgins of religious thought – volume: 18 start-page: 293 year: 2003 ident: ref55 article-title: Optimization of experimental design in fMRI: a general framework using a genetic algorithm. publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00046-0 – volume: 148 start-page: 67 year: 2006 ident: ref30 article-title: The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during glossolalia: a preliminary SPECT study. publication-title: Psychiatry Res doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.07.001 – volume: 40 start-page: 655 year: 2003 ident: ref36 article-title: Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. publication-title: Neuron doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2 – year: 2007 ident: ref59 article-title: Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation. publication-title: FMRIB technical report TR07JA2 – volume: 3 start-page: 244 year: 2008 ident: ref4 article-title: Self-reflection across time: cortical midline structures differentiate between present and past selves. publication-title: Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn020 – volume: 14 start-page: 178 year: 2004 ident: ref11 article-title: The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in achieving goals. publication-title: Curr Opin Neurobiol doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.005 – reference: 18439412 - Neuron. 2008 Apr 24;58(2):284-94 – reference: 18307389 - J Altern Complement Med. 2008 Mar;14(2):163-5 – reference: 18793699 - Neuroscience. 2008 Nov 11;157(1):120-31 – reference: 11707093 - Neuroimage. 2001 Dec;14(6):1370-86 – reference: 16466680 - Neuroimage. 2006 May 15;31(1):440-57 – reference: 16437582 - Ann Neurol. 2006 Feb;59(2):257-64 – reference: 14642287 - Neuron. 2003 Oct 30;40(3):655-64 – reference: 10731224 - Cereb Cortex. 2000 Mar;10(3):295-307 – reference: 15082322 - Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004 Apr;14(2):178-85 – reference: 18468458 - Neuroimage. 2008 Jul 15;41(4):1504-13 – reference: 16406760 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 Feb;10(2):59-63 – reference: 17596341 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007 Jul 3;104(27):11483-8 – reference: 16989558 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2006 Sep;18(9):1586-94 – reference: 15072681 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2004 Apr;16(3):463-78 – reference: 11516708 - Med Image Anal. 2001 Jun;5(2):143-56 – reference: 18026167 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007 Dec;8(12):976-87 – reference: 11135651 - Nat Neurosci. 2001 Jan;4(1):95-102 – reference: 12595184 - Neuroimage. 2003 Feb;18(2):293-309 – reference: 12639693 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Mar;7(3):119-124 – reference: 10637620 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2000 Jan;4(1):29-34 – reference: 18365029 - PLoS One. 2008;3(3):e1897 – reference: 12377157 - Neuroimage. 2002 Oct;17(2):825-41 – reference: 14568446 - Neuroimage. 2003 Oct;20(2):713-28 – reference: 17707683 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2007 Sep;11(9):379-86 – reference: 14976305 - Science. 2004 Feb 20;303(5661):1157-62 – reference: 11328359 - Eur J Neurosci. 2001 Apr;13(8):1649-52 – reference: 18072236 - Ann Neurol. 2008 Feb;63(2):141-7 – reference: 14976306 - Science. 2004 Feb 20;303(5661):1162-7 – reference: 16439158 - Conscious Cogn. 2006 Sep;15(3):500-39 – reference: 18682275 - Neurosci Lett. 2008 Oct 10;443(3):165-8 – reference: 18400922 - Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008 Mar;1124:1-38 – reference: 12459213 - Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(2):147-55 – reference: 19291205 - Psychol Sci. 2009 Mar;20(3):385-92 – reference: 15534199 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 16;101(46):16369-73 – reference: 19015116 - Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008 Sep;3(3):244-52 – reference: 18329323 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Apr;12(4):163-9 – reference: 17046214 - Psychiatry Res. 2006 Nov 22;148(1):67-71 – reference: 11209064 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001 Jan 16;98(2):676-82 – reference: 14594742 - Am J Psychiatry. 2003 Nov;160(11):1965-9 – reference: 14620252 - Percept Mot Skills. 2003 Oct;97(2):625-30 – reference: 19246473 - Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009 Jun;4(2):199-207 – reference: 11306250 - Psychiatry Res. 2001 Apr 10;106(2):113-22 – reference: 19273839 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 Mar 24;106(12):4876-81 – reference: 12167262 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 Jul 1;14(5):785-94 – reference: 9563953 - Science. 1998 May 1;280(5364):747-9 – reference: 11305883 - Cogn Psychol. 2001 May;42(3):217-66 |
SSID | ssj0053866 |
Score | 2.3482118 |
Snippet | While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the level of the... Background While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the... Background While religious faith remains one of the most significant features of human life, little is known about its relationship to ordinary belief at the... |
SourceID | plos doaj pubmedcentral proquest gale pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | e7272 |
SubjectTerms | 21st century Adolescent Adult Behavior Brain Brain - anatomy & histology Brain - physiology Brain Mapping Brain research Child development Christianity Cognition Cognition & reasoning Cognition - physiology Cognitive ability Cortex (frontal) Cortex (parietal) Cortex (temporal) Departments Ecstasy Emotions - physiology Female Functional magnetic resonance imaging Genetic algorithms Humans Judgment - physiology Judgments Lobes Magnetic resonance Magnetic resonance imaging Magnetic Resonance Imaging - methods Male Medical imaging Memory Neural Pathways - anatomy & histology Neural Pathways - physiology Neuroimaging Neurology Neuropsychological Tests Neuroscience Neuroscience/Cognitive Neuroscience Neuroscience/Psychology Neuroscience/Theoretical Neuroscience Neurosciences Prefrontal cortex Psychiatry Public opinion Registration Reinforcement Religion Religious aspects Spirituality Studies Temporal lobe Trends |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3db9MwELdQn3hBjK8FBosQEvAQFid2bPM2ENNAAiRgaG-WvzKQqqRa2v-fO8fNFjQYD6hSVdnXpr4v38l3PxPyzEuugpSu8LAZFcybsoARXzQIZg5bnGoCNid__NQcn7APp_z00lVfWBM2wgOPjDuwvuRtZWxZ-8B4UEq0rHIB4nrJrPGxdQ8es02mRh8MVtw0qVGuFvQgyeXVqu8CIhbi6eNsI4p4_ZNXXqyW_XBVyPl75eSlrejoNrmVYsj8cPzvO-RG6O6QnWSlQ_4iQUm_vEtegxbkCFkJ5A7v4VhiaJn3bX4-liJvhtx0Pu_67mLAwqfQ3iMnR---vT0u0m0JhROKrgtV1sa4oBpKjTQUXkZRa-vK2sop33DmIPqjtZSGK26cK5UzyuAbVQYm7pMFPC3sklxWDvIq-CmQJHMlMy0PAlJZLkTrbVtlpN6yTrsEJY43Wix1PB8TkFKMnNDIcJ0YnpFi-tZqhNK4hv4NSmWiRSDsOADqoZN66OvUIyP7KFM9dpVO5qwPmaggUgHvlJGnkQLBMDqstjkzm2HQ7z9__weir19mRM8TUdsDO5xJHQ6wJgTZmlHuooptlz1oiLqqmNzCqve2anf19P40DY4AT3dMF0A5NELOQ-wr_krR1EyUGcn_QIHYmEqCmDPyYFT0C0kpsF5FWUbEzARm4pnPdD9_RLzySkBWTOXD_yHPR-RmPM-L5ZR7ZLE-34THEBau7ZPoAX4BxHph_g priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – databaseName: ProQuest Technology Collection dbid: 8FG link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3_a9QwFH_oCeIv4uaX1U1XRFB_qGvatEn8RbbhOQUnqJP9FtIkncLR3ta7_9_3eunNypxSOErycpD3Pc3LJwDPnSyUl9ImDoNRwp1JE2xxSUlg5hjiVOnpcPKn4_LohH88LU7DB7culFUOPrF31K619I18D-NS1qf_2dv5eUK3RtHuarhC4ybcYhhpqKRLTt8PnhhtuSzDcblcsL0gndfztvGEW0h7kKNw1KP2r33zZD5ru6sSzz_rJ38LSNN7cDdkkvH-SvQbcMM3m3B7KDHehI1gtl38MmBLv7oPb1AtYgLkwJGHdDHHjHLNuK3jMHDZxaZx8XGLajU0HOCbrx_AyfTdt8OjJFyfkFih2CJRaW6M9apkzEjD8DGKVVWeVVVmlSsLbjEdZLmUplCFsTZV1ihDP0wZ7HgIkwZZtQWxzCwutPCvULTcptzUhRe4ti2EqF1VZxHkAxe1DdjidMXFTPcbZgLXGCumaOK9DryPIFmPmq-wNf5Bf0ACWtMSMnbf0F6c6WBounJpUWemSnPneeGVEjXPrMd1oOSVcSqCXRKvXh0zXdu33uciw9QF3VUEz3oKQsdoqPzmzCy7Tn_4_P0_iL5-GRG9CER1i-ywJhx5wDkR6taIcou0bZh2py_VPYKdQQOv7t5dd6NnoO0e03hUDk0Y9JgMi2spypyLNIL4LxQElqkkijmCRyudv5SUQnNWjEcgRtYwEs-4p_n5owcwzwQuk5l8fP3MtuFOv3XXV07uwGRxsfRPMANcVE97M_8FRu5bLA priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest |
Title | The Neural Correlates of Religious and Nonreligious Belief |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19794914 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1292301442 https://www.proquest.com/docview/21454357 https://www.proquest.com/docview/21463470 https://www.proquest.com/docview/733698577 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC2748718 https://doaj.org/article/bd05f2ab03de45e997f42ce49584bad9 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007272 |
Volume | 4 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3db9MwELe27oUXxPhaYHQRQgIeMsWJE9tICK1Vy0BaQYOivkWO42xIVdI1rTRe-Nu5S51AUMdQJCuyz1F9H_Zdbf-OkBeZiKQRQnsZLEYey5TvQU3mxQhmDkucjA1eTj6bxKdT9nEWzXZIk7PVMrDaGtphPqnpcn58ffXjHRj82zprA6dNp-NFWRjEI8S9xV2yB2sTx5wGZ6zdVwDrjmN7ge6mnp0Fqsbxb2fr3mJeVttc0b9PVP6xRI3vkbvWt3RPNsqwT3ZMcZ_sW-ut3FcWYvr1A_IGtMNFXA4gH2J-jjm6nG6Zu_aI8rpyVZG5kxK0q6kYwJvJH5LpePR1eOrZLAqe5pKuPOmHSmkjY0qVUBQeJWmahkGaBlpmccQ0eIU0FEJFMlJa-1IrqbCgUkHDI9IrgD8HxBWBhngLPgUSZtpnKo8MhxA34jzP0jxwSNiwLtEWYhwzXcyTet-MQ6ix4USCDE8swx3itb0WG4iNW-gHKJWWFgGy64pyeZFYe0vSzI_yQKV-mBkWGSl5zgJtIBwULFWZdMgRyjTZ3DZtzTw5YTwADwZmLYc8rykQJKPAUzgXal1VyYdP3_6D6Mt5h-ilJcpLYIdW9uYDjAnBtzqUB6hizbCrBLyxoA56YdSHjdptbz5qm2GCwF0fVRhQjgSh6MEn5v-kiEPGfYe4N1AgZqYUIGaHPN4o-m9JSbBqSZlDeMcEOuLpthTfL2sc84BDtEzFk1t_-lNyp97Eq89QHpLeark2z8AXXKV9sstnHEoxpFiO3_fJ3mA0-Xzer_9d6dfmj-XP0S_u82W5 |
linkProvider | Scholars Portal |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3db9MwED-NIgEviI2PBQaNEAh4CMuHE8dICG2D0rKtSLChvRnHcQZSlZSlFeKf4m_kLnE6isbgZapUVfYlku_O91H7fgfwKE9jYdJUezk6I4_lyvdwJPcSAjNHFycSQ8XJ--NkeMjeHcVHK_Czq4Wha5WdTWwMdV5p-o98E_1S2IT_4avpN4-6RtHpatdCo1WLXfPjO6Zs9cvRa5Tv4zAcvDnYGXq2q4CnuQhmnvAjpbQRSRCoVAX4USLIsijMslCLPImZxigpiNJUxSJWWvtCK6HoKxAKJ_C9l-Ayi9CTU2X64G1n-dF2JIktz4t4sGm14fm0Kg3hJNKZ55L7a7oELHxBbzqp6rMC3T_va_7mAAc34LqNXN2tVtVWYcWUa3Clu9K8BqvWTNTuU4tl_ewmvEA1dAkABJ_coUYgE4pt3apw7YPz2lVl7o4rVONuYBt_meIWHF4IY29Dr0RWrYObhhoTO3wVqhLTPlNFbDjm0jHnRZ4VoQNRx0WpLZY5tdSYyOaAjmNO0zJFEu-l5b0D3uKpaYvl8Q_6bRLQgpaQuJuB6uRY2o0ts9yPi1BlfpQbFhsheMFCbTDvTFmmcuFAn8Qr27LWhT2RW4yHGCqheXTgYUNBaBwlXfc5VvO6lqP3n_6D6OOHJaInlqiokB1a2RILXBOhfC1RrpO2dcuu5en2cmCj08Czp_uLabREdLykSoPKIQnzHoNvfi5FEjHuO-D-hYLAOUWKYnbgTqvzp5ISaD5EwBzgS7thSTzLM-XXLw1gesgxLQ_Su-evrA9Xhwf7e3JvNN69B9eaY8Pm1uYG9GYnc3Mfo89Z9qDZ8i58vmgb8wtL8Jhi |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3db9MwELdGkSZeEBsfCwwaIRDwEJo4H7aRENo6qpVBQcDQ3ozjOAOpSsrSCvGv8ddxlzgZQWPwMlWqKvsSyXfn-6jPvyPkQcZjYTjXXgbOyIsy5XswknkJgpmDixOJwcvJb2bJ_mH06ig-WiM_27swWFbZ2sTaUGelxv_IR-CXaB3-01FuyyLe7U1eLL552EEKT1rbdhqNihyYH98hfaueT_dA1g8pnbz8ON73bIcBTzMRLD3hh0ppI5IgUFwF8FEiSNOQpinVIkviSEPEFIScq1jESmtfaCUUfgVCwQS89xK5zELGcY_xcVdeAnYkSexVvZAFI6sZTxdlYRAzEc8_e66w7hjQ-YXBYl5WZwW9f9Zu_uYMJ9fIVRvFujuN2m2QNVNskvW2vHmTbFiTUbmPLa71k-vkGaiki2Ag8OQYm4LMMc51y9y1D64qVxWZOytBpduBXfhl8hvk8EIYe5MMCmDVFnE51ZDkwatArSLtRyqPDYO8OmYsz9KcOiRsuSi1xTXH9hpzWR_WMchvGqZI5L20vHeI1z21aHA9_kG_iwLqaBGVux4oT46l3eQyzfw4pyr1w8xEsRGC5RHVBnJQHqUqEw4Zonhlc8W1sy1yJ2IUwiYwlQ65X1MgMkeBOn6sVlUlp28__QfRh_c9okeWKC-BHVrZ6xawJkT86lFuoba1y67k6VZzyHargWdPD7tpsEp41KQKA8ohEf8eAnF2LkUSRsx3iPsXCgTqFBzE7JBbjc6fSkqAKRFB5BDW2w098fRniq9favB0yiBFD_jt81c2JOtgXeTr6ezgDrlSnyDWBZzbZLA8WZm7EIgu03v1jnfJ54s2Mb8A62CcYw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+Neural+Correlates+of+Religious+and+Nonreligious+Belief&rft.jtitle=PloS+one&rft.au=Harris%2C+Sam&rft.au=Kaplan%2C+Jonas+T&rft.au=Curiel%2C+Ashley&rft.au=Bookheimer%2C+Susan+Y&rft.date=2009-10-01&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft.volume=4&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=e7272&rft.epage=e7272&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0007272&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon |