Are There Multiple Visual Short-Term Memory Stores?

Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional ite...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inPloS one Vol. 3; no. 2; p. e1699
Main Authors Sligte, Ilja G., Scholte, H. Steven, Lamme, Victor A. F.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Public Library of Science 27.02.2008
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1932-6203
1932-6203
DOI10.1371/journal.pone.0001699

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.
AbstractList Background Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. Methodology/Principal Findings We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. Conclusions/Significance We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.
Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.BACKGROUNDClassic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGSWe presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCEWe find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.
BackgroundClassic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.Methodology/principal findingsWe presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.Conclusions/significanceWe find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.
Background Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. Methodology/Principal Findings We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. Conclusions/Significance We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.
Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.
Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.
Audience Academic
Author Scholte, H. Steven
Sligte, Ilja G.
Lamme, Victor A. F.
AuthorAffiliation 2 Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), Amsterdam, The Netherlands
University of Minnesota, United States of America
1 Cognitive Neuroscience Group, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: 1 Cognitive Neuroscience Group, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
– name: University of Minnesota, United States of America
– name: 2 Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Ilja G.
  surname: Sligte
  fullname: Sligte, Ilja G.
– sequence: 2
  givenname: H. Steven
  surname: Scholte
  fullname: Scholte, H. Steven
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Victor A. F.
  surname: Lamme
  fullname: Lamme, Victor A. F.
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18301775$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqNkmuLEzEUhgdZcS_6D0QLwoIfWnOZycUPSlm8FHZZsHW_hkzmTJslM6lJRtx_b2q7S7uISCAJJ8_7JufknBZHve-hKF5iNMGU43e3fgi9dpN1Dk8QQphJ-aQ4wZKSMSOIHu3tj4vTGG8Rqqhg7FlxjAVFmPPqpKDTAKPFCvJ8Nbhk1w5GNzYO2o3mKx_SeAGhG11B58PdaJ58gPjxefG01S7Ci916Vnz__Glx8XV8ef1ldjG9HBvGSBq3TYUqTupaClljyWRJsTFMslrQWkveEEP1Jo7KWiPSEiZrjhkDwoVo6oqeFa-3vmvno9rlGxWmmGBBhKCZmG2JxutbtQ620-FOeW3Vn4APS6VDssaBYqQtiTZYypaVlIDUui0Bl0AI5S2V2evD7rah7qAx0Keg3YHp4UlvV2rpfypCSobo5jHnO4PgfwwQk-psNOCc7sEPUfEMlQLxDL55BP49t8mWWur8fNu3Pt9q8migsyZ_emtzfFpywqpcYZEFbw8EmUnwKy31EKOazb_9P3t9c8ie77Er0C6tondDsr6Ph-Cr_Qo-lO6-2zLwfguY4GMM0Cpjk9745NSsUxipTWvfl0NtWlvtWjuLy0fiB_9_yX4DurD7JA
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1109_TSMC_2013_2270227
crossref_primary_10_2478_disp_2018_0002
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_02150
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01881_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychres_2018_10_018
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2012_753925
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_014_0687_2
crossref_primary_10_3934_Neuroscience_2015_4_259
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00402_2014
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00870
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_014_0690_7
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_21509
crossref_primary_10_3390_vision8010005
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_01053
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0050042
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_59975_4
crossref_primary_10_1093_analys_any022
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_024_01664_y
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_76431_5
crossref_primary_10_3390_su13126866
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2017_05_003
crossref_primary_10_1086_715184
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2017_1352639
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11245_015_9326_3
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02799_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2020_102990
crossref_primary_10_1177_1745691617707269
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_017_1335_4
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11427_020_1831_8
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_021422_041757
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2012_11_047
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_yebeh_2013_09_012
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2018_00428
crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niae035
crossref_primary_10_1134_S0362119715020036
crossref_primary_10_17759_exppsy_2023160404
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00526
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2022_05_007
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2011_11_014
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2018_11_002
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_13_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2009_08_007
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4072416
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_00830
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2435652
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0219504
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2015_10_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2018_10_020
crossref_primary_10_1080_13869795_2016_1176235
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_021_01138_5
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_0913_6
crossref_primary_10_1111_mila_12217
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2014_01399
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_011_0218_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2011_06_008
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2023_08_010
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2021_09_052
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X0700307X
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_64843_2
crossref_primary_10_3390_vision5040059
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2021_06_037
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhw011
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_5701_09_2010
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02836_9
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_011_0153_2
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00068_2013
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0154228
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci11020252
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0225068
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep21442
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_08608_4
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_12_044
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0268577
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_12_043
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_024_02918_2
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2011_604786
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brs_2012_11_003
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1259_4
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_12150
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07003111
crossref_primary_10_1080_17588921003731586
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0083671
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2011_11_010
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797619872749
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2010_492622
crossref_primary_10_1080_00222895_2022_2134287
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00838
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2012_696560
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_010_0033_4
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01973_2
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002932
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_02023_7
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002816
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_013_0572_4
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_12574
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep35573
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11065_020_09439_3
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_022_02179_w
crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_4585
crossref_primary_10_1038_ncomms13804
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2017_09_003
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2010_511237
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_021_01183_0
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00727
crossref_primary_10_1111_papq_12152
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_0836_2
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002804
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797617719949
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_00853
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2017_10_005
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0029296
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_013_0531_7
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_00591
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_013_0392_8
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02359_1
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2020_1733155
crossref_primary_10_1007_s41465_017_0055_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2018_06_012
crossref_primary_10_1590_1982_02752017000100015
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_014_0723_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2018_06_016
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2025_2471454
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2016_1225745
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_022_00401_w
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_00128
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2012_739155
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_12_010
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2011_09_022
crossref_primary_10_1027_1618_3169_a000400
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2015_05_004
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_01435
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00876_2017
crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218211052502
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01470
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2025_108542
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_15199_7
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2023_2221047
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_025_03043_4
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2015_00204
crossref_primary_10_1111_mila_12277
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797614560648
crossref_primary_10_1093_bjps_axx055
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2022_103453
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2010_04_008
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01023_7
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_012_0193_5
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_019_01640_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2018_02_012
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2021_103173
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_00033
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_86515_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2014_06_005
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2013_773522
crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niac003
crossref_primary_10_1080_09515089_2015_1079604
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2017_08_057
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11229_013_0385_y
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2015_1049623
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2023_103624
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_5732_12_2013
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2022_103446
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00076
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bspc_2021_102996
crossref_primary_10_1111_phpr_12625
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_24_9_8
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_49577_0
crossref_primary_10_1177_09637214211029977
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_020_01757_0
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2012_06_053
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2022_987051
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_12603
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2011_09_003
crossref_primary_10_1080_09515089_2017_1298086
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2011_02_017
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2020_112897
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2014_01_010
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_015_0584_5
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_017_0738_8
crossref_primary_10_3390_sym12122106
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_00938
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_014_0707_1
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_020_05862_3
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1306023
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0148504
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2010_05_002
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797612457782
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_012_0235_9
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0347
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0349
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07003007
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0254719
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1820_13_2014
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_016_0599_6
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2009_01_014
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_16156_0
crossref_primary_10_3758_PBR_17_5_680
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2011_618773
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797613516146
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0283257
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2015_05_029
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2020_104124
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4059199
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2017_08_005
crossref_primary_10_1093_geronb_gby045
crossref_primary_10_1080_09672559_2015_1047164
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002786
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976231186798
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0086666
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4060923
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1108_5
crossref_primary_10_1007_s13194_016_0136_2
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2020_00352
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2010_03_013
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2019_02_001
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_clinph_2017_10_019
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2018_00078
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2024_1354671
crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218241299918
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_016_1094_6
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2015_11_013
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2010_01_021
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2015_07_034
crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niy010
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1468_0017_2011_01422_x
crossref_primary_10_1167_18_9_2
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_012_0354_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2014_05_023
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0350
crossref_primary_10_1111_rati_12189
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107442
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0354
crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_020_01485_0
crossref_primary_10_1111_rati_12183
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2011_06_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_02_015
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0355
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2016_01_010
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11406_018_0021_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2023_105480
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_12589
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_018_1593_9
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2016_1253758
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2012_09_010
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2674_15_2015
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_20_2_11
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2023_105574
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02394_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2011_11_001
crossref_primary_10_4236_psych_2013_48093
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_012_0305_0
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01859
crossref_primary_10_1080_09515089_2010_543415
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_019_00944_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2015_08_017
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_016_1034_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_02_009
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00424
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_011_0242_3
crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2019_2937948
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2021_104692
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2016_11_007
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2022_105284
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_44080_z
Cites_doi 10.1007/BF00309030
10.1126/science.663675
10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001
10.1073/pnas.0500810102
10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502006.x
10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.683
10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006
10.3758/BF03210419
10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.92
10.3758/BF03203943
10.3758/BF03206454
10.3758/BF03194892
10.1038/29537
10.1017/S0140525X01003922
10.1126/science.1057099
10.3758/BF03334092
10.1073/pnas.0500834102
10.1162/089892902320474490
10.1162/089892903322598139
10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9
10.3758/BF03196567
10.3758/BF03192957
10.1038/85170
10.1038/36846
10.3758/BF03196951
10.3758/BF03193093
10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007
10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X
10.1037/h0034309
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.026
10.1093/cercor/bhl116
10.1073/pnas.95.6.3263
10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772833
10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X
10.1037/h0093759
10.3758/BF03204258
10.1016/0010-0285(91)90015-G
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright COPYRIGHT 2008 Public Library of Science
2008 Sligte et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Sligte et al. 2008
Copyright_xml – notice: COPYRIGHT 2008 Public Library of Science
– notice: 2008 Sligte et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
– notice: Sligte et al. 2008
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
IOV
ISR
3V.
7QG
7QL
7QO
7RV
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TG
7TM
7U9
7X2
7X7
7XB
88E
8AO
8C1
8FD
8FE
8FG
8FH
8FI
8FJ
8FK
ABJCF
ABUWG
AEUYN
AFKRA
ARAPS
ATCPS
AZQEC
BBNVY
BENPR
BGLVJ
BHPHI
C1K
CCPQU
D1I
DWQXO
FR3
FYUFA
GHDGH
GNUQQ
H94
HCIFZ
K9.
KB.
KB0
KL.
L6V
LK8
M0K
M0S
M1P
M7N
M7P
M7S
NAPCQ
P5Z
P62
P64
PATMY
PDBOC
PHGZM
PHGZT
PIMPY
PJZUB
PKEHL
PPXIY
PQEST
PQGLB
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
PTHSS
PYCSY
RC3
7X8
5PM
DOA
DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints
Gale In Context: Science
ProQuest Central (Corporate)
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Biotechnology Research Abstracts
Nursing & allied health premium.
Ecology Abstracts
Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)
Immunology Abstracts
Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Agricultural Science Collection
Health & Medical Collection
ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)
Medical Database (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Pharma Collection
Public Health Database
Technology Research Database
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Technology Collection
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Hospital Collection
Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)
Materials Science & Engineering Collection
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest One Sustainability
ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection
Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection
ProQuest Central Essentials
Biological Science Database
ProQuest Central
Technology collection
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Materials Science Collection
ProQuest Central Korea
Engineering Research Database
Health Research Premium Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Central Student
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
SciTech Premium Collection
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Materials Science Database
Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)
Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic
ProQuest Engineering Collection
ProQuest Biological Science Collection
Agricultural Science Database
Health & Medical Collection (Alumni)
Proquest Medical Database
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
Biological Science Database
Engineering Database
Nursing & Allied Health Premium
Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database
ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Science Database
Materials Science Collection
ProQuest Central Premium
ProQuest One Academic
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest One Health & Nursing
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central China
Engineering Collection
Environmental Science Collection
Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Agricultural Science Database
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection
ProQuest Central Essentials
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
SciTech Premium Collection
ProQuest Central China
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences
ProQuest One Sustainability
Health Research Premium Collection
Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts
Natural Science Collection
Health & Medical Research Collection
Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central (New)
ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni)
Engineering Collection
Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection
Engineering Database
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
ProQuest Biological Science Collection
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
Agricultural Science Collection
ProQuest Hospital Collection
ProQuest Technology Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
Biological Science Database
Ecology Abstracts
ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Science Collection
Entomology Abstracts
Nursing & Allied Health Premium
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
Environmental Science Database
ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source (Alumni)
Engineering Research Database
ProQuest One Academic
Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic
ProQuest One Academic (New)
Technology Collection
Technology Research Database
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
Materials Science Collection
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest One Health & Nursing
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Pharma Collection
ProQuest Central
ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection
Genetics Abstracts
ProQuest Engineering Collection
Biotechnology Research Abstracts
Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central Korea
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Materials Science Database
ProQuest Materials Science Collection
ProQuest Public Health
ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source
ProQuest SciTech Collection
Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database
ProQuest Medical Library
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Materials Science & Engineering Collection
Immunology Abstracts
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList
MEDLINE - Academic

Agricultural Science Database



MEDLINE

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: DOA
  name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 4
  dbid: 8FG
  name: ProQuest Technology Collection
  url: https://search.proquest.com/technologycollection1
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Sciences (General)
DocumentTitleAlternate High Capacity Vision
EISSN 1932-6203
ExternalDocumentID 1312182883
oai_doaj_org_article_62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39
PMC2246033
2900604491
A472659898
18301775
10_1371_journal_pone_0001699
Genre Journal Article
GeographicLocations Netherlands
GeographicLocations_xml – name: Netherlands
GroupedDBID ---
123
29O
2WC
53G
5VS
7RV
7X2
7X7
7XC
88E
8AO
8C1
8CJ
8FE
8FG
8FH
8FI
8FJ
A8Z
AAFWJ
AAUCC
AAWOE
AAYXX
ABDBF
ABIVO
ABJCF
ABUWG
ACGFO
ACIHN
ACIWK
ACPRK
ACUHS
ADBBV
ADRAZ
AEAQA
AENEX
AEUYN
AFKRA
AFPKN
AFRAH
AHMBA
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
APEBS
ARAPS
ATCPS
BAWUL
BBNVY
BCNDV
BENPR
BGLVJ
BHPHI
BKEYQ
BPHCQ
BVXVI
BWKFM
CCPQU
CITATION
CS3
D1I
D1J
D1K
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EAP
EAS
EBD
EMOBN
ESX
EX3
F5P
FPL
FYUFA
GROUPED_DOAJ
GX1
HCIFZ
HH5
HMCUK
HYE
IAO
IEA
IGS
IHR
IHW
INH
INR
IOV
IPY
ISE
ISR
ITC
K6-
KB.
KQ8
L6V
LK5
LK8
M0K
M1P
M48
M7P
M7R
M7S
M~E
NAPCQ
O5R
O5S
OK1
OVT
P2P
P62
PATMY
PDBOC
PHGZM
PHGZT
PIMPY
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PTHSS
PYCSY
RNS
RPM
SV3
TR2
UKHRP
WOQ
WOW
~02
~KM
3V.
BBORY
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
PMFND
7QG
7QL
7QO
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TG
7TM
7U9
7XB
8FD
8FK
AZQEC
C1K
DWQXO
FR3
GNUQQ
H94
K9.
KL.
M7N
P64
PJZUB
PKEHL
PPXIY
PQEST
PQGLB
PQUKI
PRINS
RC3
7X8
5PM
PUEGO
-
02
AAPBV
ABPTK
ADACO
BBAFP
KM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c662t-fd50572bb989b1969431cc696b83ba97d2c3a196904ba02f269b7166e2788db53
IEDL.DBID DOA
ISSN 1932-6203
IngestDate Fri Nov 26 17:22:18 EST 2021
Wed Aug 27 01:27:37 EDT 2025
Thu Aug 21 14:13:43 EDT 2025
Tue Aug 05 10:46:48 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 10:34:05 EDT 2025
Tue Jun 10 21:31:20 EDT 2025
Fri Jun 27 03:40:26 EDT 2025
Fri Jun 27 04:02:19 EDT 2025
Thu May 22 20:58:01 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 01:51:25 EST 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:08:01 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 03:30:49 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 2
Language English
License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited.
Creative Commons Attribution License
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c662t-fd50572bb989b1969431cc696b83ba97d2c3a196904ba02f269b7166e2788db53
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
Conceived and designed the experiments: VL IS HS. Performed the experiments: IS. Analyzed the data: IS. Wrote the paper: VL IS.
OpenAccessLink https://doaj.org/article/62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39
PMID 18301775
PQID 1312182883
PQPubID 1436336
PageCount e1699
ParticipantIDs plos_journals_1312182883
doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2246033
proquest_miscellaneous_70334807
proquest_journals_1312182883
gale_infotracacademiconefile_A472659898
gale_incontextgauss_ISR_A472659898
gale_incontextgauss_IOV_A472659898
gale_healthsolutions_A472659898
pubmed_primary_18301775
crossref_citationtrail_10_1371_journal_pone_0001699
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0001699
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2008-02-27
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2008-02-27
PublicationDate_xml – month: 02
  year: 2008
  text: 2008-02-27
  day: 27
PublicationDecade 2000
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: San Francisco
– name: San Francisco, USA
PublicationTitle PloS one
PublicationTitleAlternate PLoS One
PublicationYear 2008
Publisher Public Library of Science
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
Publisher_xml – name: Public Library of Science
– name: Public Library of Science (PLoS)
References T Makovski (ref18) 2007; 14
VM Townsend (ref28) 1973; 98
VA Lamme (ref36) 1998; 95
H Olsson (ref10) 2005; 102
EK Vogel (ref7) 2001; 27
DE Irwin (ref1) 1991; 23
J Lepsien (ref16) 2005; 26
G Sperling (ref13) 1960; 74
WV Parr (ref4) 1992; 30
R Landman (ref15) 2003; 43
N Block (ref23) 2005; 9
AO Dick (ref27) 1969; 82
DE Irwin (ref2) 1996; 5
VA Lamme (ref32) 2002; 14
M Coltheart (ref26) 1980; 27
Y Jiang (ref11) 2004; 66
J Lepsien (ref17) 2007; 17
A Pascual-Leone (ref34) 2001; 292
DJ Mewhort (ref29) 1981; 9
VA Lamme (ref24) 2003; 7
IC Griffin (ref14) 2003; 15
GA Alvarez (ref8) 2004; 15
MJLS Matsukura (ref20) 2007; 69
H Super (ref37) 2001; 4
S Dehaene (ref38) 2006; 10
H Pashler (ref5) 1988; 44
Y Jiang (ref12) 2000; 26
N Cowan (ref21) 2001; 24
WA Phillips (ref6) 1974; 16
R Landman (ref39) 2004; 11
J Jolij (ref33) 2005; 102
K McRae (ref30) 1987; 49
HY Eng (ref9) 2005; 12
EH Adelson (ref22) 1978; 201
JM Hupe (ref35) 1998; 394
T Makovski (ref19)
SJ Luck (ref3) 1997; 390
VA Lamme (ref25) 2006; 10
VA Lamme (ref31) 2000; 23
663675 - Science. 1978 Aug 11;201(4355):544-6
11515286 - Behav Brain Sci. 2001 Feb;24(1):87-114; discussion 114-85
15668096 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Feb;9(2):46-52
6992093 - Percept Psychophys. 1980 Mar;27(3):183-228
9501251 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998 Mar 17;95(6):3263-8
9723617 - Nature. 1998 Aug 20;394(6695):784-7
16615339 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2005 Dec;12(6):1127-33
18315412 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008 Mar;34(2):369-80
15937119 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 14;102(24):8776-80
11074267 - Trends Neurosci. 2000 Nov;23(11):571-9
18078232 - Percept Psychophys. 2007 Nov;69(8):1422-34
15955482 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jul 1;26(3):733-43
16030150 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jul 26;102(30):10747-51
15260190 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2004 Apr;11(2):254-61
1884598 - Cogn Psychol. 1991 Jul;23(3):420-56
16603406 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 May;10(5):204-11
17099066 - Cereb Cortex. 2007 Sep;17(9):2072-83
18229477 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2007 Dec;14(6):1072-8
10855426 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2000 May;26(3):683-702
12419127 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 Oct 1;14(7):1044-53
7231170 - Mem Cognit. 1981 Jan;9(1):50-67
11248943 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001 Feb;27(1):92-114
5378047 - J Exp Psychol. 1969 Nov;82(2):279-84
9384378 - Nature. 1997 Nov 20;390(6657):279-81
3441580 - Psychol Res. 1987;49(4):221-7
12536137 - Vision Res. 2003 Jan;43(2):149-64
16997611 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 Nov;10(11):494-501
11224548 - Nat Neurosci. 2001 Mar;4(3):304-10
15283069 - Percept Psychophys. 2004 Apr;66(3):446-53
4704202 - J Exp Psychol. 1973 Apr;98(1):113-8
14709235 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2003 Nov 15;15(8):1176-94
12517353 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Jan;7(1):12-18
14738517 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Feb;15(2):106-11
3226885 - Percept Psychophys. 1988 Oct;44(4):369-78
11313497 - Science. 2001 Apr 20;292(5516):510-2
References_xml – volume: 49
  start-page: 221
  year: 1987
  ident: ref30
  article-title: Spatiotopic and retinotopic components of iconic memory.
  publication-title: Psychol Res
  doi: 10.1007/BF00309030
– volume: 201
  start-page: 544
  year: 1978
  ident: ref22
  article-title: Iconic storage: the role of rods.
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.663675
– volume: 10
  start-page: 494
  year: 2006
  ident: ref25
  article-title: Towards a true neural stance on consciousness.
  publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001
– volume: 102
  start-page: 8776
  year: 2005
  ident: ref10
  article-title: Visual memory needs categories.
  publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500810102
– volume: 82
  start-page: 279
  year: 1969
  ident: ref27
  article-title: J Exp Psychol
– volume: 15
  start-page: 106
  year: 2004
  ident: ref8
  article-title: The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects.
  publication-title: Psychol Sci
  doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502006.x
– volume: 26
  start-page: 683
  year: 2000
  ident: ref12
  article-title: Organization of visual short-term memory.
  publication-title: J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.683
– volume: 9
  start-page: 46
  year: 2005
  ident: ref23
  article-title: Two neural correlates of consciousness.
  publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006
– volume: 44
  start-page: 369
  year: 1988
  ident: ref5
  article-title: Familiarity and visual change detection.
  publication-title: Percept Psychophys
  doi: 10.3758/BF03210419
– volume: 27
  start-page: 92
  year: 2001
  ident: ref7
  article-title: Storage of features, conjunctions and objects in visual working memory.
  publication-title: J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.92
– volume: 16
  start-page: 283
  year: 1974
  ident: ref6
  article-title: On the distinction between sensory storage and short-term visual memory.
  publication-title: Percept Psychophys
  doi: 10.3758/BF03203943
– volume: 12
  start-page: 1127
  year: 2005
  ident: ref9
  article-title: Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli.
  publication-title: Psychon Bull Rev
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206454
– volume: 66
  start-page: 446
  year: 2004
  ident: ref11
  article-title: Perceptual grouping in change detection.
  publication-title: Percept Psychophys
  doi: 10.3758/BF03194892
– volume: 394
  start-page: 784
  year: 1998
  ident: ref35
  article-title: Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure and background by V1, V2 and V3 neurons.
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/29537
– volume: 24
  start-page: 87
  year: 2001
  ident: ref21
  article-title: The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity.
  publication-title: Behav Brain Sci
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01003922
– volume: 292
  start-page: 510
  year: 2001
  ident: ref34
  article-title: Fast backprojections from the motion to the primary visual area necessary for visual awareness.
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.1057099
– volume: 30
  start-page: 369
  year: 1992
  ident: ref4
  article-title: Delayed matching-to-sample performance as a measure of human visuospatial working memory.
  publication-title: B Psychonomic Soc
  doi: 10.3758/BF03334092
– volume: 102
  start-page: 10747
  year: 2005
  ident: ref33
  article-title: Repression of unconscious information by conscious processing: evidence from affective blindsight induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation.
  publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500834102
– volume: 14
  start-page: 1044
  year: 2002
  ident: ref32
  article-title: Masking interrupts figure-ground signals in V1.
  publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci
  doi: 10.1162/089892902320474490
– volume: 15
  start-page: 1176
  year: 2003
  ident: ref14
  article-title: Orienting attention to locations in internal representations.
  publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci
  doi: 10.1162/089892903322598139
– volume: 43
  start-page: 149
  year: 2003
  ident: ref15
  article-title: Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness.
  publication-title: Vision Res
  doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9
– volume: 11
  start-page: 254
  year: 2004
  ident: ref39
  article-title: The role of figure-ground segregation in change blindness.
  publication-title: Psychon Bull Rev
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196567
– ident: ref19
  article-title: Orienting attention in visual working memory reduces interference from memory probes.
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1422
  year: 2007
  ident: ref20
  article-title: Attention effects during visual short-term memory maintenance: Protection or prioritization?
  publication-title: Percept Psychophys
  doi: 10.3758/BF03192957
– volume: 4
  start-page: 304
  year: 2001
  ident: ref37
  article-title: Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1).
  publication-title: Nat Neurosci
  doi: 10.1038/85170
– volume: 390
  start-page: 279
  year: 1997
  ident: ref3
  article-title: The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions.
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/36846
– volume: 9
  start-page: 50
  year: 1981
  ident: ref29
  article-title: Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: an evaluation of the bar-probe task.
  publication-title: Mem Cognit
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196951
– volume: 14
  start-page: 1072
  year: 2007
  ident: ref18
  article-title: Distributing versus focusing attention in visual short-term memory
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193093
– volume: 10
  start-page: 204
  year: 2006
  ident: ref38
  article-title: Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy.
  publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007
– volume: 7
  start-page: 12
  year: 2003
  ident: ref24
  article-title: Why visual attention and awareness are different.
  publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X
– volume: 98
  start-page: 113
  year: 1973
  ident: ref28
  article-title: Loss of spatial and identity information following a tachistoscopic exposure.
  publication-title: J Exp Psychol
  doi: 10.1037/h0034309
– volume: 26
  start-page: 733
  year: 2005
  ident: ref16
  article-title: Directing spatial attention in mental representations: Interactions between attentional orienting and working-memory load.
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.026
– volume: 17
  start-page: 2072
  year: 2007
  ident: ref17
  article-title: Attentional modulation of object representations in working memory.
  publication-title: Cereb Cortex
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl116
– volume: 95
  start-page: 3263
  year: 1998
  ident: ref36
  article-title: Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex is suppressed by anesthesia.
  publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3263
– volume: 5
  start-page: 94
  year: 1996
  ident: ref2
  article-title: Integrating information across saccadic eye movements.
  publication-title: Curr Dir in Psychol Sci
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772833
– volume: 23
  start-page: 571
  year: 2000
  ident: ref31
  article-title: The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing.
  publication-title: Trends Neurosci
  doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X
– volume: 74
  start-page: 1
  year: 1960
  ident: ref13
  article-title: The information available in brief visual presentations.
  publication-title: Psychol monogr
  doi: 10.1037/h0093759
– volume: 27
  start-page: 183
  year: 1980
  ident: ref26
  article-title: Iconic memory and visible persistence.
  publication-title: Percept Psychophys
  doi: 10.3758/BF03204258
– volume: 23
  start-page: 420
  year: 1991
  ident: ref1
  article-title: Information integration across saccadic eye movements.
  publication-title: Cognit Psychol
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(91)90015-G
– reference: 11248943 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001 Feb;27(1):92-114
– reference: 9723617 - Nature. 1998 Aug 20;394(6695):784-7
– reference: 16603406 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 May;10(5):204-11
– reference: 15260190 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2004 Apr;11(2):254-61
– reference: 5378047 - J Exp Psychol. 1969 Nov;82(2):279-84
– reference: 11074267 - Trends Neurosci. 2000 Nov;23(11):571-9
– reference: 4704202 - J Exp Psychol. 1973 Apr;98(1):113-8
– reference: 16030150 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jul 26;102(30):10747-51
– reference: 12536137 - Vision Res. 2003 Jan;43(2):149-64
– reference: 12419127 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 Oct 1;14(7):1044-53
– reference: 15937119 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 14;102(24):8776-80
– reference: 11224548 - Nat Neurosci. 2001 Mar;4(3):304-10
– reference: 12517353 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Jan;7(1):12-18
– reference: 663675 - Science. 1978 Aug 11;201(4355):544-6
– reference: 11515286 - Behav Brain Sci. 2001 Feb;24(1):87-114; discussion 114-85
– reference: 18229477 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2007 Dec;14(6):1072-8
– reference: 16997611 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 Nov;10(11):494-501
– reference: 1884598 - Cogn Psychol. 1991 Jul;23(3):420-56
– reference: 14709235 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2003 Nov 15;15(8):1176-94
– reference: 18078232 - Percept Psychophys. 2007 Nov;69(8):1422-34
– reference: 7231170 - Mem Cognit. 1981 Jan;9(1):50-67
– reference: 3226885 - Percept Psychophys. 1988 Oct;44(4):369-78
– reference: 17099066 - Cereb Cortex. 2007 Sep;17(9):2072-83
– reference: 16615339 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2005 Dec;12(6):1127-33
– reference: 3441580 - Psychol Res. 1987;49(4):221-7
– reference: 10855426 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2000 May;26(3):683-702
– reference: 11313497 - Science. 2001 Apr 20;292(5516):510-2
– reference: 6992093 - Percept Psychophys. 1980 Mar;27(3):183-228
– reference: 15283069 - Percept Psychophys. 2004 Apr;66(3):446-53
– reference: 15668096 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Feb;9(2):46-52
– reference: 9384378 - Nature. 1997 Nov 20;390(6657):279-81
– reference: 9501251 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998 Mar 17;95(6):3263-8
– reference: 18315412 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008 Mar;34(2):369-80
– reference: 15955482 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jul 1;26(3):733-43
– reference: 14738517 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Feb;15(2):106-11
SSID ssj0053866
Score 2.4299033
Snippet Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are...
Background Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human...
BackgroundClassic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human...
Background Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human...
SourceID plos
doaj
pubmedcentral
proquest
gale
pubmed
crossref
SourceType Open Website
Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
StartPage e1699
SubjectTerms Afterimage - physiology
Change detection
Cues
Data processing
Eye (anatomy)
Humans
Information processing
Localization
Masks
Memory
Memory, Short-Term - physiology
Neuroscience/Behavioral Neuroscience
Neuroscience/Cognitive Neuroscience
Neuroscience/Sensory Systems
Neuroscience/Theoretical Neuroscience
Neurosciences
Observers
Representations
Retina
Retina - physiology
Sensory perception
Short term memory
Taxonomy
Time Factors
Transcranial magnetic stimulation
Trends
Visual observation
Visual perception
Visual Perception - physiology
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: Public Health Database
  dbid: 8C1
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3db9MwELegvPCCGF_LNliEkICHbI3tOMnTVCamgVSQ6DbtzbIde5s0JaVpH_bfc-e4KUET8FKp8SVK7it38d3vCHmnuUor7MYxitkE_rhEmTRLjLGOG-F47hH4pt_E6Tn_epldhg9ubSirXPtE76irxuA38sOUpQg2XhTsaP4zwalRuLsaRmg8JI9glSJ2fnHcl3iALQsR2uVYnh4G6RzMm9oe-GDHI75uXkcetb_3zaP5bdPeF3j-WT_52wvp5Cl5EiLJeNKJfos8sPUzshVstY0_BEDpj88JmyxsDPoAv9NQPxhf3LQrOHt2DeF3cgbuOZ5ize1dPIMk3LZHL8j5yeez49MkDEtIjBB0mbgKUw2qdVmUGjFvIDIwRpRCF0yrMq-oYQqPj7lWY-qoKDXkSsJSSIIrnbGXZFQDY7ZJTLVTBS-5UyArnmfKibEzkPo4VwjGTUTYmmfSBCRxHGhxK_32WA4ZRccCiZyWgdMRSfqz5h2Sxj_oP6E4elrEwfYHmsWVDGYl4Z44BeUqSwehFbWlUo7blFuIenLH4CL7KEzZNZX21iwnPKciw9mZEXnrKRALo8Zimyu1alv55fvFfxDNfgyI3gci1wA7jAoNDvBMiLE1oNxG3Vo_dis3yh2RvbW-3b-83y-DH8DNHVXbZtVK8NwM4QEi8qpTzg2TC3DieZ5FJB-o7YCzw5X65tojjSPaIFx35-83tUsed1U02Oi1R0bLxcq-hlBtqd94e_wFwVY9nQ
  priority: 102
  providerName: ProQuest
– databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access
  dbid: M48
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3fb9MwELZGeeEFMX4tMFiEkICHVI3j2PEDmgpiGkgDCSjam2W79japS0rTIvbfc5c4gaAieOCx8TlqPt_Zd_Ldd4Q8NUync6zGsTpzCfzwibZpnljrPLPcM9Ew8J2858cz9u40P90hXc_WAGC9NbTDflKz1WL8_evVIRj8y6Zrg0i7SeNlVbpx48RIeY1ch7NJYDOHE9bfK4B1cx4K6P40c3BANTz-_W49Wi6qepsr-ntG5S9H1NEtcjP4lvG0VYZdsuPK22Q3WG8dPw8U0y_ukGy6cjG6fi7uMgrjbxf1BmbX54BFght2fIlZuFcx5k-6-vAumR29-fz6OAntExLLOV0nfo7BBzVGFtIgCw74CtZyyU2RGS3FnNpM4_MJM3pCPeXSQPTEHYWweG7y7B4ZlQDMHomp8bpgknkNq8dErj2feAvBkPcFz5iNSNZhpmzgFscWFwvVXJgJiDFaCBQirQLSEUn6WcuWW-Mv8q9wOXpZZMZuHlSrMxUMTcF_YhTUTUoPzhZ1UmvPXMoc-EHCZ_CSA1xM1ZaZ9vatpkxQnmM3zYg8aSSQHaPE9Jszvalr9fbDl38Q-vRxIPQsCPkK4LA6lDzANyHr1kByD3Wr--waQEiRW78osojsd_q2ffigH4adAa97dOmqTa1gL8-QMCAi91vl_AlyAdu6EHlExEBtB8gOR8qL84Z7HPkH4b0P_sdSPCQ32uwbLBDbJ6P1auMegYu3No8bq_0BEvJRfQ
  priority: 102
  providerName: Scholars Portal
Title Are There Multiple Visual Short-Term Memory Stores?
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18301775
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1312182883
https://www.proquest.com/docview/70334807
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC2246033
https://doaj.org/article/62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001699
Volume 3
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3db9MwELegvPCCGF8LjC5CSMBDtsZ27OQJddPKQNpAg6G-WbZrs0kjrZYWif-eO8fNFjRpPPBiKfE5Su7O96Hc_UzIa8N1PsNuHKuZy-DCZ9rmRWat89wKz2VA4Ds6Foen_NO0mF476gtrwlp44JZxu4J6TmF5VXlwntRVWnvucu7Ar0nPQuse-Lx1MtXaYNjFQsRGOSbz3SiXncW8djshzAlYr1eOKOD1d1Z5sLiYNzeFnH9XTl5zRZOH5EGMIdNx--4b5I6rH5GNuEub9G2Ekn73mLDxpUsxxHPpunIw_XXerGB1cwaBd4aGOf2J1ba_U6yTdM37J-R0cvBt_zCLxyRkVgi6zPwMkwxqTFVWBtFuICawVlTClMzoSs6oZRrvj7jRI-qpqAxkScJRSH9npmBPyaAGxmySlBqvS15xr0FKXBbai5G3kPR4XwrGbULYmmfKRgxxPMriQoUfYxJyiZYFCjmtIqcTknWrFi2Gxi30eyiOjhYRsMMN0AsV9ULdphcJ2UZhqradtNvHaswlFQWempmQV4ECUTBqLLP5oVdNoz5-_v4PRF9PekRvIpGfAzusjq0N8E2IrtWj3ETdWn92A0zIEUO_LFlCttb6dvP0djcNFgB_6-jazVeNApvNEBggIc9a5bxicgnmW8oiIbKntj3O9mfq87OAMY44g_Dc5_9DFC_I_bbKBhvBtshgeblyLyGUW5ohuSunEsZyP8dx8mFI7u0dHH85GYb9DOMRL_8AmlRMGg
linkProvider Directory of Open Access Journals
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3db9MwELem7gFeEONrYYNGCAQ8ZGts10ke0FQGU8vWIdFu2pvnuPY2aUpK0wrtn-Jv5C5xUoom4GUvkRJfouR8vo_47neEvE65CidYjaMVMwGc2EDpsBtobSzXwvKoROAbHov-Cf9y1j1bIz_rWhhMq6x1YqmoJ7nGf-S7IQsRbDyO2d70e4Bdo3B3tW6hUYnFobn5ASFb8WHwCeb3DaUHn8f7_cB1FQi0EHQe2An65DRNkzhJERwGTKjWIhFpzFKVRBOqmcLrHZ6qDrVUJCkEFcJQiBYnKXaJAJW_zhmLEKs_3m9SSkB3COHK81gU7jpp2JnmmdkpnasSYXZp_souAY0taE2v8-I2R_fPfM3fDODBQ_LAea5-rxK1DbJmskdkw-mGwn_nAKzfPyasNzM-yB8chy5f0T-9KhZw9-gS3P1gDObAH2KO740_gqDfFHtPyMmdsPEpaWXAmE3i09SqmCfcKpANHnWVFR2rIdSyNhaMa4-wmmdSO-RybKBxLcvtuAgimIoFEjktHac9EjR3TSvkjn_Qf8TpaGgRd7u8kM8upFvGEt6JUxDmJLHgylGTKGW5CbkBLyuyDB7SxsmUVRFroz1kj0dUdLFXp0delRSIvZFhcs-FWhSFHHw9_Q-i0bcVoreOyObADq1cQQV8E2J6rVBuomzVn13I5WLyyHYtb7cPt5th0Du4maQyky8KCZaCIRyBR55VwrlkcgxGI4q6HolWxHaFs6sj2dVliWyO6Ibw3Od_f6k2udcfD4_k0eD4cIvcrzJ4sMhsm7Tms4V5AW7iPH1Zrk2fnN-1MvgFqRx5Bw
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3db9MwELemIiFeEONrgUEjBAIesjaxaycPaCqMamV0ILpNffMc194mTUlpWqH9a_x13CVOStEEvOwlUuJLlJzP9xHf_Y6QlylT4RSrcbSiJoATGygd9gKtjWWaWyZKBL7RId8_Zp8mvckG-VnXwmBaZa0TS0U9zTX-I--ENESw8TimHevSIr7uDXZn3wPsIIU7rXU7jUpEDszVDwjfinfDPZjrV1E0-Hj0YT9wHQYCzXm0COwU_fMoTZM4SREoBsyp1jzhaUxTlYhppKnC612Wqm5kI56kEGBwE0HkOE2xYwSo_1uCgtmEtSQmTbAHeoRzV6pHRdhxkrEzyzOzUzpaJdrsyhSWHQMau9CaXebFdU7vn7mbvxnDwT1y13mxfr8Su02yYbL7ZNPpicJ_48Cs3z4gtD83PsgiHEcud9E_uSiWcPf4HFz_4AhMgz_CfN8rf7zIIfrffUiOb4SNj0grA8ZsET9KrYpZwqwCOWGipyzvWg1hl7Uxp0x7hNY8k9qhmGMzjUtZbs0JiGYqFkjktHSc9kjQ3DWrUDz-Qf8ep6OhRQzu8kI-P5NuSUt4JxaBYCeJBbcuMolSlpmQGfC4hKXwkDZOpqwKWhtNIvtMRLyHfTs98qKkQByODCX6TC2LQg6_nPwH0fjbGtFrR2RzYIdWrrgCvgnxvdYot1C26s8u5GpheWS7lrfrh9vNMOgg3FhSmcmXhQSrQRGawCOPK-FcMTkGAyJEzyNiTWzXOLs-kl2clyjniHQIz33y95dqk9ugBuTn4eHBU3KnSubBerNt0lrMl-YZeIyL9Hm5NH1yetO64Be8RX1p
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Are+there+multiple+visual+short-term+memory+stores%3F&rft.jtitle=PloS+one&rft.au=Ilja+G+Sligte&rft.au=H+Steven+Scholte&rft.au=Victor+A+F+Lamme&rft.date=2008-02-27&rft.pub=Public+Library+of+Science+%28PLoS%29&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft.volume=3&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=e1699&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0001699&rft.externalDBID=DOA&rft.externalDocID=oai_doaj_org_article_62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon