Are There Multiple Visual Short-Term Memory Stores?
Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional ite...
Saved in:
Published in | PloS one Vol. 3; no. 2; p. e1699 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Public Library of Science
27.02.2008
Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1932-6203 1932-6203 |
DOI | 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.
We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.
We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Background
Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.
Methodology/Principal Findings
We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.
Conclusions/Significance
We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will. Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.BACKGROUNDClassic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGSWe presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will.CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCEWe find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will. BackgroundClassic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations.Methodology/principal findingsWe presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array.Conclusions/significanceWe find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will. Background Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. Methodology/Principal Findings We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. Conclusions/Significance We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will. Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will. Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are cued to shift attention to one item in VSTM during retention, it seems as if there is a much larger representation, which keeps additional items in a more fragile VSTM store. Thus far, it is not clear whether the capacity of this fragile VSTM store indeed exceeds the traditional capacity limits of VSTM. The current experiments address this issue and explore the capacity, stability, and duration of fragile VSTM representations. We presented cues in a change-detection task either just after off-set of the memory array (iconic-cue), 1,000 ms after off-set of the memory array (retro-cue) or after on-set of the probe array (post-cue). We observed three stages in visual information processing 1) iconic memory with unlimited capacity, 2) a four seconds lasting fragile VSTM store with a capacity that is at least a factor of two higher than 3) the robust and capacity-limited form of VSTM. Iconic memory seemed to depend on the strength of the positive after-image resulting from the memory display and was virtually absent under conditions of isoluminance or when intervening light masks were presented. This suggests that iconic memory is driven by prolonged retinal activation beyond stimulus duration. Fragile VSTM representations were not affected by light masks, but were completely overwritten by irrelevant pattern masks that spatially overlapped the memory array. We find that immediately after a stimulus has disappeared from view, subjects can still access information from iconic memory because they can see an after-image of the display. After that period, human observers can still access a substantial, but somewhat more limited amount of information from a high-capacity, but fragile VSTM that is overwritten when new items are presented to the eyes. What is left after that is the traditional VSTM store, with a limit of about four objects. We conclude that human observers store more sustained representations than is evident from standard change detection tasks and that these representations can be accessed at will. |
Audience | Academic |
Author | Scholte, H. Steven Sligte, Ilja G. Lamme, Victor A. F. |
AuthorAffiliation | 2 Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), Amsterdam, The Netherlands University of Minnesota, United States of America 1 Cognitive Neuroscience Group, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 1 Cognitive Neuroscience Group, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands – name: University of Minnesota, United States of America – name: 2 Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences (KNAW), Amsterdam, The Netherlands |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Ilja G. surname: Sligte fullname: Sligte, Ilja G. – sequence: 2 givenname: H. Steven surname: Scholte fullname: Scholte, H. Steven – sequence: 3 givenname: Victor A. F. surname: Lamme fullname: Lamme, Victor A. F. |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18301775$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkmuLEzEUhgdZcS_6D0QLwoIfWnOZycUPSlm8FHZZsHW_hkzmTJslM6lJRtx_b2q7S7uISCAJJ8_7JufknBZHve-hKF5iNMGU43e3fgi9dpN1Dk8QQphJ-aQ4wZKSMSOIHu3tj4vTGG8Rqqhg7FlxjAVFmPPqpKDTAKPFCvJ8Nbhk1w5GNzYO2o3mKx_SeAGhG11B58PdaJ58gPjxefG01S7Ci916Vnz__Glx8XV8ef1ldjG9HBvGSBq3TYUqTupaClljyWRJsTFMslrQWkveEEP1Jo7KWiPSEiZrjhkDwoVo6oqeFa-3vmvno9rlGxWmmGBBhKCZmG2JxutbtQ620-FOeW3Vn4APS6VDssaBYqQtiTZYypaVlIDUui0Bl0AI5S2V2evD7rah7qAx0Keg3YHp4UlvV2rpfypCSobo5jHnO4PgfwwQk-psNOCc7sEPUfEMlQLxDL55BP49t8mWWur8fNu3Pt9q8migsyZ_emtzfFpywqpcYZEFbw8EmUnwKy31EKOazb_9P3t9c8ie77Er0C6tondDsr6Ph-Cr_Qo-lO6-2zLwfguY4GMM0Cpjk9745NSsUxipTWvfl0NtWlvtWjuLy0fiB_9_yX4DurD7JA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1109_TSMC_2013_2270227 crossref_primary_10_2478_disp_2018_0002 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_02150 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01881_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychres_2018_10_018 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2012_753925 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_014_0687_2 crossref_primary_10_3934_Neuroscience_2015_4_259 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00402_2014 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00870 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_014_0690_7 crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_21509 crossref_primary_10_3390_vision8010005 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_01053 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0050042 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_59975_4 crossref_primary_10_1093_analys_any022 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_024_01664_y crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_76431_5 crossref_primary_10_3390_su13126866 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2017_05_003 crossref_primary_10_1086_715184 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2017_1352639 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11245_015_9326_3 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02799_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2020_102990 crossref_primary_10_1177_1745691617707269 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_017_1335_4 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11427_020_1831_8 crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_021422_041757 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2012_11_047 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_yebeh_2013_09_012 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2018_00428 crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niae035 crossref_primary_10_1134_S0362119715020036 crossref_primary_10_17759_exppsy_2023160404 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00526 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2022_05_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2011_11_014 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2018_11_002 crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_13_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2009_08_007 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4072416 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_00830 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2024_2435652 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0219504 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2015_10_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2018_10_020 crossref_primary_10_1080_13869795_2016_1176235 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_021_01138_5 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_0913_6 crossref_primary_10_1111_mila_12217 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2014_01399 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_011_0218_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2011_06_008 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2023_08_010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2021_09_052 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X0700307X crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_64843_2 crossref_primary_10_3390_vision5040059 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2021_06_037 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhw011 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_5701_09_2010 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02836_9 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_011_0153_2 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00068_2013 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0154228 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci11020252 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0225068 crossref_primary_10_1038_srep21442 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_08608_4 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_12_044 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0268577 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_12_043 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_024_02918_2 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2011_604786 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brs_2012_11_003 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1259_4 crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_12150 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07003111 crossref_primary_10_1080_17588921003731586 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0083671 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2011_11_010 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797619872749 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2010_492622 crossref_primary_10_1080_00222895_2022_2134287 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00838 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2012_696560 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_010_0033_4 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01973_2 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002932 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_02023_7 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002816 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_013_0572_4 crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_12574 crossref_primary_10_1038_srep35573 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11065_020_09439_3 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_022_02179_w crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_4585 crossref_primary_10_1038_ncomms13804 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2017_09_003 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2010_511237 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_021_01183_0 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00727 crossref_primary_10_1111_papq_12152 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_0836_2 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002804 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797617719949 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_00853 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2017_10_005 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0029296 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_013_0531_7 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_00591 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_013_0392_8 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02359_1 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2020_1733155 crossref_primary_10_1007_s41465_017_0055_y crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2018_06_012 crossref_primary_10_1590_1982_02752017000100015 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_014_0723_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2018_06_016 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2025_2471454 crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2016_1225745 crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_022_00401_w crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_00128 crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2012_739155 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_12_010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2011_09_022 crossref_primary_10_1027_1618_3169_a000400 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2015_05_004 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_01435 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00876_2017 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218211052502 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01470 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2025_108542 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_15199_7 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2023_2221047 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_025_03043_4 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2015_00204 crossref_primary_10_1111_mila_12277 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797614560648 crossref_primary_10_1093_bjps_axx055 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2022_103453 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2010_04_008 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01023_7 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_012_0193_5 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_019_01640_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2018_02_012 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2021_103173 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_00033 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_86515_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2014_06_005 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2013_773522 crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niac003 crossref_primary_10_1080_09515089_2015_1079604 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2017_08_057 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11229_013_0385_y crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2015_1049623 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2023_103624 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_5732_12_2013 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2022_103446 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00076 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bspc_2021_102996 crossref_primary_10_1111_phpr_12625 crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_24_9_8 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_49577_0 crossref_primary_10_1177_09637214211029977 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_020_01757_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2012_06_053 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2022_987051 crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_12603 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2011_09_003 crossref_primary_10_1080_09515089_2017_1298086 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2011_02_017 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2020_112897 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2014_01_010 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_015_0584_5 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_017_0738_8 crossref_primary_10_3390_sym12122106 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2014_00938 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_014_0707_1 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_020_05862_3 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1306023 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0148504 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2010_05_002 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797612457782 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_012_0235_9 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0347 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0349 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07003007 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0254719 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1820_13_2014 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_016_0599_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2009_01_014 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_16156_0 crossref_primary_10_3758_PBR_17_5_680 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2011_618773 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797613516146 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0283257 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2015_05_029 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2020_104124 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4059199 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2017_08_005 crossref_primary_10_1093_geronb_gby045 crossref_primary_10_1080_09672559_2015_1047164 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X07002786 crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976231186798 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0086666 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_4060923 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1108_5 crossref_primary_10_1007_s13194_016_0136_2 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2020_00352 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2010_03_013 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2019_02_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_clinph_2017_10_019 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2018_00078 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2024_1354671 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218241299918 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_016_1094_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2015_11_013 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2010_01_021 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2015_07_034 crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niy010 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1468_0017_2011_01422_x crossref_primary_10_1167_18_9_2 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_012_0354_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2014_05_023 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0350 crossref_primary_10_1111_rati_12189 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107442 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0354 crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_020_01485_0 crossref_primary_10_1111_rati_12183 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2011_06_005 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_02_015 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0355 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2016_01_010 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11406_018_0021_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2023_105480 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_12589 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_018_1593_9 crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2016_1253758 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2012_09_010 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2674_15_2015 crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_20_2_11 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2023_105574 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_021_02394_y crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2011_11_001 crossref_primary_10_4236_psych_2013_48093 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_012_0305_0 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01859 crossref_primary_10_1080_09515089_2010_543415 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_019_00944_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2015_08_017 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_016_1034_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_02_009 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00424 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_011_0242_3 crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2019_2937948 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2021_104692 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2016_11_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2022_105284 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_44080_z |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/BF00309030 10.1126/science.663675 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001 10.1073/pnas.0500810102 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502006.x 10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.683 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006 10.3758/BF03210419 10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.92 10.3758/BF03203943 10.3758/BF03206454 10.3758/BF03194892 10.1038/29537 10.1017/S0140525X01003922 10.1126/science.1057099 10.3758/BF03334092 10.1073/pnas.0500834102 10.1162/089892902320474490 10.1162/089892903322598139 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9 10.3758/BF03196567 10.3758/BF03192957 10.1038/85170 10.1038/36846 10.3758/BF03196951 10.3758/BF03193093 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X 10.1037/h0034309 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.026 10.1093/cercor/bhl116 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3263 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772833 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X 10.1037/h0093759 10.3758/BF03204258 10.1016/0010-0285(91)90015-G |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | COPYRIGHT 2008 Public Library of Science 2008 Sligte et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. Sligte et al. 2008 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: COPYRIGHT 2008 Public Library of Science – notice: 2008 Sligte et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. – notice: Sligte et al. 2008 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM IOV ISR 3V. 7QG 7QL 7QO 7RV 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TG 7TM 7U9 7X2 7X7 7XB 88E 8AO 8C1 8FD 8FE 8FG 8FH 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABJCF ABUWG AEUYN AFKRA ARAPS ATCPS AZQEC BBNVY BENPR BGLVJ BHPHI C1K CCPQU D1I DWQXO FR3 FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ H94 HCIFZ K9. KB. KB0 KL. L6V LK8 M0K M0S M1P M7N M7P M7S NAPCQ P5Z P62 P64 PATMY PDBOC PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PJZUB PKEHL PPXIY PQEST PQGLB PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PTHSS PYCSY RC3 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Gale In Context: Opposing Viewpoints Gale In Context: Science ProQuest Central (Corporate) Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Biotechnology Research Abstracts Nursing & allied health premium. Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Immunology Abstracts Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts Virology and AIDS Abstracts Agricultural Science Collection Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Pharma Collection Public Health Database Technology Research Database ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Technology Collection ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Hospital Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) Materials Science & Engineering Collection ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest One Sustainability ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection ProQuest Central Essentials Biological Science Database ProQuest Central Technology collection ProQuest Natural Science Collection Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Materials Science Collection ProQuest Central Korea Engineering Research Database Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Materials Science Database Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition) Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic ProQuest Engineering Collection ProQuest Biological Science Collection Agricultural Science Database Health & Medical Collection (Alumni) Proquest Medical Database Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Biological Science Database Engineering Database Nursing & Allied Health Premium Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Science Database Materials Science Collection ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China Engineering Collection Environmental Science Collection Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Agricultural Science Database Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection ProQuest Central Essentials Nucleic Acids Abstracts SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest Central China Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences ProQuest One Sustainability Health Research Premium Collection Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts Natural Science Collection Health & Medical Research Collection Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) Engineering Collection Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Collection Engineering Database Virology and AIDS Abstracts ProQuest Biological Science Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition Agricultural Science Collection ProQuest Hospital Collection ProQuest Technology Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) Biological Science Database Ecology Abstracts ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Science Collection Entomology Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Premium ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition Environmental Science Database ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source (Alumni) Engineering Research Database ProQuest One Academic Meteorological & Geoastrophysical Abstracts - Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) Technology Collection Technology Research Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) Materials Science Collection ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Pharma Collection ProQuest Central ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection Genetics Abstracts ProQuest Engineering Collection Biotechnology Research Abstracts Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Agricultural & Environmental Science Collection AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Materials Science Database ProQuest Materials Science Collection ProQuest Public Health ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source ProQuest SciTech Collection Advanced Technologies & Aerospace Database ProQuest Medical Library Animal Behavior Abstracts Materials Science & Engineering Collection Immunology Abstracts ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic Agricultural Science Database MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 4 dbid: 8FG name: ProQuest Technology Collection url: https://search.proquest.com/technologycollection1 sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Sciences (General) |
DocumentTitleAlternate | High Capacity Vision |
EISSN | 1932-6203 |
ExternalDocumentID | 1312182883 oai_doaj_org_article_62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39 PMC2246033 2900604491 A472659898 18301775 10_1371_journal_pone_0001699 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GeographicLocations | Netherlands |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: Netherlands |
GroupedDBID | --- 123 29O 2WC 53G 5VS 7RV 7X2 7X7 7XC 88E 8AO 8C1 8CJ 8FE 8FG 8FH 8FI 8FJ A8Z AAFWJ AAUCC AAWOE AAYXX ABDBF ABIVO ABJCF ABUWG ACGFO ACIHN ACIWK ACPRK ACUHS ADBBV ADRAZ AEAQA AENEX AEUYN AFKRA AFPKN AFRAH AHMBA ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS APEBS ARAPS ATCPS BAWUL BBNVY BCNDV BENPR BGLVJ BHPHI BKEYQ BPHCQ BVXVI BWKFM CCPQU CITATION CS3 D1I D1J D1K DIK DU5 E3Z EAP EAS EBD EMOBN ESX EX3 F5P FPL FYUFA GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HCIFZ HH5 HMCUK HYE IAO IEA IGS IHR IHW INH INR IOV IPY ISE ISR ITC K6- KB. KQ8 L6V LK5 LK8 M0K M1P M48 M7P M7R M7S M~E NAPCQ O5R O5S OK1 OVT P2P P62 PATMY PDBOC PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PTHSS PYCSY RNS RPM SV3 TR2 UKHRP WOQ WOW ~02 ~KM 3V. BBORY CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PMFND 7QG 7QL 7QO 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TG 7TM 7U9 7XB 8FD 8FK AZQEC C1K DWQXO FR3 GNUQQ H94 K9. KL. M7N P64 PJZUB PKEHL PPXIY PQEST PQGLB PQUKI PRINS RC3 7X8 5PM PUEGO - 02 AAPBV ABPTK ADACO BBAFP KM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c662t-fd50572bb989b1969431cc696b83ba97d2c3a196904ba02f269b7166e2788db53 |
IEDL.DBID | DOA |
ISSN | 1932-6203 |
IngestDate | Fri Nov 26 17:22:18 EST 2021 Wed Aug 27 01:27:37 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 14:13:43 EDT 2025 Tue Aug 05 10:46:48 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 10:34:05 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 10 21:31:20 EDT 2025 Fri Jun 27 03:40:26 EDT 2025 Fri Jun 27 04:02:19 EDT 2025 Thu May 22 20:58:01 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 01:51:25 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:01 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:30:49 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 2 |
Language | English |
License | This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are properly credited. Creative Commons Attribution License |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c662t-fd50572bb989b1969431cc696b83ba97d2c3a196904ba02f269b7166e2788db53 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Conceived and designed the experiments: VL IS HS. Performed the experiments: IS. Analyzed the data: IS. Wrote the paper: VL IS. |
OpenAccessLink | https://doaj.org/article/62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39 |
PMID | 18301775 |
PQID | 1312182883 |
PQPubID | 1436336 |
PageCount | e1699 |
ParticipantIDs | plos_journals_1312182883 doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2246033 proquest_miscellaneous_70334807 proquest_journals_1312182883 gale_infotracacademiconefile_A472659898 gale_incontextgauss_ISR_A472659898 gale_incontextgauss_IOV_A472659898 gale_healthsolutions_A472659898 pubmed_primary_18301775 crossref_citationtrail_10_1371_journal_pone_0001699 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0001699 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2008-02-27 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2008-02-27 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2008 text: 2008-02-27 day: 27 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: San Francisco – name: San Francisco, USA |
PublicationTitle | PloS one |
PublicationTitleAlternate | PLoS One |
PublicationYear | 2008 |
Publisher | Public Library of Science Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
Publisher_xml | – name: Public Library of Science – name: Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
References | T Makovski (ref18) 2007; 14 VM Townsend (ref28) 1973; 98 VA Lamme (ref36) 1998; 95 H Olsson (ref10) 2005; 102 EK Vogel (ref7) 2001; 27 DE Irwin (ref1) 1991; 23 J Lepsien (ref16) 2005; 26 G Sperling (ref13) 1960; 74 WV Parr (ref4) 1992; 30 R Landman (ref15) 2003; 43 N Block (ref23) 2005; 9 AO Dick (ref27) 1969; 82 DE Irwin (ref2) 1996; 5 VA Lamme (ref32) 2002; 14 M Coltheart (ref26) 1980; 27 Y Jiang (ref11) 2004; 66 J Lepsien (ref17) 2007; 17 A Pascual-Leone (ref34) 2001; 292 DJ Mewhort (ref29) 1981; 9 VA Lamme (ref24) 2003; 7 IC Griffin (ref14) 2003; 15 GA Alvarez (ref8) 2004; 15 MJLS Matsukura (ref20) 2007; 69 H Super (ref37) 2001; 4 S Dehaene (ref38) 2006; 10 H Pashler (ref5) 1988; 44 Y Jiang (ref12) 2000; 26 N Cowan (ref21) 2001; 24 WA Phillips (ref6) 1974; 16 R Landman (ref39) 2004; 11 J Jolij (ref33) 2005; 102 K McRae (ref30) 1987; 49 HY Eng (ref9) 2005; 12 EH Adelson (ref22) 1978; 201 JM Hupe (ref35) 1998; 394 T Makovski (ref19) SJ Luck (ref3) 1997; 390 VA Lamme (ref25) 2006; 10 VA Lamme (ref31) 2000; 23 663675 - Science. 1978 Aug 11;201(4355):544-6 11515286 - Behav Brain Sci. 2001 Feb;24(1):87-114; discussion 114-85 15668096 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Feb;9(2):46-52 6992093 - Percept Psychophys. 1980 Mar;27(3):183-228 9501251 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998 Mar 17;95(6):3263-8 9723617 - Nature. 1998 Aug 20;394(6695):784-7 16615339 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2005 Dec;12(6):1127-33 18315412 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008 Mar;34(2):369-80 15937119 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 14;102(24):8776-80 11074267 - Trends Neurosci. 2000 Nov;23(11):571-9 18078232 - Percept Psychophys. 2007 Nov;69(8):1422-34 15955482 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jul 1;26(3):733-43 16030150 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jul 26;102(30):10747-51 15260190 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2004 Apr;11(2):254-61 1884598 - Cogn Psychol. 1991 Jul;23(3):420-56 16603406 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 May;10(5):204-11 17099066 - Cereb Cortex. 2007 Sep;17(9):2072-83 18229477 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2007 Dec;14(6):1072-8 10855426 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2000 May;26(3):683-702 12419127 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 Oct 1;14(7):1044-53 7231170 - Mem Cognit. 1981 Jan;9(1):50-67 11248943 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001 Feb;27(1):92-114 5378047 - J Exp Psychol. 1969 Nov;82(2):279-84 9384378 - Nature. 1997 Nov 20;390(6657):279-81 3441580 - Psychol Res. 1987;49(4):221-7 12536137 - Vision Res. 2003 Jan;43(2):149-64 16997611 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 Nov;10(11):494-501 11224548 - Nat Neurosci. 2001 Mar;4(3):304-10 15283069 - Percept Psychophys. 2004 Apr;66(3):446-53 4704202 - J Exp Psychol. 1973 Apr;98(1):113-8 14709235 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2003 Nov 15;15(8):1176-94 12517353 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Jan;7(1):12-18 14738517 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Feb;15(2):106-11 3226885 - Percept Psychophys. 1988 Oct;44(4):369-78 11313497 - Science. 2001 Apr 20;292(5516):510-2 |
References_xml | – volume: 49 start-page: 221 year: 1987 ident: ref30 article-title: Spatiotopic and retinotopic components of iconic memory. publication-title: Psychol Res doi: 10.1007/BF00309030 – volume: 201 start-page: 544 year: 1978 ident: ref22 article-title: Iconic storage: the role of rods. publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.663675 – volume: 10 start-page: 494 year: 2006 ident: ref25 article-title: Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001 – volume: 102 start-page: 8776 year: 2005 ident: ref10 article-title: Visual memory needs categories. publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500810102 – volume: 82 start-page: 279 year: 1969 ident: ref27 article-title: J Exp Psychol – volume: 15 start-page: 106 year: 2004 ident: ref8 article-title: The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual information load and by number of objects. publication-title: Psychol Sci doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502006.x – volume: 26 start-page: 683 year: 2000 ident: ref12 article-title: Organization of visual short-term memory. publication-title: J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.683 – volume: 9 start-page: 46 year: 2005 ident: ref23 article-title: Two neural correlates of consciousness. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006 – volume: 44 start-page: 369 year: 1988 ident: ref5 article-title: Familiarity and visual change detection. publication-title: Percept Psychophys doi: 10.3758/BF03210419 – volume: 27 start-page: 92 year: 2001 ident: ref7 article-title: Storage of features, conjunctions and objects in visual working memory. publication-title: J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.92 – volume: 16 start-page: 283 year: 1974 ident: ref6 article-title: On the distinction between sensory storage and short-term visual memory. publication-title: Percept Psychophys doi: 10.3758/BF03203943 – volume: 12 start-page: 1127 year: 2005 ident: ref9 article-title: Visual working memory for simple and complex visual stimuli. publication-title: Psychon Bull Rev doi: 10.3758/BF03206454 – volume: 66 start-page: 446 year: 2004 ident: ref11 article-title: Perceptual grouping in change detection. publication-title: Percept Psychophys doi: 10.3758/BF03194892 – volume: 394 start-page: 784 year: 1998 ident: ref35 article-title: Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure and background by V1, V2 and V3 neurons. publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/29537 – volume: 24 start-page: 87 year: 2001 ident: ref21 article-title: The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. publication-title: Behav Brain Sci doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01003922 – volume: 292 start-page: 510 year: 2001 ident: ref34 article-title: Fast backprojections from the motion to the primary visual area necessary for visual awareness. publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.1057099 – volume: 30 start-page: 369 year: 1992 ident: ref4 article-title: Delayed matching-to-sample performance as a measure of human visuospatial working memory. publication-title: B Psychonomic Soc doi: 10.3758/BF03334092 – volume: 102 start-page: 10747 year: 2005 ident: ref33 article-title: Repression of unconscious information by conscious processing: evidence from affective blindsight induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500834102 – volume: 14 start-page: 1044 year: 2002 ident: ref32 article-title: Masking interrupts figure-ground signals in V1. publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci doi: 10.1162/089892902320474490 – volume: 15 start-page: 1176 year: 2003 ident: ref14 article-title: Orienting attention to locations in internal representations. publication-title: J Cogn Neurosci doi: 10.1162/089892903322598139 – volume: 43 start-page: 149 year: 2003 ident: ref15 article-title: Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. publication-title: Vision Res doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9 – volume: 11 start-page: 254 year: 2004 ident: ref39 article-title: The role of figure-ground segregation in change blindness. publication-title: Psychon Bull Rev doi: 10.3758/BF03196567 – ident: ref19 article-title: Orienting attention in visual working memory reduces interference from memory probes. publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition – volume: 69 start-page: 1422 year: 2007 ident: ref20 article-title: Attention effects during visual short-term memory maintenance: Protection or prioritization? publication-title: Percept Psychophys doi: 10.3758/BF03192957 – volume: 4 start-page: 304 year: 2001 ident: ref37 article-title: Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). publication-title: Nat Neurosci doi: 10.1038/85170 – volume: 390 start-page: 279 year: 1997 ident: ref3 article-title: The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/36846 – volume: 9 start-page: 50 year: 1981 ident: ref29 article-title: Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: an evaluation of the bar-probe task. publication-title: Mem Cognit doi: 10.3758/BF03196951 – volume: 14 start-page: 1072 year: 2007 ident: ref18 article-title: Distributing versus focusing attention in visual short-term memory publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03193093 – volume: 10 start-page: 204 year: 2006 ident: ref38 article-title: Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007 – volume: 7 start-page: 12 year: 2003 ident: ref24 article-title: Why visual attention and awareness are different. publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X – volume: 98 start-page: 113 year: 1973 ident: ref28 article-title: Loss of spatial and identity information following a tachistoscopic exposure. publication-title: J Exp Psychol doi: 10.1037/h0034309 – volume: 26 start-page: 733 year: 2005 ident: ref16 article-title: Directing spatial attention in mental representations: Interactions between attentional orienting and working-memory load. publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.026 – volume: 17 start-page: 2072 year: 2007 ident: ref17 article-title: Attentional modulation of object representations in working memory. publication-title: Cereb Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl116 – volume: 95 start-page: 3263 year: 1998 ident: ref36 article-title: Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex is suppressed by anesthesia. publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3263 – volume: 5 start-page: 94 year: 1996 ident: ref2 article-title: Integrating information across saccadic eye movements. publication-title: Curr Dir in Psychol Sci doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772833 – volume: 23 start-page: 571 year: 2000 ident: ref31 article-title: The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. publication-title: Trends Neurosci doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X – volume: 74 start-page: 1 year: 1960 ident: ref13 article-title: The information available in brief visual presentations. publication-title: Psychol monogr doi: 10.1037/h0093759 – volume: 27 start-page: 183 year: 1980 ident: ref26 article-title: Iconic memory and visible persistence. publication-title: Percept Psychophys doi: 10.3758/BF03204258 – volume: 23 start-page: 420 year: 1991 ident: ref1 article-title: Information integration across saccadic eye movements. publication-title: Cognit Psychol doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(91)90015-G – reference: 11248943 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001 Feb;27(1):92-114 – reference: 9723617 - Nature. 1998 Aug 20;394(6695):784-7 – reference: 16603406 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 May;10(5):204-11 – reference: 15260190 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2004 Apr;11(2):254-61 – reference: 5378047 - J Exp Psychol. 1969 Nov;82(2):279-84 – reference: 11074267 - Trends Neurosci. 2000 Nov;23(11):571-9 – reference: 4704202 - J Exp Psychol. 1973 Apr;98(1):113-8 – reference: 16030150 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jul 26;102(30):10747-51 – reference: 12536137 - Vision Res. 2003 Jan;43(2):149-64 – reference: 12419127 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2002 Oct 1;14(7):1044-53 – reference: 15937119 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 14;102(24):8776-80 – reference: 11224548 - Nat Neurosci. 2001 Mar;4(3):304-10 – reference: 12517353 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2003 Jan;7(1):12-18 – reference: 663675 - Science. 1978 Aug 11;201(4355):544-6 – reference: 11515286 - Behav Brain Sci. 2001 Feb;24(1):87-114; discussion 114-85 – reference: 18229477 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2007 Dec;14(6):1072-8 – reference: 16997611 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2006 Nov;10(11):494-501 – reference: 1884598 - Cogn Psychol. 1991 Jul;23(3):420-56 – reference: 14709235 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2003 Nov 15;15(8):1176-94 – reference: 18078232 - Percept Psychophys. 2007 Nov;69(8):1422-34 – reference: 7231170 - Mem Cognit. 1981 Jan;9(1):50-67 – reference: 3226885 - Percept Psychophys. 1988 Oct;44(4):369-78 – reference: 17099066 - Cereb Cortex. 2007 Sep;17(9):2072-83 – reference: 16615339 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2005 Dec;12(6):1127-33 – reference: 3441580 - Psychol Res. 1987;49(4):221-7 – reference: 10855426 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2000 May;26(3):683-702 – reference: 11313497 - Science. 2001 Apr 20;292(5516):510-2 – reference: 6992093 - Percept Psychophys. 1980 Mar;27(3):183-228 – reference: 15283069 - Percept Psychophys. 2004 Apr;66(3):446-53 – reference: 15668096 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Feb;9(2):46-52 – reference: 9384378 - Nature. 1997 Nov 20;390(6657):279-81 – reference: 9501251 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998 Mar 17;95(6):3263-8 – reference: 18315412 - J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008 Mar;34(2):369-80 – reference: 15955482 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jul 1;26(3):733-43 – reference: 14738517 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Feb;15(2):106-11 |
SSID | ssj0053866 |
Score | 2.4299033 |
Snippet | Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human observers are... Background Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human... BackgroundClassic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human... Background Classic work on visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that people store a limited amount of items for subsequent report. However, when human... |
SourceID | plos doaj pubmedcentral proquest gale pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | e1699 |
SubjectTerms | Afterimage - physiology Change detection Cues Data processing Eye (anatomy) Humans Information processing Localization Masks Memory Memory, Short-Term - physiology Neuroscience/Behavioral Neuroscience Neuroscience/Cognitive Neuroscience Neuroscience/Sensory Systems Neuroscience/Theoretical Neuroscience Neurosciences Observers Representations Retina Retina - physiology Sensory perception Short term memory Taxonomy Time Factors Transcranial magnetic stimulation Trends Visual observation Visual perception Visual Perception - physiology |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: Public Health Database dbid: 8C1 link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3db9MwELegvPCCGF_LNliEkICHbI3tOMnTVCamgVSQ6DbtzbIde5s0JaVpH_bfc-e4KUET8FKp8SVK7it38d3vCHmnuUor7MYxitkE_rhEmTRLjLGOG-F47hH4pt_E6Tn_epldhg9ubSirXPtE76irxuA38sOUpQg2XhTsaP4zwalRuLsaRmg8JI9glSJ2fnHcl3iALQsR2uVYnh4G6RzMm9oe-GDHI75uXkcetb_3zaP5bdPeF3j-WT_52wvp5Cl5EiLJeNKJfos8sPUzshVstY0_BEDpj88JmyxsDPoAv9NQPxhf3LQrOHt2DeF3cgbuOZ5ize1dPIMk3LZHL8j5yeez49MkDEtIjBB0mbgKUw2qdVmUGjFvIDIwRpRCF0yrMq-oYQqPj7lWY-qoKDXkSsJSSIIrnbGXZFQDY7ZJTLVTBS-5UyArnmfKibEzkPo4VwjGTUTYmmfSBCRxHGhxK_32WA4ZRccCiZyWgdMRSfqz5h2Sxj_oP6E4elrEwfYHmsWVDGYl4Z44BeUqSwehFbWlUo7blFuIenLH4CL7KEzZNZX21iwnPKciw9mZEXnrKRALo8Zimyu1alv55fvFfxDNfgyI3gci1wA7jAoNDvBMiLE1oNxG3Vo_dis3yh2RvbW-3b-83y-DH8DNHVXbZtVK8NwM4QEi8qpTzg2TC3DieZ5FJB-o7YCzw5X65tojjSPaIFx35-83tUsed1U02Oi1R0bLxcq-hlBtqd94e_wFwVY9nQ priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest – databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access dbid: M48 link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3fb9MwELZGeeEFMX4tMFiEkICHVI3j2PEDmgpiGkgDCSjam2W79japS0rTIvbfc5c4gaAieOCx8TlqPt_Zd_Ldd4Q8NUync6zGsTpzCfzwibZpnljrPLPcM9Ew8J2858cz9u40P90hXc_WAGC9NbTDflKz1WL8_evVIRj8y6Zrg0i7SeNlVbpx48RIeY1ch7NJYDOHE9bfK4B1cx4K6P40c3BANTz-_W49Wi6qepsr-ntG5S9H1NEtcjP4lvG0VYZdsuPK22Q3WG8dPw8U0y_ukGy6cjG6fi7uMgrjbxf1BmbX54BFght2fIlZuFcx5k-6-vAumR29-fz6OAntExLLOV0nfo7BBzVGFtIgCw74CtZyyU2RGS3FnNpM4_MJM3pCPeXSQPTEHYWweG7y7B4ZlQDMHomp8bpgknkNq8dErj2feAvBkPcFz5iNSNZhpmzgFscWFwvVXJgJiDFaCBQirQLSEUn6WcuWW-Mv8q9wOXpZZMZuHlSrMxUMTcF_YhTUTUoPzhZ1UmvPXMoc-EHCZ_CSA1xM1ZaZ9vatpkxQnmM3zYg8aSSQHaPE9Jszvalr9fbDl38Q-vRxIPQsCPkK4LA6lDzANyHr1kByD3Wr--waQEiRW78osojsd_q2ffigH4adAa97dOmqTa1gL8-QMCAi91vl_AlyAdu6EHlExEBtB8gOR8qL84Z7HPkH4b0P_sdSPCQ32uwbLBDbJ6P1auMegYu3No8bq_0BEvJRfQ priority: 102 providerName: Scholars Portal |
Title | Are There Multiple Visual Short-Term Memory Stores? |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18301775 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1312182883 https://www.proquest.com/docview/70334807 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC2246033 https://doaj.org/article/62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39 http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001699 |
Volume | 3 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3db9MwELegvPCCGF8LjC5CSMBDtsZ27OQJddPKQNpAg6G-WbZrs0kjrZYWif-eO8fNFjRpPPBiKfE5Su7O96Hc_UzIa8N1PsNuHKuZy-DCZ9rmRWat89wKz2VA4Ds6Foen_NO0mF476gtrwlp44JZxu4J6TmF5VXlwntRVWnvucu7Ar0nPQuse-Lx1MtXaYNjFQsRGOSbz3SiXncW8djshzAlYr1eOKOD1d1Z5sLiYNzeFnH9XTl5zRZOH5EGMIdNx--4b5I6rH5GNuEub9G2Ekn73mLDxpUsxxHPpunIw_XXerGB1cwaBd4aGOf2J1ba_U6yTdM37J-R0cvBt_zCLxyRkVgi6zPwMkwxqTFVWBtFuICawVlTClMzoSs6oZRrvj7jRI-qpqAxkScJRSH9npmBPyaAGxmySlBqvS15xr0FKXBbai5G3kPR4XwrGbULYmmfKRgxxPMriQoUfYxJyiZYFCjmtIqcTknWrFi2Gxi30eyiOjhYRsMMN0AsV9ULdphcJ2UZhqradtNvHaswlFQWempmQV4ECUTBqLLP5oVdNoz5-_v4PRF9PekRvIpGfAzusjq0N8E2IrtWj3ETdWn92A0zIEUO_LFlCttb6dvP0djcNFgB_6-jazVeNApvNEBggIc9a5bxicgnmW8oiIbKntj3O9mfq87OAMY44g_Dc5_9DFC_I_bbKBhvBtshgeblyLyGUW5ohuSunEsZyP8dx8mFI7u0dHH85GYb9DOMRL_8AmlRMGg |
linkProvider | Directory of Open Access Journals |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3db9MwELem7gFeEONrYYNGCAQ8ZGts10ke0FQGU8vWIdFu2pvnuPY2aUpK0wrtn-Jv5C5xUoom4GUvkRJfouR8vo_47neEvE65CidYjaMVMwGc2EDpsBtobSzXwvKoROAbHov-Cf9y1j1bIz_rWhhMq6x1YqmoJ7nGf-S7IQsRbDyO2d70e4Bdo3B3tW6hUYnFobn5ASFb8WHwCeb3DaUHn8f7_cB1FQi0EHQe2An65DRNkzhJERwGTKjWIhFpzFKVRBOqmcLrHZ6qDrVUJCkEFcJQiBYnKXaJAJW_zhmLEKs_3m9SSkB3COHK81gU7jpp2JnmmdkpnasSYXZp_souAY0taE2v8-I2R_fPfM3fDODBQ_LAea5-rxK1DbJmskdkw-mGwn_nAKzfPyasNzM-yB8chy5f0T-9KhZw9-gS3P1gDObAH2KO740_gqDfFHtPyMmdsPEpaWXAmE3i09SqmCfcKpANHnWVFR2rIdSyNhaMa4-wmmdSO-RybKBxLcvtuAgimIoFEjktHac9EjR3TSvkjn_Qf8TpaGgRd7u8kM8upFvGEt6JUxDmJLHgylGTKGW5CbkBLyuyDB7SxsmUVRFroz1kj0dUdLFXp0delRSIvZFhcs-FWhSFHHw9_Q-i0bcVoreOyObADq1cQQV8E2J6rVBuomzVn13I5WLyyHYtb7cPt5th0Du4maQyky8KCZaCIRyBR55VwrlkcgxGI4q6HolWxHaFs6sj2dVliWyO6Ibw3Od_f6k2udcfD4_k0eD4cIvcrzJ4sMhsm7Tms4V5AW7iPH1Zrk2fnN-1MvgFqRx5Bw |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3db9MwELemIiFeEONrgUEjBAIesjaxaycPaCqMamV0ILpNffMc194mTUlpWqH9a_x13CVOStEEvOwlUuJLlJzP9xHf_Y6QlylT4RSrcbSiJoATGygd9gKtjWWaWyZKBL7RId8_Zp8mvckG-VnXwmBaZa0TS0U9zTX-I--ENESw8TimHevSIr7uDXZn3wPsIIU7rXU7jUpEDszVDwjfinfDPZjrV1E0-Hj0YT9wHQYCzXm0COwU_fMoTZM4SREoBsyp1jzhaUxTlYhppKnC612Wqm5kI56kEGBwE0HkOE2xYwSo_1uCgtmEtSQmTbAHeoRzV6pHRdhxkrEzyzOzUzpaJdrsyhSWHQMau9CaXebFdU7vn7mbvxnDwT1y13mxfr8Su02yYbL7ZNPpicJ_48Cs3z4gtD83PsgiHEcud9E_uSiWcPf4HFz_4AhMgz_CfN8rf7zIIfrffUiOb4SNj0grA8ZsET9KrYpZwqwCOWGipyzvWg1hl7Uxp0x7hNY8k9qhmGMzjUtZbs0JiGYqFkjktHSc9kjQ3DWrUDz-Qf8ep6OhRQzu8kI-P5NuSUt4JxaBYCeJBbcuMolSlpmQGfC4hKXwkDZOpqwKWhtNIvtMRLyHfTs98qKkQByODCX6TC2LQg6_nPwH0fjbGtFrR2RzYIdWrrgCvgnxvdYot1C26s8u5GpheWS7lrfrh9vNMOgg3FhSmcmXhQSrQRGawCOPK-FcMTkGAyJEzyNiTWzXOLs-kl2clyjniHQIz33y95dqk9ugBuTn4eHBU3KnSubBerNt0lrMl-YZeIyL9Hm5NH1yetO64Be8RX1p |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Are+there+multiple+visual+short-term+memory+stores%3F&rft.jtitle=PloS+one&rft.au=Ilja+G+Sligte&rft.au=H+Steven+Scholte&rft.au=Victor+A+F+Lamme&rft.date=2008-02-27&rft.pub=Public+Library+of+Science+%28PLoS%29&rft.eissn=1932-6203&rft.volume=3&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=e1699&rft_id=info:doi/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0001699&rft.externalDBID=DOA&rft.externalDocID=oai_doaj_org_article_62f42ac199f6432e9aaf4e14e2237f39 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1932-6203&client=summon |