Optimality and stability of intentional and unintentional actions: II. Motor equivalence and structure of variance
We address the nature of unintentional changes in performance in two papers. This second paper tested hypotheses related to stability of task-specific performance variables estimated using the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Our first hypothesis was that selective stability...
Saved in:
Published in | Experimental brain research Vol. 235; no. 2; pp. 457 - 470 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Berlin/Heidelberg
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
01.02.2017
Springer Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | We address the nature of unintentional changes in performance in two papers. This second paper tested hypotheses related to stability of task-specific performance variables estimated using the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Our first hypothesis was that selective stability of performance variables would be observed even when the magnitudes of those variables drifted unintentionally because of the lack of visual feedback. Our second hypothesis was that stability of a variable would depend on the number of explicit task constraints. Subjects performed four-finger isometric pressing tasks that required the accurate production of a combination of total moment and total force with natural or modified finger involvement under full visual feedback, which was removed later for some or all of the salient variables. We used inter-trial analysis of variance and drifts in the space of finger forces within the UCM and within the orthogonal to the UCM space. The two variance components were used to estimate a synergy index stabilizing the force/moment combination, while the two drift components were used to estimate motor equivalent and non-motor equivalent force changes, respectively. Without visual feedback, both force and moment drifted toward lower absolute magnitudes. The non-motor equivalent component of motion in the finger force space was larger than the motor equivalent component for variables that stopped receiving visual feedback. In contrast, variables that continued to receive visual feedback showed larger motor equivalent component, compared to non-motor equivalent component, over the same time interval. These data falsified the first hypothesis; indeed, selective stabilization of a variable over the duration of a trial allows expecting comparably large motor equivalent components both with and without visual feedback. Adding a new constraint (presented as a target magnitude of middle finger force) resulted in a drop in the synergy index in support of the second hypothesis. We interpret the force drift as a natural relaxation process toward states with lower potential energy in the physical (physiological) system involved in the task. The results show that presenting sensory feedback on a performance variable makes synergies stabilizing that variable dependent on that particular sensory feedback. |
---|---|
AbstractList | We address the nature of unintentional changes in performance in two papers. This second paper tested hypotheses related to stability of task-specific performance variables estimated using the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Our first hypothesis was that selective stability of performance variables would be observed even when the magnitudes of those variables drifted unintentionally because of the lack of visual feedback. Our second hypothesis was that stability of a variable would depend on the number of explicit task constraints. Subjects performed four-finger isometric pressing tasks that required the accurate production of a combination of total moment and total force with natural or modified finger involvement under full visual feedback, which was removed later for some or all of the salient variables. We used inter-trial analysis of variance and drifts in the space of finger forces within the UCM and within the orthogonal to the UCM space. The two variance components were used to estimate a synergy index stabilizing the force/moment combination, while the two drift components were used to estimate motor equivalent and non-motor equivalent force changes, respectively. Without visual feedback, both force and moment drifted toward lower absolute magnitudes. The non-motor equivalent component of motion in the finger force space was larger than the motor equivalent component for variables that stopped receiving visual feedback. In contrast, variables that continued to receive visual feedback showed larger motor equivalent component, compared to non-motor equivalent component, over the same time interval. These data falsified the first hypothesis; indeed, selective stabilization of a variable over the duration of a trial allows expecting comparably large motor equivalent components both with and without visual feedback. Adding a new constraint (presented as a target magnitude of middle finger force) resulted in a drop in the synergy index in support of the second hypothesis. We interpret the force drift as a natural relaxation process toward states with lower potential energy in the physical (physiological) system involved in the task. The results show that presenting sensory feedback on a performance variable makes synergies stabilizing that variable dependent on that particular sensory feedback. We address the nature of unintentional changes in performance in two papers. This second paper tested hypotheses related to stability of task-specific performance variables estimated using the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Our first hypothesis was that selective stability of performance variables would be observed even when the magnitudes of those variables drifted unintentionally because of the lack of visual feedback. Our second hypothesis was that stability of a variable would depend on the number of explicit task constraints. Subjects performed four-finger isometric pressing tasks that required the accurate production of a combination of total moment and total force with natural or modified finger involvement under full visual feedback, which was removed later for some or all of the salient variables. We used inter-trial analysis of variance and drifts in the space of finger forces within the UCM and within the orthogonal to the UCM space. The two variance components were used to estimate a synergy index stabilizing the force/moment combination, while the two drift components were used to estimate motor equivalent and non-motor equivalent force changes, respectively. Without visual feedback, both force and moment drifted toward lower absolute magnitudes. The non-motor equivalent component of motion in the finger force space was larger that the motor equivalent component for variables that stopped receiving visual feedback. In contrast, variables that continued to receive visual feedback showed larger motor equivalent component, compared to non-motor equivalent component, over the same time interval. These data falsified the first hypothesis; indeed, selective stabilization of a variable over the duration of a trial allows expecting comparably large motor equivalent components both with and without visual feedback. Adding a new constraint (presented as a target magnitude of middle finger force) resulted in a drop in the synergy index in support of the second hypothesis. We interpret the force drift as a natural relaxation process toward states with lower potential energy in the physical (physiological) system involved in the task. The results show that presenting sensory feedback on a performance variable makes synergies stabilizing that variable dependent on that particular sensory feedback. We address the nature of unintentional changes in performance in two papers. This second paper tested hypotheses related to stability of task-specific performance variables estimated using the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Our first hypothesis was that selective stability of performance variables would be observed even when the magnitudes of those variables drifted unintentionally because of the lack of visual feedback. Our second hypothesis was that stability of a variable would depend on the number of explicit task constraints. Subjects performed four-finger isometric pressing tasks that required the accurate production of a combination of total moment and total force with natural or modified finger involvement under full visual feedback, which was removed later for some or all of the salient variables. We used inter-trial analysis of variance and drifts in the space of finger forces within the UCM and within the orthogonal to the UCM space. The two variance components were used to estimate a synergy index stabilizing the force/moment combination, while the two drift components were used to estimate motor equivalent and non-motor equivalent force changes, respectively. Without visual feedback, both force and moment drifted toward lower absolute magnitudes. The non-motor equivalent component of motion in the finger force space was larger than the motor equivalent component for variables that stopped receiving visual feedback. In contrast, variables that continued to receive visual feedback showed larger motor equivalent component, compared to non-motor equivalent component, over the same time interval. These data falsified the first hypothesis; indeed, selective stabilization of a variable over the duration of a trial allows expecting comparably large motor equivalent components both with and without visual feedback. Adding a new constraint (presented as a target magnitude of middle finger force) resulted in a drop in the synergy index in support of the second hypothesis. We interpret the force drift as a natural relaxation process toward states with lower potential energy in the physical (physiological) system involved in the task. The results show that presenting sensory feedback on a performance variable makes synergies stabilizing that variable dependent on that particular sensory feedback.We address the nature of unintentional changes in performance in two papers. This second paper tested hypotheses related to stability of task-specific performance variables estimated using the framework of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Our first hypothesis was that selective stability of performance variables would be observed even when the magnitudes of those variables drifted unintentionally because of the lack of visual feedback. Our second hypothesis was that stability of a variable would depend on the number of explicit task constraints. Subjects performed four-finger isometric pressing tasks that required the accurate production of a combination of total moment and total force with natural or modified finger involvement under full visual feedback, which was removed later for some or all of the salient variables. We used inter-trial analysis of variance and drifts in the space of finger forces within the UCM and within the orthogonal to the UCM space. The two variance components were used to estimate a synergy index stabilizing the force/moment combination, while the two drift components were used to estimate motor equivalent and non-motor equivalent force changes, respectively. Without visual feedback, both force and moment drifted toward lower absolute magnitudes. The non-motor equivalent component of motion in the finger force space was larger than the motor equivalent component for variables that stopped receiving visual feedback. In contrast, variables that continued to receive visual feedback showed larger motor equivalent component, compared to non-motor equivalent component, over the same time interval. These data falsified the first hypothesis; indeed, selective stabilization of a variable over the duration of a trial allows expecting comparably large motor equivalent components both with and without visual feedback. Adding a new constraint (presented as a target magnitude of middle finger force) resulted in a drop in the synergy index in support of the second hypothesis. We interpret the force drift as a natural relaxation process toward states with lower potential energy in the physical (physiological) system involved in the task. The results show that presenting sensory feedback on a performance variable makes synergies stabilizing that variable dependent on that particular sensory feedback. |
Audience | Academic |
Author | Parsa, Behnoosh Zatsiorsky, Vladimir M. Latash, Mark L. |
AuthorAffiliation | 1 Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University 2 Moscow Institute of Physics and Technology |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 2 Moscow Institute of Physics and Technology – name: 1 Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Behnoosh surname: Parsa fullname: Parsa, Behnoosh organization: Department of Kinesiology, Rec.Hall-268N, The Pennsylvania State University – sequence: 2 givenname: Vladimir M. surname: Zatsiorsky fullname: Zatsiorsky, Vladimir M. organization: Department of Kinesiology, Rec.Hall-268N, The Pennsylvania State University – sequence: 3 givenname: Mark L. surname: Latash fullname: Latash, Mark L. email: mll11@psu.edu organization: Department of Kinesiology, Rec.Hall-268N, The Pennsylvania State University, Moscow Institute of Physics and Technology |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27778048$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkl1v0zAUhi00xLrBD-AGRUJCcJFiO_7KLpCmaUCloUl8XFuu47SeUruznYr9-zltB80EiOQisc_zvjo-fk_AkfPOAPASwSmCkL-PEGKMSohYSQRkJX4CJohUuEQIsiMwgRCRXEH1MTiJ8WZYVhw-A8eYcy4gERMQrtfJrlRn012hXFPEpOZ2u_JtYV0yLlnvVLct9m60o4efeFbMZtPii08-FOa2txvVGafN3i30OvXBDG4bFazKlefgaau6aF7sv6fgx8fL7xefy6vrT7OL86tSs4qmUnPEa1K1iKF5RbgWBrGWUY4N4bSBAjV1S0hNYFvXPJ-lJsSghraCUSaIwdUp-LDzXffzlWl0bjyoTq5DPm-4k15ZOa44u5QLv5EUc0I5yQZv9wbB3_YmJrmyUZuuU874PkokmKggxYL_B1pRVuUHZfT1I_TG9yEPdGtIMWWciN_UIo9TWtf63KIeTOU5pQhWEFOYqekfqPw2ZmV1Dktr8_5I8G4kyEwyP9NC9THK2bevY_bNAbs0qkvL6Lt-e-tj8NXhoH9N-CFlGeA7QAcfYzCt1DapwSe3azuJoBzyLHd5ljnPcsizHO4QPVI-mP9Lg3eamFm3MOFgwH8V3QNnfgMh |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_019_05548_5 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_00721_9 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_23274_x crossref_primary_10_1123_mc_2020_0031 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4809_z crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00485_2017 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_4972_x |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.004 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.02.166 10.1007/s00221-014-4148-x 10.1007/s002210050738 10.1007/978-1-4939-1338-1_7 10.1007/s00221-010-2440-y 10.1038/29528 10.1007/s00221-007-1259-7 10.1007/s00221-014-4128-1 10.1093/brain/118.2.495 10.1152/jn.00681.2004 10.3200/JMBR.41.4.317-330 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.057174 10.1007/s00422-005-0548-0 10.1007/s004220100279 10.1007/s00221-012-3383-2 10.1123/mcj.14.3.294 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.054 10.1152/jn.00163.2011 10.1152/jn.00043.2012 10.1152/jn.00180.2016 10.1007/s00221-002-1081-1 10.1038/nn1010 10.1123/mcj.2.4.306 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.003 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333169.001.0001 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.075 10.1097/00003677-200201000-00006 10.1207/s15326969eco0704_5 10.1007/s002210000540 10.1007/978-1-4939-2736-4 10.1007/s002210100861 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2007.01.022 10.1162/neco.2008.01-08-698 10.1123/mcj.17.2.145 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.051 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.005 10.1007/s00221-011-2541-2 10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.37 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.12.079 10.1038/nn963 10.1123/mcj.11.3.276 10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4 10.1152/jn.2000.84.4.1708 10.1007/s00221-016-4757-7 10.1007/s00221-016-4809-z 10.3390/medicina46060054 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016 COPYRIGHT 2017 Springer Experimental Brain Research is a copyright of Springer, 2017. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016 – notice: COPYRIGHT 2017 Springer – notice: Experimental Brain Research is a copyright of Springer, 2017. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM ISR 0-V 3V. 7QP 7QR 7RV 7TK 7TM 7X7 7XB 88E 88G 88J 8AO 8FD 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABUWG AFKRA ALSLI AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO FR3 FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ K9. KB0 M0S M1P M2M M2R NAPCQ P64 PHGZM PHGZT PJZUB PKEHL POGQB PPXIY PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PRQQA PSYQQ Q9U RC3 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1007/s00221-016-4806-2 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Gale In Context Science ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】 ProQuest Central (Corporate) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Database Neurosciences Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) Psychology Database (Alumni) Social Science Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Pharma Collection Technology Research Database Hospital Premium Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Engineering Research Database Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Health & Medical Collection Medical Database Psychology Database Social Science Database Nursing & Allied Health Premium Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Basic Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Central Essentials Nucleic Acids Abstracts Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest Central China Health Research Premium Collection Health & Medical Research Collection Chemoreception Abstracts ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Premium ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest Social Science Journals ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source (Alumni) Engineering Research Database ProQuest One Academic Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection Technology Research Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest Pharma Collection ProQuest Central ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection Genetics Abstracts Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni) ProQuest Medical Library ProQuest Psychology Journals ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest One Psychology MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology Psychology |
EISSN | 1432-1106 |
EndPage | 470 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC5274574 4312388401 A551030250 27778048 10_1007_s00221_016_4806_2 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: National Institutes of Health grantid: NS035032; AR048563 funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000002 – fundername: NINDS NIH HHS grantid: R01 NS035032 – fundername: NIAMS NIH HHS grantid: R01 AR048563 |
GroupedDBID | --- -4W -56 -5G -BR -DZ -EM -XW -Y2 -~C -~X .55 .86 .GJ .VR 0-V 06C 06D 0R~ 0VY 199 1N0 1SB 2.D 203 28- 29G 29~ 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 2P1 2VQ 2~H 30V 36B 3O- 3SX 3V. 4.4 406 408 409 40D 40E 53G 5GY 5QI 5RE 5VS 67N 67Z 6NX 78A 7RV 7X7 88E 8AO 8FI 8FJ 8TC 8UJ 95- 95. 95~ 96X AAAVM AABHQ AACDK AAHNG AAIAL AAJBT AAJKR AANXM AANZL AARHV AARTL AASML AATNV AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYJJ AAYQN AAYTO AAYZH ABAKF ABBBX ABBXA ABDZT ABECU ABFTV ABHLI ABHQN ABIVO ABJNI ABJOX ABKCH ABKTR ABLJU ABMNI ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABQBU ABQSL ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTMW ABULA ABUWG ABWNU ABXPI ACAOD ACBXY ACDTI ACGFS ACHSB ACHXU ACIWK ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACNCT ACOKC ACOMO ACPIV ACPRK ACZOJ ADBBV ADHHG ADHIR ADIMF ADINQ ADKNI ADKPE ADRFC ADTPH ADURQ ADYFF ADYPR ADZKW AEBTG AEFIE AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEKMD AEMSY AENEX AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETLH AEVLU AEXYK AFBBN AFEXP AFFNX AFGCZ AFKRA AFLOW AFQWF AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGGDS AGJBK AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGRTI AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHAVH AHBYD AHKAY AHMBA AHSBF AHYZX AIAKS AIGIU AIIXL AILAN AITGF AJBLW AJRNO AJZVZ AKMHD ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ALWAN AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AMYQR AOCGG ARALO ARMRJ ASPBG AVWKF AXYYD AZFZN AZQEC B-. BA0 BBWZM BDATZ BENPR BGNMA BKEYQ BPHCQ BSONS BVXVI CAG CCPQU COF CS3 CSCUP DDRTE DL5 DNIVK DPUIP DU5 DWQXO EBD EBLON EBS EIOEI EJD EMB EMOBN EN4 EPAXT ESBYG EX3 FA8 FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FINBP FNLPD FRRFC FSGXE FWDCC FYUFA G-Y G-Z GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNUQQ GNWQR GQ6 GQ7 GQ8 GXS H13 HF~ HG5 HG6 HMCUK HMJXF HQYDN HRMNR HVGLF HZ~ I09 IAO IHE IHR IHW IJ- IKXTQ INH INR IPY ISR ITC ITM IWAJR IXC IZIGR IZQ I~X I~Z J-C J0Z JBSCW JCJTX JZLTJ KDC KOV KOW KPH L7B LAS LLZTM M1P M2M M2R M4Y MA- N2Q NAPCQ NB0 NDZJH NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O9- O93 O9G O9I O9J OAM OHT OVD P19 P2P PF- PKN PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ PT4 PT5 Q2X QOK QOR QOS R4E R89 R9I RHV RIG RNI ROL RPX RRX RSV RZK S16 S1Z S26 S27 S28 S3A S3B SAP SBL SBY SCLPG SDH SDM SHX SISQX SJYHP SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SV3 SZN T13 T16 TEORI TSG TSK TSV TUC U2A U9L UG4 UKHRP UOJIU UTJUX UZXMN VC2 VFIZW W23 W48 WH7 WJK WK6 WK8 WOW X7M YLTOR Z45 Z7R Z7U Z7W Z7X Z82 Z83 Z87 Z88 Z8M Z8O Z8Q Z8R Z8V Z8W Z91 Z92 ZGI ZMTXR ZOVNA ZXP ~EX ~KM AAPKM AAYXX ABBRH ABDBE ABFSG ACSTC ADHKG AEZWR AFDZB AFHIU AFOHR AGQPQ AHPBZ AHWEU AIXLP ATHPR AYFIA CITATION PHGZM PHGZT ABRTQ CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PJZUB PPXIY PRQQA AEIIB PMFND 7QP 7QR 7TK 7TM 7XB 8FD 8FK FR3 K9. P64 PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQUKI PRINS Q9U RC3 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c635t-c717943f161b347c8e16f6572e475d081d9f44940f997048944e1d5f865684e23 |
IEDL.DBID | 7X7 |
ISSN | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 17:32:04 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 18:06:08 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 11:56:27 EDT 2025 Sat Aug 23 14:39:45 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 17 21:36:29 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 10 20:32:38 EDT 2025 Fri Jun 27 04:36:26 EDT 2025 Thu May 22 21:18:56 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:53:46 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:44:33 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:11:41 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 21 02:37:24 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 2 |
Keywords | Motor equivalence Synergy Force Unintentional movement Finger |
Language | English |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c635t-c717943f161b347c8e16f6572e475d081d9f44940f997048944e1d5f865684e23 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/5274574 |
PMID | 27778048 |
PQID | 1865256748 |
PQPubID | 47176 |
PageCount | 14 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5274574 proquest_miscellaneous_1868305287 proquest_miscellaneous_1835633331 proquest_journals_1865256748 gale_infotracmisc_A551030250 gale_infotracacademiconefile_A551030250 gale_incontextgauss_ISR_A551030250 gale_healthsolutions_A551030250 pubmed_primary_27778048 crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s00221_016_4806_2 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4806_2 springer_journals_10_1007_s00221_016_4806_2 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2017-02-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2017-02-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2017 text: 2017-02-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Berlin/Heidelberg |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Berlin/Heidelberg – name: Germany – name: Heidelberg |
PublicationTitle | Experimental brain research |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Exp Brain Res |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Exp Brain Res |
PublicationYear | 2017 |
Publisher | Springer Berlin Heidelberg Springer Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer Berlin Heidelberg – name: Springer – name: Springer Nature B.V |
References | Feldman (CR10) 2015 Ivanenko, Poppele, Lacquaniti (CR14) 2004; 556 Olafsdottir, Yoshida, Zatsiorsky, Latash (CR33) 2005; 381 CR37 Martin, Scholz, Schöner (CR28) 2009; 21 Bernstein (CR4) 1947 Diedrichsen, Shadmehr, Ivry (CR9) 2010; 14 Bernstein (CR5) 1967 Latash, Shim, Smilga, Zatsiorsky (CR25) 2005; 92 Vaillancourt, Russell (CR52) 2002; 145 Scholz, Danion, Latash, Schöner (CR43) 2002; 86 Hammond (CR12) 1954; 127 CR2 Harris, Wolpert (CR13) 1998; 394 Gelfand, Latash (CR11) 1998; 2 Latash, Huang (CR21) 2015; 301 Latash, Scholz, Danion, Schöner (CR23) 2001; 141 Ranganathan, Newell (CR39) 2009; 41 Latash (CR17) 2008 Park, Wu, Lewis, Huang, Latash (CR35) 2012; 108 Scholz, Schöner, Latash (CR42) 2000; 135 DeWald, Pope, Given, Buchanan, Rymer (CR7) 1995; 118 Scholz, Dwight-Higgin, Lynch, Tseng, Martin, Schöner (CR44) 2011; 209 Latash, Scholz, Schöner (CR26) 2007; 11 Newell, Carlton (CR32) 1993; 14 Ting, Macpherson (CR50) 2005; 93 Slifkin, Vaillancourt, Newell (CR47) 2000; 84 Diedrichsen (CR8) 2007; 17 Park, Zatsiorsky, Latash (CR34) 2010; 207 Mattos, Kuhl, Scholz, Latash (CR30) 2013; 17 Latash (CR19) 2012; 217 Ranganathan, Newell (CR38) 2008; 186 d’Avella, Saltiel, Bizzi (CR6) 2003; 6 Latash, Scholz, Schöner (CR24) 2002; 30 Latash (CR20) 2016; 69 Latash, Zatsiorsky (CR22) 2016 Scholz, Schöner (CR40) 1999; 126 Scholz, Schöner (CR41) 2014; 826 Venkadesan, Guckenheimer, Valero-Cuevas (CR53) 2007; 40 Solnik, Pazin, Coelho, Rosenbaum, Scholz, Zatsiorsky, Latash (CR48) 2013; 225 Kugler, Turvey (CR16) 1987 Latash, Levin, Scholz, Schöner (CR27) 2010; 46 Mattos, Latash, Park, Kuhl, Scholz (CR29) 2011; 106 Jo, Maenza, Good, Huang, Park, Sainburg, Latash (CR15) 2016; 319 Schöner (CR45) 1995; 8 Parsa, O’Shea, Zatsiorsky, Latash (CR36) 2016; 116 Mattos, Schöner, Zatsiorsky, Latash (CR31) 2015; 233 Babinski (CR3) 1899; 7 Latash (CR18) 2010; 14 Todorov, Jordan (CR51) 2002; 5 Tatton, Bawa, Bruce, Lee (CR49) 1978; 4 Zhou, Zhang, Latash (CR54) 2015; 289 Ambike, Zatsiorsky, Latash (CR1) 2015; 233 Shemmel, Krutky, Perreault (CR46) 2010; 121 JP Scholz (4806_CR40) 1999; 126 H Olafsdottir (4806_CR33) 2005; 381 ML Latash (4806_CR20) 2016; 69 J Shemmel (4806_CR46) 2010; 121 S Solnik (4806_CR48) 2013; 225 D Mattos (4806_CR30) 2013; 17 S Ambike (4806_CR1) 2015; 233 ML Latash (4806_CR21) 2015; 301 YP Ivanenko (4806_CR14) 2004; 556 V Martin (4806_CR28) 2009; 21 J Park (4806_CR35) 2012; 108 ML Latash (4806_CR18) 2010; 14 ML Latash (4806_CR22) 2016 B Parsa (4806_CR36) 2016; 116 D Mattos (4806_CR29) 2011; 106 CM Harris (4806_CR13) 1998; 394 NA Bernstein (4806_CR4) 1947 IM Gelfand (4806_CR11) 1998; 2 ML Latash (4806_CR23) 2001; 141 F Babinski (4806_CR3) 1899; 7 R Ranganathan (4806_CR38) 2008; 186 PH Hammond (4806_CR12) 1954; 127 DE Vaillancourt (4806_CR52) 2002; 145 LH Ting (4806_CR50) 2005; 93 R Ranganathan (4806_CR39) 2009; 41 T Zhou (4806_CR54) 2015; 289 AG Feldman (4806_CR10) 2015 G Schöner (4806_CR45) 1995; 8 M Venkadesan (4806_CR53) 2007; 40 A d’Avella (4806_CR6) 2003; 6 ML Latash (4806_CR17) 2008 AB Slifkin (4806_CR47) 2000; 84 JP DeWald (4806_CR7) 1995; 118 4806_CR2 ML Latash (4806_CR25) 2005; 92 E Todorov (4806_CR51) 2002; 5 J Park (4806_CR34) 2010; 207 ML Latash (4806_CR24) 2002; 30 JP Scholz (4806_CR42) 2000; 135 WG Tatton (4806_CR49) 1978; 4 JP Scholz (4806_CR44) 2011; 209 KM Newell (4806_CR32) 1993; 14 NA Bernstein (4806_CR5) 1967 ML Latash (4806_CR27) 2010; 46 JP Scholz (4806_CR41) 2014; 826 JP Scholz (4806_CR43) 2002; 86 J Diedrichsen (4806_CR9) 2010; 14 HJ Jo (4806_CR15) 2016; 319 D Mattos (4806_CR31) 2015; 233 4806_CR37 PN Kugler (4806_CR16) 1987 ML Latash (4806_CR19) 2012; 217 ML Latash (4806_CR26) 2007; 11 J Diedrichsen (4806_CR8) 2007; 17 11713627 - Exp Brain Res. 2001 Nov;141(2):153-65 15342720 - J Neurophysiol. 2005 Jan;93(1):609-13 27193319 - J Neurophysiol. 2016 Aug 1;116(2):698-708 27785549 - Exp Brain Res. 2017 Feb;235(2):481-496 11918210 - Biol Cybern. 2002 Jan;86(1):29-39 11146817 - Exp Brain Res. 2000 Dec;135(3):382-404 20944446 - Medicina (Kaunas). 2010;46(6):382-92 22552184 - J Neurophysiol. 2012 Aug 1;108(3):915-24 19718817 - Neural Comput. 2009 May;21(5):1371-414 21287157 - Exp Brain Res. 2011 Mar;209(3):319-32 20434396 - Clin Neurophysiol. 2010 Oct;121(10):1680-9 9723616 - Nature. 1998 Aug 20;394(6695):780-4 25596318 - Neuroscience. 2015 Mar 19;289:181-93 12136377 - Exp Brain Res. 2002 Aug;145(3):275-85 26047732 - Neuroscience. 2015 Aug 20;301:39-48 26828408 - Neuroscience. 2016 Apr 5;319:194-205 17400231 - J Biomech. 2007;40(8):1653-61 14354685 - J Physiol. 1955 Feb 28;127(2):23-5P 12563264 - Nat Neurosci. 2003 Mar;6(3):300-8 15739110 - Biol Cybern. 2005 Mar;92(3):186-91 25344311 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Feb;233(2):487-502 2964505 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1988 Feb;14(1):37-44 17900901 - Curr Biol. 2007 Oct 9;17(19):1675-9 15882796 - Neurosci Lett. 2005 Jun 10-17;381(1-2):92-6 23370796 - Motor Control. 2013 Apr;17(2):145-75 18183373 - Exp Brain Res. 2008 Apr;186(4):561-70 10382616 - Exp Brain Res. 1999 Jun;126(3):289-306 7735890 - Brain. 1995 Apr;118 ( Pt 2):495-510 14724214 - J Physiol. 2004 Apr 1;556(Pt 1):267-82 11800496 - Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2002 Jan;30(1):26-31 25417192 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Mar;233(3):711-21 21676927 - J Neurophysiol. 2011 Sep;106(3):1424-36 17715460 - Motor Control. 2007 Jul;11(3):276-308 22246105 - Exp Brain Res. 2012 Mar;217(1):1-5 20702893 - Motor Control. 2010 Jul;14(3):294-322 12404008 - Nat Neurosci. 2002 Nov;5(11):1226-35 9758883 - Motor Control. 1998 Oct;2(4):306-13 27497717 - Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016 Oct;69:136-46 20005767 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2010 Jan;14(1):31-9 19508958 - J Mot Behav. 2009 Jul;41(4):317-30 25330887 - Adv Exp Med Biol. 2014;826:91-100 23288326 - Exp Brain Res. 2013 Mar;225(3):431-42 11024063 - J Neurophysiol. 2000 Oct;84(4):1708-18 27540726 - Exp Brain Res. 2016 Dec;234(12 ):3597-3611 |
References_xml | – volume: 14 start-page: 31 year: 2010 end-page: 39 ident: CR9 article-title: The coordination of movement: optimal feedback control and beyond publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.004 – volume: 121 start-page: 1680 year: 2010 end-page: 1689 ident: CR46 article-title: Stretch sensitive reflexes as an adaptive mechanism for maintaining limb stability publication-title: Clin Neurophysiol doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.02.166 – volume: 233 start-page: 711 year: 2015 end-page: 721 ident: CR1 article-title: Processes underlying unintentional finger force changes in the absence of visual feedback publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4148-x – volume: 126 start-page: 289 year: 1999 end-page: 306 ident: CR40 article-title: The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s002210050738 – volume: 826 start-page: 91 year: 2014 end-page: 100 ident: CR41 article-title: Use of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) approach to understand motor variability, motor equivalence, and self-motion publication-title: Adv Exp Med Biol doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1338-1_7 – volume: 207 start-page: 119 year: 2010 end-page: 132 ident: CR34 article-title: Optimality vs. variability: An example of multi-finger redundant tasks publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2440-y – volume: 394 start-page: 780 year: 1998 end-page: 784 ident: CR13 article-title: Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/29528 – volume: 186 start-page: 561 year: 2008 end-page: 570 ident: CR38 article-title: Motor synergies: feedback and error compensation within and between trials publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1259-7 – volume: 233 start-page: 487 year: 2015 end-page: 502 ident: CR31 article-title: Motor equivalence during accurate multi-finger force production publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4128-1 – volume: 118 start-page: 495 year: 1995 end-page: 510 ident: CR7 article-title: Abnormal muscle coactivation patterns during isometric torque generation at the elbow and shoulder in hemiparetic subjects publication-title: Brain doi: 10.1093/brain/118.2.495 – volume: 93 start-page: 609 year: 2005 end-page: 613 ident: CR50 article-title: A limited set of muscle synergies for force control during a postural task publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00681.2004 – volume: 7 start-page: 806 year: 1899 end-page: 816 ident: CR3 article-title: De l’asynergie cerebelleuse publication-title: Rev Neurol – volume: 41 start-page: 317 year: 2009 end-page: 330 ident: CR39 article-title: Influence of augmented feedback on coordination strategies publication-title: J Mot Behav doi: 10.3200/JMBR.41.4.317-330 – volume: 556 start-page: 267 year: 2004 end-page: 282 ident: CR14 article-title: Five basic muscle activation patterns account for muscle activity during human locomotion publication-title: J Physiol doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.057174 – volume: 92 start-page: 186 year: 2005 end-page: 191 ident: CR25 article-title: A central back-coupling hypothesis on the organization of motor synergies: a physical metaphor and a neural model publication-title: Biol Cybern doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0548-0 – volume: 86 start-page: 29 year: 2002 end-page: 39 ident: CR43 article-title: Understanding finger coordination through analysis of the structure of force variability publication-title: Biol Cybern doi: 10.1007/s004220100279 – volume: 225 start-page: 431 year: 2013 end-page: 442 ident: CR48 article-title: End-state comfort and joint configuration variance during reaching publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3383-2 – year: 1947 ident: CR4 publication-title: On the construction of movements – volume: 14 start-page: 294 year: 2010 end-page: 322 ident: CR18 article-title: Motor synergies and the equilibrium-point hypothesis publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.14.3.294 – volume: 84 start-page: 1708 year: 2000 end-page: 1718 ident: CR47 article-title: Intermittency in the control of continuous force production publication-title: J Neurophysiol – volume: 319 start-page: 194 year: 2016 end-page: 205 ident: CR15 article-title: Effects of unilateral stroke on multi-finger synergies and their feed-forward adjustments publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.054 – volume: 106 start-page: 1424 year: 2011 end-page: 1436 ident: CR29 article-title: Unpredictable elbow joint perturbation during reaching results in multijoint motor equivalence publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00163.2011 – volume: 108 start-page: 915 year: 2012 end-page: 924 ident: CR35 article-title: Changes in multi-finger interaction and coordination in Parkinson’s disease publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00043.2012 – volume: 116 start-page: 698 year: 2016 end-page: 708 ident: CR36 article-title: On the nature of unintentional action: a study of force/moment drifts during multi-finger tasks publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00180.2016 – volume: 145 start-page: 275 year: 2002 end-page: 285 ident: CR52 article-title: Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1081-1 – volume: 6 start-page: 300 year: 2003 end-page: 308 ident: CR6 article-title: Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a natural motor behavior publication-title: Nat Neurosci doi: 10.1038/nn1010 – volume: 46 start-page: 382 year: 2010 end-page: 392 ident: CR27 article-title: Motor control theories and their applications publication-title: Medicina – year: 1987 ident: CR16 publication-title: Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement – volume: 2 start-page: 306 year: 1998 end-page: 313 ident: CR11 article-title: On the problem of adequate language in movement science publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.2.4.306 – volume: 127 start-page: 23P year: 1954 end-page: 25P ident: CR12 article-title: Involuntary activity in biceps following the sudden application of velocity to the abducted forearm publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 381 start-page: 92 year: 2005 end-page: 96 ident: CR33 article-title: Anticipatory covariation of finger forces during self-paced and reaction time force production publication-title: Neurosci Lett doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.003 – year: 2008 ident: CR17 publication-title: Synergy doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333169.001.0001 – ident: CR2 – ident: CR37 – volume: 301 start-page: 39 year: 2015 end-page: 48 ident: CR21 article-title: Neural control of movement stability: lessons from studies of neurological patients publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.075 – year: 2016 ident: CR22 publication-title: Biomechanics and motor control: defining central concepts – volume: 30 start-page: 26 year: 2002 end-page: 31 ident: CR24 article-title: Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variability publication-title: Exerc Sport Sci Rev doi: 10.1097/00003677-200201000-00006 – volume: 8 start-page: 291 year: 1995 end-page: 314 ident: CR45 article-title: Recent developments and problems in human movement science and their conceptual implications publication-title: Ecol Psychol doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0704_5 – volume: 135 start-page: 382 year: 2000 end-page: 404 ident: CR42 article-title: Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s002210000540 – year: 2015 ident: CR10 publication-title: Referent control of action and perception: challenging conventional theories in behavioral science doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-2736-4 – volume: 141 start-page: 153 year: 2001 end-page: 165 ident: CR23 article-title: Structure of motor variability in marginally redundant multi-finger force production tasks publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s002210100861 – volume: 40 start-page: 1653 year: 2007 end-page: 1661 ident: CR53 article-title: Manipulating the edge of instability publication-title: J Biomech doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2007.01.022 – volume: 21 start-page: 1371 year: 2009 end-page: 1414 ident: CR28 article-title: Redundancy, self-motion, and motor control publication-title: Neural Comput doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.01-08-698 – volume: 17 start-page: 145 year: 2013 end-page: 175 ident: CR30 article-title: Motor equivalence (ME) during reaching: is ME observable at the muscle level? publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.17.2.145 – volume: 4 start-page: 229 year: 1978 end-page: 245 ident: CR49 article-title: Long loop reflexes in monkeys: an interpretive base for human reflexes publication-title: Prog Clin Neurophysiol – volume: 17 start-page: 1675 year: 2007 end-page: 1679 ident: CR8 article-title: Optimal task-dependent changes of bimanual feedback control and adaptation publication-title: Curr Biol doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.051 – volume: 69 start-page: 136 year: 2016 end-page: 146 ident: CR20 article-title: Towards physics of neural processes and behavior publication-title: Neurosci Biobehav Rev doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.005 – volume: 209 start-page: 319 year: 2011 end-page: 332 ident: CR44 article-title: Motor equivalence and self-motion induced by different movement speeds publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2541-2 – volume: 14 start-page: 37 year: 1993 end-page: 44 ident: CR32 article-title: Force variability in isometric responses publication-title: J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.37 – volume: 289 start-page: 181 year: 2015 end-page: 193 ident: CR54 article-title: Intentional and unintentional multi-joint movements: their nature and structure of variance publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.12.079 – volume: 5 start-page: 1226 year: 2002 end-page: 1235 ident: CR51 article-title: Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination publication-title: Nat Neurosci doi: 10.1038/nn963 – volume: 11 start-page: 276 year: 2007 end-page: 308 ident: CR26 article-title: Toward a new theory of motor synergies publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.11.3.276 – year: 1967 ident: CR5 publication-title: The co-ordination and regulation of movements – volume: 217 start-page: 1 year: 2012 end-page: 5 ident: CR19 article-title: The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy) publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4 – volume: 319 start-page: 194 year: 2016 ident: 4806_CR15 publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.054 – volume: 14 start-page: 294 year: 2010 ident: 4806_CR18 publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.14.3.294 – volume: 289 start-page: 181 year: 2015 ident: 4806_CR54 publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.12.079 – volume: 394 start-page: 780 year: 1998 ident: 4806_CR13 publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/29528 – volume: 126 start-page: 289 year: 1999 ident: 4806_CR40 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s002210050738 – volume: 127 start-page: 23P year: 1954 ident: 4806_CR12 publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 2 start-page: 306 year: 1998 ident: 4806_CR11 publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.2.4.306 – volume: 207 start-page: 119 year: 2010 ident: 4806_CR34 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2440-y – volume: 86 start-page: 29 year: 2002 ident: 4806_CR43 publication-title: Biol Cybern doi: 10.1007/s004220100279 – volume: 4 start-page: 229 year: 1978 ident: 4806_CR49 publication-title: Prog Clin Neurophysiol – volume: 141 start-page: 153 year: 2001 ident: 4806_CR23 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s002210100861 – volume: 233 start-page: 711 year: 2015 ident: 4806_CR1 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4148-x – volume: 69 start-page: 136 year: 2016 ident: 4806_CR20 publication-title: Neurosci Biobehav Rev doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.005 – volume: 14 start-page: 37 year: 1993 ident: 4806_CR32 publication-title: J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.37 – volume: 5 start-page: 1226 year: 2002 ident: 4806_CR51 publication-title: Nat Neurosci doi: 10.1038/nn963 – volume: 92 start-page: 186 year: 2005 ident: 4806_CR25 publication-title: Biol Cybern doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0548-0 – volume: 17 start-page: 145 year: 2013 ident: 4806_CR30 publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.17.2.145 – volume: 8 start-page: 291 year: 1995 ident: 4806_CR45 publication-title: Ecol Psychol doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0704_5 – volume: 84 start-page: 1708 year: 2000 ident: 4806_CR47 publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.4.1708 – volume: 17 start-page: 1675 year: 2007 ident: 4806_CR8 publication-title: Curr Biol doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.051 – volume: 381 start-page: 92 year: 2005 ident: 4806_CR33 publication-title: Neurosci Lett doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.003 – volume: 30 start-page: 26 year: 2002 ident: 4806_CR24 publication-title: Exerc Sport Sci Rev doi: 10.1097/00003677-200201000-00006 – volume-title: The co-ordination and regulation of movements year: 1967 ident: 4806_CR5 – volume: 41 start-page: 317 year: 2009 ident: 4806_CR39 publication-title: J Mot Behav doi: 10.3200/JMBR.41.4.317-330 – volume: 116 start-page: 698 year: 2016 ident: 4806_CR36 publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00180.2016 – volume: 40 start-page: 1653 year: 2007 ident: 4806_CR53 publication-title: J Biomech doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2007.01.022 – volume: 217 start-page: 1 year: 2012 ident: 4806_CR19 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4 – volume: 233 start-page: 487 year: 2015 ident: 4806_CR31 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4128-1 – volume-title: On the construction of movements year: 1947 ident: 4806_CR4 – volume: 145 start-page: 275 year: 2002 ident: 4806_CR52 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1081-1 – volume-title: Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement year: 1987 ident: 4806_CR16 – volume: 118 start-page: 495 year: 1995 ident: 4806_CR7 publication-title: Brain doi: 10.1093/brain/118.2.495 – volume: 556 start-page: 267 year: 2004 ident: 4806_CR14 publication-title: J Physiol doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.057174 – volume-title: Synergy year: 2008 ident: 4806_CR17 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333169.001.0001 – volume: 209 start-page: 319 year: 2011 ident: 4806_CR44 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2541-2 – volume: 186 start-page: 561 year: 2008 ident: 4806_CR38 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1259-7 – volume: 7 start-page: 806 year: 1899 ident: 4806_CR3 publication-title: Rev Neurol – volume: 14 start-page: 31 year: 2010 ident: 4806_CR9 publication-title: Trends Cogn Sci doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.004 – volume: 108 start-page: 915 year: 2012 ident: 4806_CR35 publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00043.2012 – volume-title: Referent control of action and perception: challenging conventional theories in behavioral science year: 2015 ident: 4806_CR10 doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-2736-4 – ident: 4806_CR2 doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4757-7 – ident: 4806_CR37 doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4809-z – volume: 106 start-page: 1424 year: 2011 ident: 4806_CR29 publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00163.2011 – volume: 21 start-page: 1371 year: 2009 ident: 4806_CR28 publication-title: Neural Comput doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.01-08-698 – volume: 826 start-page: 91 year: 2014 ident: 4806_CR41 publication-title: Adv Exp Med Biol doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1338-1_7 – volume: 225 start-page: 431 year: 2013 ident: 4806_CR48 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3383-2 – volume: 6 start-page: 300 year: 2003 ident: 4806_CR6 publication-title: Nat Neurosci doi: 10.1038/nn1010 – volume: 301 start-page: 39 year: 2015 ident: 4806_CR21 publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.075 – volume: 11 start-page: 276 year: 2007 ident: 4806_CR26 publication-title: Mot Control doi: 10.1123/mcj.11.3.276 – volume: 46 start-page: 382 year: 2010 ident: 4806_CR27 publication-title: Medicina doi: 10.3390/medicina46060054 – volume: 135 start-page: 382 year: 2000 ident: 4806_CR42 publication-title: Exp Brain Res doi: 10.1007/s002210000540 – volume: 121 start-page: 1680 year: 2010 ident: 4806_CR46 publication-title: Clin Neurophysiol doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.02.166 – volume: 93 start-page: 609 year: 2005 ident: 4806_CR50 publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.00681.2004 – volume-title: Biomechanics and motor control: defining central concepts year: 2016 ident: 4806_CR22 – reference: 21287157 - Exp Brain Res. 2011 Mar;209(3):319-32 – reference: 11918210 - Biol Cybern. 2002 Jan;86(1):29-39 – reference: 20944446 - Medicina (Kaunas). 2010;46(6):382-92 – reference: 11713627 - Exp Brain Res. 2001 Nov;141(2):153-65 – reference: 26828408 - Neuroscience. 2016 Apr 5;319:194-205 – reference: 27193319 - J Neurophysiol. 2016 Aug 1;116(2):698-708 – reference: 20005767 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2010 Jan;14(1):31-9 – reference: 9723616 - Nature. 1998 Aug 20;394(6695):780-4 – reference: 20434396 - Clin Neurophysiol. 2010 Oct;121(10):1680-9 – reference: 14724214 - J Physiol. 2004 Apr 1;556(Pt 1):267-82 – reference: 22246105 - Exp Brain Res. 2012 Mar;217(1):1-5 – reference: 11024063 - J Neurophysiol. 2000 Oct;84(4):1708-18 – reference: 27785549 - Exp Brain Res. 2017 Feb;235(2):481-496 – reference: 12563264 - Nat Neurosci. 2003 Mar;6(3):300-8 – reference: 26047732 - Neuroscience. 2015 Aug 20;301:39-48 – reference: 17400231 - J Biomech. 2007;40(8):1653-61 – reference: 11800496 - Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2002 Jan;30(1):26-31 – reference: 27540726 - Exp Brain Res. 2016 Dec;234(12 ):3597-3611 – reference: 17715460 - Motor Control. 2007 Jul;11(3):276-308 – reference: 14354685 - J Physiol. 1955 Feb 28;127(2):23-5P – reference: 22552184 - J Neurophysiol. 2012 Aug 1;108(3):915-24 – reference: 25417192 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Mar;233(3):711-21 – reference: 19718817 - Neural Comput. 2009 May;21(5):1371-414 – reference: 27497717 - Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016 Oct;69:136-46 – reference: 9758883 - Motor Control. 1998 Oct;2(4):306-13 – reference: 25330887 - Adv Exp Med Biol. 2014;826:91-100 – reference: 12136377 - Exp Brain Res. 2002 Aug;145(3):275-85 – reference: 21676927 - J Neurophysiol. 2011 Sep;106(3):1424-36 – reference: 17900901 - Curr Biol. 2007 Oct 9;17(19):1675-9 – reference: 23370796 - Motor Control. 2013 Apr;17(2):145-75 – reference: 20702893 - Motor Control. 2010 Jul;14(3):294-322 – reference: 12404008 - Nat Neurosci. 2002 Nov;5(11):1226-35 – reference: 18183373 - Exp Brain Res. 2008 Apr;186(4):561-70 – reference: 15739110 - Biol Cybern. 2005 Mar;92(3):186-91 – reference: 19508958 - J Mot Behav. 2009 Jul;41(4):317-30 – reference: 15342720 - J Neurophysiol. 2005 Jan;93(1):609-13 – reference: 15882796 - Neurosci Lett. 2005 Jun 10-17;381(1-2):92-6 – reference: 10382616 - Exp Brain Res. 1999 Jun;126(3):289-306 – reference: 11146817 - Exp Brain Res. 2000 Dec;135(3):382-404 – reference: 7735890 - Brain. 1995 Apr;118 ( Pt 2):495-510 – reference: 25596318 - Neuroscience. 2015 Mar 19;289:181-93 – reference: 25344311 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Feb;233(2):487-502 – reference: 23288326 - Exp Brain Res. 2013 Mar;225(3):431-42 – reference: 2964505 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1988 Feb;14(1):37-44 |
SSID | ssj0014370 |
Score | 2.2656434 |
Snippet | We address the nature of unintentional changes in performance in two papers. This second paper tested hypotheses related to stability of task-specific... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest gale pubmed crossref springer |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 457 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Analysis of Variance Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedicine Feedback, Sensory - physiology Female Fingers - physiology Humans Hypotheses Intention Male Movement - physiology Nervous system Neurology Neurosciences Psychomotor Performance - physiology Research Article Variables Young Adult |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: SpringerLink Journals (ICM) dbid: U2A link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Nb9QwELVQufSCoOUjUMAgBBIoKB_jOOG2qlp1kQoSsFJvUeLYpRJNoGmQ9t8z4zhWs2orscf1czZrj8dv4pkXxt5ArESuRRrSS9JCAIxT6jTRoayqWuWFbHJb5Xr8JTtawecTceLquPsp2306krSe2he70XZDoW8WQo5hMPrduwJDd8rjWiULf3QAqRzrTmJAYFxMR5nXXWK2GW265Ct70ma-5Mahqd2LDu-ze45E8sU46w_YHd3usN1FiwH0-Zq_5Tat0z4v32Hb3sWtd9nFV_QQ55Z686ptOFJDmxy75p3hpBzh8s9t49DOvrEFEP0nvlx-5Mcdhupc_xnO0FDJN7irkRbtcKHpan8xCCeLeshWhwc_9o9C99aFUCH5uAyVtKJxBqlgnYJUuY4zkwmZaJCiQQbRFAaggMgUhcT1XwDouBEmR2aYg07SR2yr7Vr9hHHIorQqaqgr7FKpqkauoSKT6TgyptZJwKJp-EvlJMnpzRi_Si-mbGespDQ0mrESu7z3XX6Pehy3gV_SnJZjSalfy-VCkI4gsb-AvbYIUsJoKdXmtBr6vlx-_zYDvXMg0-HtqcpVLuCfJPGsGXJvhsSlqubNk3GVzlX0ZYwDh7xTQh6wV76ZelL6W6u7gTCpyFL8xLdhshydN0bAAXs82qsfoURKEprCX5AzS_YAEhmft7RnP63YuEgkCAkB-zDZ_JVbv2ngn_4X-hnbTogs2Vz4PbaFxqqfI9W7rF_Ypf0PY51Ipw priority: 102 providerName: Springer Nature |
Title | Optimality and stability of intentional and unintentional actions: II. Motor equivalence and structure of variance |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00221-016-4806-2 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27778048 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1865256748 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1835633331 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1868305287 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC5274574 |
Volume | 235 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELagvfSCoOURKMUgBBIokDh2nHBBAW3pglpQYaXtKUocGyrRpG0apP33zHid0KzE7mWlzDgPezz-xh5_JuQ5D5VItIh8PCTN5xzilDJi2pdFUaoklVVid7keHsUHM_55LuZuwq11aZW9T7SOumoUzpG_DZNYwPAsefL-_MLHU6NwddUdoXGTbCJ1GVq1nA8BF0ABudyCEnKfw9DXr2oGlkSUMQykY5BAUM1G49Kqd742PK2mTq6sn9phaf82ueXwJM2WBnCH3ND1NtnJaoilzxb0BbUZnnbqfJtsDd5usUMuv4KzOLMonBZ1RQEl2jzZBW0MRRIJl4puhV09umL3QrTv6HT6hh42ELVTfdGdgs2im3B3Q1ra7lLj3f5API7GdZfM9ic_Ph747gAGXwEOufKVtPxxBlBhGXGpEh3GJhaSaS5FBWCiSg3nKQ9MmkpwBSnnOqyEgaaKE65ZdI9s1E2tHxDK4yAq0pKXBRQpVFEC7FCBiXUYGFNq5pGgr_5cOXZyPCTjdz7wKtsWyzEjDVsshyKvhiLnS2qOdcpPsE3z5e7SoVvnmUBKQQSCHnlmNZAUo8asm59F17b59PvxSOmlUzINvJ4q3CYG-Ejk0Rpp7o40odeqsbg3rtx5jTb_Z-MeeTqIsSRmwtW66VAnEnEEv3CdTpyAH4dg2CP3l_Y61BCTEjmn4AlyZMmDAvKNjyX16S_LOy6Y5EJyj7zubf7aq_-v4h-u_9BHZIshULJ58LtkA6xTPwaYd1Xu2b68RzazTydfJvD_YXL07Riuzlj2F5inUGc |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Zb9QwELaq8kBfELQcgUIN4pBAgcSx4wQJoRVQbWi3SNBK-5YmjtNWokm72YD2T_EbmXEOmpXYt-7jepzDM54jnvmGkOfcVSLQwrOxSZrNOcQpqce0LZMkVUEos8BUuU4O_PER_zoV0zXyp6uFwbTKTicaRZ2VCr-Rv3MDX4B5ljz4eHFpY9coPF3tWmg0YrGnF78hZKs-RJ-Bvy8Y2_1y-Glst10FbAXGdW4raUDRcnB1Uo9LFWjXz30hmeZSZGAhszDnPOROHoYS5DvkXLuZyOH-fsA1Ah2Ayr8BhtfBYE9O-wAPXA_ZlLy43OZgartTVMeAljKGgbsPIxDEs4EdXLYGV8zhcqrm0nmtMYO7t8mt1n-lo0bg7pA1XWySrVEBsfv5gr6kJqPUfKrfJBu9dl1skdk3UE7nxuunSZFR8EpNXu6CljlF0Io29d0M1sXgH1N7Ub2nUfSWTsp5OaP6sj6DPYJqqb0awuDWM41X-wXxPwrzXXJ0Lay5R9aLstAPCOW-4yVhytMEpiQqScHNUU7ua9fJ81Qzizjd8seqRUPHphw_4x7H2XAsxgw45FgMU173Uy4aKJBVxDvI07ipZu3VSDwSCGGIjqdFnhkKBOEoMMvnJKmrKo5-fB8QvWqJ8hIeTyVt0QS8JOJ2DSi3B5SgJdRwuBOuuNVSVfxvT1nkaT-MMzHzrtBljTSe8D34uato_ADsBgTfFrnfyGu_QkxKxLiCO8iBJPcEiG8-HCnOTg3OuWCSC8kt8qaT-SuP_r-Ff7j6RXfIzfHhZD_ejw72HpENhk6aycHfJusgqfoxuJjz9InZ15QcX7ci-QvOeIX5 |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Zb9QwELaqrYT6gqDlCBRqEIcECk0cO06QEFpoV11Kl6qlUt9C4thQiSbtHqD9a_w6ZpyDZiX2rfsYj7OOZzyHPfOZkGfcVyLSInDxkjSXc4hTsoBpV6ZppqJY5pGtcj0YhXsn_NOpOF0hf5paGEyrbHSiVdR5qXCPfNuPQgHmWfJo29RpEYc7g_cXly7eIIUnrc11GpWI7Ov5bwjfJu-GO8Dr54wNdr9-3HPrGwZcBYZ26ippAdIMuD1ZwKWKtB-aUEimuRQ5WMs8NpzH3DNxLEHWY861nwsDYwkjrhH0ANT_qsSoqEdWP-yODo_aMwweyKoAxucuB8PbnKl6FsKUMQzjQ2iBkJ51rOKibbhiHBcTNxdOb61RHNwiN2tvlvYr8btNVnSxTjb6BUTy53P6gtr8Urtxv07WWl073yDjL6Cqzm0MQNMip-Cj2izdOS0NRQiLOhHeNs6KzhNbiTF5S4fDN_SgnJZjqi9nZ7BiUEnVb0NQ3NlY49t-pWPcuNF3yMm1MOcu6RVloe8TykMvSOOMZyl0SVWagdOjPBNq3zMm08whXjP9iaqx0fGKjp9Ji-psOZZgPhxyLIEur9ouFxUwyDLiLeRpUtW2tkol6QsENEQ31CFPLQVCchQo3N_T2WSSDI-POkQvayJTwvBUWpdQwEciileHcrNDCTpDdZsb4UpqnTVJ_q0whzxpm7En5uEVupwhTSDCAH7-MpowAisCobhD7lXy2s4QkxIRr-AfZEeSWwJEO--2FGc_LOq5YJILyR3yupH5K0P_38Q_WP6hW-QGKJHk83C0_5CsMfTYbEL-JumBoOpH4G9Os8f1wqbk23Xrkr8yRYuU |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Optimality+and+stability+of+intentional+and+unintentional+actions%3A+II.+Motor+equivalence+and+structure+of+variance&rft.jtitle=Experimental+brain+research&rft.au=Parsa%2C+Behnoosh&rft.au=Zatsiorsky%2C+Vladimir+M&rft.au=Latash%2C+Mark+L&rft.date=2017-02-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft.volume=235&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=457&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs00221-016-4806-2&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK&rft.externalDocID=4312388401 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |