Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals
Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also capable of helping others altruistically, humans are considered unique in our voluntary helping and our variety of helping behaviors. Many still...
Saved in:
Published in | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS Vol. 109; no. 9; pp. 3588 - 3592 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
National Academy of Sciences
28.02.2012
National Acad Sciences |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also capable of helping others altruistically, humans are considered unique in our voluntary helping and our variety of helping behaviors. Many still believe that this is because only humans can understand others’ goals due to our unique "theory of mind" abilities, especially shared intentionality. However, we know little of the cognitive mechanisms underlying helping in nonhuman animals, especially if and how they understand others’ goals. The present study provides the empirical evidence for flexible targeted helping depending on conspecifics’ needs in chimpanzees. The subjects of this study selected an appropriate tool from a random set of seven objects to transfer to a conspecific partner confronted with differing tool-use situations, indicating that they understood what their partner needed. This targeted helping, (i.e., selecting the appropriate tool to transfer), was observed only when the helpers could visually assess their partner's situation. If visual access was obstructed, the chimpanzees still tried to help their partner upon request, but failed to select and donate the appropriate tool needed by their partner. These results suggest that the limitation in chimpanzees’ voluntary helping is not necessarily due to failure in understanding others’ goals. Chimpanzees can understand conspecifics’ goals and demonstrate cognitively advanced targeted helping as long as they are able to visually evaluate their conspecifics’ predicament. However, they will seldom help others without direct request for help. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also capable of helping others altruistically, humans are considered unique in our voluntary helping and our variety of helping behaviors. Many still believe that this is because only humans can understand others' goals due to our unique "theory of mind" abilities, especially shared intentionality. However, we know little of the cognitive mechanisms underlying helping in nonhuman animals, especially if and how they understand others' goals. The present study provides the empirical evidence for flexible targeted helping depending on conspecifics' needs in chimpanzees. The subjects of this study selected an appropriate tool from a random set of seven objects to transfer to a conspecific partner confronted with differing tool-use situations, indicating that they understood what their partner needed. This targeted helping, (i.e., selecting the appropriate tool to transfer), was observed only when the helpers could visually assess their partner's situation. If visual access was obstructed, the chimpanzees still tried to help their partner upon request, but failed to select and donate the appropriate tool needed by their partner. These results suggest that the limitation in chimpanzees' voluntary helping is not necessarily due to failure in understanding others' goals. Chimpanzees can understand conspecifics' goals and demonstrate cognitively advanced targeted helping as long as they are able to visually evaluate their conspecifics' predicament. However, they will seldom help others without direct request for help. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT] Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also capable of helping others altruistically, humans are considered unique in our voluntary helping and our variety of helping behaviors. Many still believe that this is because only humans can understand others' goals due to our unique "theory of mind" abilities, especially shared intentionality. However, we know little of the cognitive mechanisms underlying helping in nonhuman animals, especially if and how they understand others' goals. The present study provides the empirical evidence for flexible targeted helping depending on conspecifics' needs in chimpanzees. The subjects of this study selected an appropriate tool from a random set of seven objects to transfer to a conspecific partner confronted with differing tool-use situations, indicating that they understood what their partner needed. This targeted helping, (i.e., selecting the appropriate tool to transfer), was observed only when the helpers could visually assess their partner's situation. If visual access was obstructed, the chimpanzees still tried to help their partner upon request, but failed to select and donate the appropriate tool needed by their partner. These results suggest that the limitation in chimpanzees' voluntary helping is not necessarily due to failure in understanding others' goals. Chimpanzees can understand conspecifics' goals and demonstrate cognitively advanced targeted helping as long as they are able to visually evaluate their conspecifics' predicament. However, they will seldom help others without direct request for help.Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also capable of helping others altruistically, humans are considered unique in our voluntary helping and our variety of helping behaviors. Many still believe that this is because only humans can understand others' goals due to our unique "theory of mind" abilities, especially shared intentionality. However, we know little of the cognitive mechanisms underlying helping in nonhuman animals, especially if and how they understand others' goals. The present study provides the empirical evidence for flexible targeted helping depending on conspecifics' needs in chimpanzees. The subjects of this study selected an appropriate tool from a random set of seven objects to transfer to a conspecific partner confronted with differing tool-use situations, indicating that they understood what their partner needed. This targeted helping, (i.e., selecting the appropriate tool to transfer), was observed only when the helpers could visually assess their partner's situation. If visual access was obstructed, the chimpanzees still tried to help their partner upon request, but failed to select and donate the appropriate tool needed by their partner. These results suggest that the limitation in chimpanzees' voluntary helping is not necessarily due to failure in understanding others' goals. Chimpanzees can understand conspecifics' goals and demonstrate cognitively advanced targeted helping as long as they are able to visually evaluate their conspecifics' predicament. However, they will seldom help others without direct request for help. Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also capable of helping others altruistically, humans are considered unique in our voluntary helping and our variety of helping behaviors. Many still believe that this is because only humans can understand others’ goals due to our unique “theory of mind” abilities, especially shared intentionality. However, we know little of the cognitive mechanisms underlying helping in nonhuman animals, especially if and how they understand others’ goals. The present study provides the empirical evidence for flexible targeted helping depending on conspecifics’ needs in chimpanzees. The subjects of this study selected an appropriate tool from a random set of seven objects to transfer to a conspecific partner confronted with differing tool-use situations, indicating that they understood what their partner needed. This targeted helping, (i.e., selecting the appropriate tool to transfer), was observed only when the helpers could visually assess their partner's situation. If visual access was obstructed, the chimpanzees still tried to help their partner upon request, but failed to select and donate the appropriate tool needed by their partner. These results suggest that the limitation in chimpanzees’ voluntary helping is not necessarily due to failure in understanding others’ goals. Chimpanzees can understand conspecifics’ goals and demonstrate cognitively advanced targeted helping as long as they are able to visually evaluate their conspecifics’ predicament. However, they will seldom help others without direct request for help. Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also capable of helping others altruistically, humans are considered unique in our voluntary helping and our variety of helping behaviors. Many still believe that this is because only humans can understand others' goals due to our unique "theory of mind" abilities, especially shared intentionality. However, we know little of the cognitive mechanisms underlying helping in nonhuman animals, especially if and how they understand others' goals. The present study provides the empirical evidence for flexible targeted helping depending on conspecifics' needs in chimpanzees. The subjects of this study selected an appropriate tool from a random set of seven objects to transfer to a conspecific partner confronted with differing tooluse situations, indicating that they understood what their partner needed. This targeted helping, (i.e., selecting the appropriate tool to transfer), was observed only when the helpers could visually assess their partner's situation. If visual access was obstructed, the chimpanzees still tried to help their partner upon request, but failed to select and donate the appropriate tool needed by their partner. These results suggest that the limitation in chimpanzees' voluntary helping is not necessarily due to failure in understanding others' goals. Chimpanzees can understand conspecifics' goals and demonstrate cognitively advanced targeted helping as long as they are able to visually evaluate their conspecifics' predicament. However, they will seldom help others without direct request for help. |
Author | Humle, Tatyana Yamamoto, Shinya Tanaka, Masayuki |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 fullname: Yamamoto, Shinya – sequence: 2 fullname: Humle, Tatyana – sequence: 3 fullname: Tanaka, Masayuki |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22315399$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kr1uFDEUhS0URDaBmgoY0UCziX_GHrtBQiv-pEgUkIrC8niud72atQd7FpFUvAavx5PgySYbSJHGlnW-e3x8fY_QQYgBEHpK8AnBDTsdgsknhGDJSUOweoBmZSVzUSt8gGYY02Yua1ofoqOc1xhjxSV-hA4pZYQzpWbo22LlN4MJlwD5z6_flevhp297qEaTljBCV62gH3xYVq3J5RRDZUK1DR2kPJrQTUp0lY0hD2C98_bKZhlNnx-jh65s8OR6P0bn7999XXycn33-8Gnx9mxuBSPjXHDjcCMc67CoQXDetQ5EJx12FFPJaAOqpdIyQ2tpO6DCEEprxq3CLVDMjtGbne-wbTfQWQhjMr0ekt-YdKGj8fp_JfiVXsYfmlHFGamLwatrgxS_byGPeuOzhb43AeI2a0V5Q5qSrJCv7yWpkqL0ll6lenkHXcdtCqURxa94CcxUgZ7_G32f-eaDCnC6A2yKOSdwe4RgPY2AnkZA345AqeB3Kqwfzejj9Hbf31N3E2USbm9RWmnGpSzAsx2wzmNMe6ImHAulpi6-2OnORG2WyWd9_oViUpe5k6Rhkv0FeDDUiA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1907476116 crossref_primary_10_1002_evan_21501 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jebo_2012_12_014 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_73256_0 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_022_01662_0 crossref_primary_10_1111_eth_12944 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10329_019_00745_w crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0129726 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11211_012_0160_0 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_68159_3 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsbl_2020_0370 crossref_primary_10_1111_cdep_12529 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_021_01499_z crossref_primary_10_1038_nrn_2017_72 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10329_020_00805_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2020_01_021 crossref_primary_10_21706_KA_24_4_330 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_95682_4 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12922 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10539_022_09857_y crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_015_0872_2 crossref_primary_10_1038_s44159_023_00207_7 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0084299 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2024_105747 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1220806110 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2020_1473 crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_113011_143812 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2014_11_025 crossref_primary_10_1126_science_1220999 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1503 crossref_primary_10_1126_science_adq6217 crossref_primary_10_21706_KA_24_4_344 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_016_0994_1 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_024_01846_w crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_95727_8 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_017_1122_6 crossref_primary_10_1038_srep10218 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_017_02321_6 crossref_primary_10_21706_KA_24_4_342 crossref_primary_10_1111_brv_12342 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2017_09_006 crossref_primary_10_52732_BORX9839 crossref_primary_10_21706_KA_24_4_305 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10329_016_0512_8 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2018_1536 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_016_1056_4 crossref_primary_10_1038_srep23911 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2021_0310 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_579792 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_104144 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2014_1699 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0130682 crossref_primary_10_1111_eth_13420 crossref_primary_10_21706_KA_24_4_317 crossref_primary_10_3390_bs4020087 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_019_01263_4 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797614536402 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0196074 crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_3919946 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani13050774 crossref_primary_10_1111_eth_13426 crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_122216_011813 crossref_primary_10_21706_KA_24_4_283 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_013_0700_5 crossref_primary_10_1002_evan_21507 crossref_primary_10_1093_pq_pqw049 crossref_primary_10_1111_brv_12994 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_015_0905_x crossref_primary_10_1177_2059204318761622 crossref_primary_10_2502_janip_65_1_4 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2014_00443 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10539_017_9598_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_evolhumbehav_2018_07_004 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0068440 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1213173109 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2019_04_014 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1431 crossref_primary_10_1038_ncomms13915 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_isci_2022_103991 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_langsci_2018_04_001 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2019_0499 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2021_0093 crossref_primary_10_1111_jzo_12204 crossref_primary_10_1079_hai_2023_0001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2014_09_001 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_240632 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2016_09_008 crossref_primary_10_1142_S0219635215500120 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0103422 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1551 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_conb_2013_01_012 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2021_104424 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10329_019_00735_y crossref_primary_10_3389_frobt_2022_977303 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10539_023_09913_1 crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_5315 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_60307_9 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_018_1205_z crossref_primary_10_1007_s11245_016_9372_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2019_104715 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_718251 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2021_0109 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2017_10_016 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0253416 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsbl_2012_0554 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_020_01466_0 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_220194 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2012_08_009 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2023_1390 crossref_primary_10_21706_KA_24_4_356 crossref_primary_10_1111_jtsb_12065 crossref_primary_10_1075_is_16_3_01bra crossref_primary_10_1098_rsbl_2013_0009 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1700351114 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2021_104407 crossref_primary_10_2117_psysoc_2021_B020 crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_01385 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani11061526 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2014_00822 crossref_primary_10_1111_brv_13080 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1005833 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2018_10_015 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2017_08_019 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_evolhumbehav_2019_01_003 crossref_primary_10_1038_srep34783 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2012_10_006 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_201688 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2022_101176 crossref_primary_10_4000_primatologie_2781 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_15320_w crossref_primary_10_1002_wics_1631 crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2020_1820339 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1529 crossref_primary_10_54097_ehss_v8i_4258 |
Cites_doi | 10.1098/rspb.2003.2509 10.1073/pnas.0807060105 10.1007/s10071-009-0262-8 10.1126/science.1121448 10.1038/nature04243 10.1098/rspb.2005.3417 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.009 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.036 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.057 10.1075/is.10.2.04yam 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010 10.1073/pnas.1111088108 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.038 10.1098/rspb.2010.1735 10.1017/S0140525X00076536 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.040 10.1037/a0015838 10.1007/s10071-009-0276-2 10.1007/s10071-009-0218-z 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625 10.1371/journal.pone.0007416 10.1093/oso/9780195086218.001.0001 10.4159/9780674033177 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.034 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050184 10.1007/3-540-28277-7 10.1126/science.207.4430.543 10.1007/s10329-004-0085-9 10.1073/pnas.0710310104 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | copyright © 1993-2008 National Acadamy of Sciences of the United States of America Copyright National Academy of Sciences Feb 28, 2012 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: copyright © 1993-2008 National Acadamy of Sciences of the United States of America – notice: Copyright National Academy of Sciences Feb 28, 2012 |
DBID | FBQ AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 M7N P64 RC3 7S9 L.6 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1073/pnas.1108517109 |
DatabaseName | AGRIS CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Immunology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Virology and AIDS Abstracts Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Engineering Research Database AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Genetics Abstracts AGRICOLA AGRICOLA - Academic MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Virology and AIDS Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Technology Research Database Nucleic Acids Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Entomology Abstracts Genetics Abstracts Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Immunology Abstracts Engineering Research Database Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts AGRICOLA AGRICOLA - Academic MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | Virology and AIDS Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef AGRICOLA MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: FBQ name: AGRIS url: http://www.fao.org/agris/Centre.asp?Menu_1ID=DB&Menu_2ID=DB1&Language=EN&Content=http://www.fao.org/agris/search?Language=EN sourceTypes: Publisher |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Sciences (General) |
EISSN | 1091-6490 |
EndPage | 3592 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC3295314 2603681201 22315399 10_1073_pnas_1108517109 109_9_3588 41506994 US201400081738 |
Genre | Video-Audio Media Comparative Study Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Feature |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .55 .GJ 0R~ 123 29P 2AX 2FS 2WC 3O- 4.4 53G 5RE 5VS 692 6TJ 79B 85S AACGO AAFWJ AANCE AAYJJ ABBHK ABOCM ABPLY ABPPZ ABPTK ABTLG ABZEH ACGOD ACIWK ACKIV ACNCT ACPRK ADULT ADZLD AENEX AEUPB AEXZC AFDAS AFFNX AFOSN AFRAH ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ASUFR AS~ BKOMP CS3 D0L DCCCD DIK DNJUQ DOOOF DU5 DWIUU E3Z EBS EJD F20 F5P FBQ FRP GX1 HGD HH5 HQ3 HTVGU HYE JAAYA JBMMH JENOY JHFFW JKQEH JLS JLXEF JPM JSG JSODD JST KQ8 L7B LU7 MVM N9A NEJ NHB N~3 O9- OK1 P-O PNE PQQKQ R.V RHF RHI RNA RNS RPM RXW SA0 SJN TAE TN5 UKR VOH VQA W8F WH7 WHG WOQ WOW X7M XFK XSW Y6R YBH YKV YSK ZA5 ZCA ZCG ~02 ~KM ABXSQ ACHIC ADQXQ ADXHL AQVQM H13 IPSME - 02 0R 1AW 55 AAPBV ABFLS ADACO AJYGW DZ KM PQEST X XHC AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 M7N P64 RC3 7S9 L.6 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c631t-65af076f3d064e655dbfe6d8f0f2028327e9b28c3a248cde26a122435c90be203 |
ISSN | 0027-8424 1091-6490 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:04:17 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 04:21:31 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 03:25:16 EDT 2025 Mon Jun 30 08:33:38 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:05:35 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:09:17 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:39:12 EDT 2025 Wed Nov 11 00:29:45 EST 2020 Thu May 29 08:40:43 EDT 2025 Wed Dec 27 18:34:41 EST 2023 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 9 |
Language | English |
License | Freely available online through the PNAS open access option. |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c631t-65af076f3d064e655dbfe6d8f0f2028327e9b28c3a248cde26a122435c90be203 |
Notes | SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 14 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-2 ObjectType-Undefined-3 Author contributions: S.Y., T.H., and M.T. designed research; S.Y. performed research; S.Y. analyzed data; and S.Y., T.H., and M.T. wrote the paper. Edited by Frans B. M. de Waal, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, and approved January 5, 2012 (received for review June 1, 2011) |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/109/9/3588.full.pdf |
PMID | 22315399 |
PQID | 926556039 |
PQPubID | 42026 |
PageCount | 5 |
ParticipantIDs | pnas_primary_109_9_3588 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1108517109 proquest_miscellaneous_2986231220 jstor_primary_41506994 fao_agris_US201400081738 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3295314 proquest_journals_926556039 crossref_citationtrail_10_1073_pnas_1108517109 pubmed_primary_22315399 proquest_miscellaneous_925717655 |
ProviderPackageCode | RNA PNE CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2012-02-28 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2012-02-28 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2012 text: 2012-02-28 day: 28 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: Washington |
PublicationTitle | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A |
PublicationYear | 2012 |
Publisher | National Academy of Sciences National Acad Sciences |
Publisher_xml | – name: National Academy of Sciences – name: National Acad Sciences |
References | The Animal Care Committee (e_1_3_3_32_2) 2002 e_1_3_3_17_2 e_1_3_3_19_2 e_1_3_3_18_2 e_1_3_3_13_2 e_1_3_3_12_2 e_1_3_3_15_2 e_1_3_3_14_2 e_1_3_3_11_2 e_1_3_3_30_2 e_1_3_3_10_2 e_1_3_3_31_2 e_1_3_3_6_2 e_1_3_3_5_2 Dugatkin LA (e_1_3_3_2_2) 1997 e_1_3_3_8_2 e_1_3_3_7_2 e_1_3_3_28_2 e_1_3_3_9_2 e_1_3_3_27_2 e_1_3_3_29_2 e_1_3_3_24_2 e_1_3_3_23_2 e_1_3_3_26_2 e_1_3_3_25_2 e_1_3_3_20_2 e_1_3_3_1_2 Goodall J (e_1_3_3_16_2) 1986 e_1_3_3_4_2 e_1_3_3_22_2 e_1_3_3_3_2 e_1_3_3_21_2 |
References_xml | – ident: e_1_3_3_4_2 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2509 – ident: e_1_3_3_5_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0807060105 – ident: e_1_3_3_7_2 doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0262-8 – ident: e_1_3_3_10_2 doi: 10.1126/science.1121448 – ident: e_1_3_3_18_2 doi: 10.1038/nature04243 – volume-title: The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior year: 1986 ident: e_1_3_3_16_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_19_2 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3417 – volume-title: Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Primates year: 2002 ident: e_1_3_3_32_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_17_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.009 – ident: e_1_3_3_20_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.036 – ident: e_1_3_3_30_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x – ident: e_1_3_3_6_2 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.057 – ident: e_1_3_3_26_2 doi: 10.1075/is.10.2.04yam – ident: e_1_3_3_27_2 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010 – ident: e_1_3_3_14_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1111088108 – ident: e_1_3_3_9_2 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.038 – ident: e_1_3_3_13_2 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1735 – ident: e_1_3_3_28_2 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076536 – ident: e_1_3_3_31_2 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.040 – ident: e_1_3_3_24_2 doi: 10.1037/a0015838 – ident: e_1_3_3_22_2 doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0276-2 – ident: e_1_3_3_25_2 doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0218-z – ident: e_1_3_3_15_2 doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625 – ident: e_1_3_3_12_2 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007416 – volume-title: Cooperation Among Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective year: 1997 ident: e_1_3_3_2_2 doi: 10.1093/oso/9780195086218.001.0001 – ident: e_1_3_3_1_2 doi: 10.4159/9780674033177 – ident: e_1_3_3_21_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.034 – ident: e_1_3_3_11_2 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050184 – ident: e_1_3_3_3_2 doi: 10.1007/3-540-28277-7 – ident: e_1_3_3_29_2 doi: 10.1126/science.207.4430.543 – ident: e_1_3_3_23_2 doi: 10.1007/s10329-004-0085-9 – ident: e_1_3_3_8_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710310104 |
SSID | ssj0009580 |
Score | 2.4546897 |
Snippet | Humans extensively help others altruistically, which plays an important role in maintaining cooperative societies. Although some nonhuman animals are also... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref pnas jstor fao |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 3588 |
SubjectTerms | Altruism Animal cognition Animals Biological altruism Biological Sciences Chimpanzees collective action Comprehension Conspecifics Empathy Evolution Female Goals Helping Behavior Humans Learning Legal objections Male Monkeys & apes Objectives Pan troglodytes Pan troglodytes - psychology Primates Research universities Social Sciences Straw Theory of Mind Tool Use Behavior Vision, Ocular |
Title | Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals |
URI | https://www.jstor.org/stable/41506994 http://www.pnas.org/content/109/9/3588.abstract https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22315399 https://www.proquest.com/docview/926556039 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2986231220 https://www.proquest.com/docview/925717655 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC3295314 |
Volume | 109 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Pb9MwFLe6ceGCGDAWBshIHIaqlNT56-M0barQKJOWSkUcLDt11mqdO5FWojvxNfh6fBKenThJxyYNLlFkuy-p389-z877PSP0nmSZR0QcuUmS-bBAiQKXBrnnBtqc-GEmMsPi_zyMBqPg0zgcdzo_WlFLq6XoZTd38kr-R6tQBnrVLNl_0GwtFArgHvQLV9AwXB-k46OpJjmqGwljvQpaoN1cp7jUfKgyyBscyqmcG1aUtliTrgk_NsffNqQWE1uuNOuyjByqhV0s-LxoO7BntcErbHjB0O4nHjbslGrKKLpu92zYnHX8lV9xwIbZnj2fztSat2BVBjanfLnmqi5P4f6Sl6yigq9Xl7P2PoUO-Kh535Y3APYwKBnT9dzr0RbIaPe654egZD8sD8irZlVd1rLQtvav2R-mK31kseKFYTeE_djK38izPfzCTkanpyw9Hqdb6BGBBYYJCR200zUnJXmpemebFCr2P94Sv-HPbOV8YQNbdbZcaHrXyuV2AG7Lo0mfoifVUgQflrjaQR2pnqEdqzl8UGUk__AcfWsB7ffPX9hCDFuI4Qpi2EAMLxTmCm9ADC9y3IaYFmPA9QKNTo7To4FbncrhZpHfX7pRyHMvjnJ_AgNbRmE4EbmMJknu5cQcfBVLKgiMfE6CJJtIEnH99RbGPfWEJJ6_i7bVQsk9hD0RcyE8GeQRDXgueSh45PngEotMJIQ7qGf7lmVVynp9csqcmdCJ2Ge6h1mjDAcd1D-4LrO13N90D5TF-AXYUjY6J3qnQfvHsZ84aNdosBYR6DSclAYOemmkNKIpo0yj00H7VsusmiAKRgn0DvwfeNa7uhZmb_1Jjiu5WBWM0ATWH9A_noPwPW0oWNV-DKL04w1s6hcA376vU0s7KN4AVN1AJ4_frFGzqUki7xMK5jd49YBX20ePmwH9Gm0vv6_kG3DFl-KtGTV_AJf13OM |
linkProvider | ABC ChemistRy |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Chimpanzees%E2%80%99+flexible+targeted+helping+based+on+an+understanding+of+conspecifics%E2%80%99+goals&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+National+Academy+of+Sciences+-+PNAS&rft.au=Yamamoto%2C+Shinya&rft.au=Humle%2C+Tatyana&rft.au=Tanaka%2C+Masayuki&rft.date=2012-02-28&rft.issn=0027-8424&rft.volume=109&rft.issue=9+p.3588-3592&rft.spage=3588&rft.epage=3592&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073%2Fpnas.1108517109&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_m | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/image/custom?url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.pnas.org%2Fcontent%2F109%2F9.cover.gif |
thumbnail_s | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/image/custom?url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.pnas.org%2Fcontent%2F109%2F9.cover.gif |