Sociality genes are associated with human-directed social behaviour in golden and Labrador retriever dogs
Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the pres...
Saved in:
Published in | PeerJ (San Francisco, CA) Vol. 6; p. e5889 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
PeerJ. Ltd
06.11.2018
PeerJ, Inc PeerJ Inc |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection.
Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing.
There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (
= 4.389,
= 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (
= 6.330,
= 0.003) and Labradors (
= 5.209,
= 0.025).
Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. |
---|---|
AbstractList | BACKGROUNDDogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. METHODSDogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. RESULTSThere was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F 2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F 2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F 1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). DISCUSSIONOur results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F.sub.2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F.sub.2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F.sub.1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner ( F 2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers ( F 2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors ( F 1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F.sub.2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F.sub.2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F.sub.1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner ( = 4.389, = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers ( = 6.330, = 0.003) and Labradors ( = 5.209, = 0.025). Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. Background: Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et A, 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods: Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results: There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP I was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F-2,F-56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F-2,F-55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F-1,F-93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion: Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication. |
ArticleNumber | e5889 |
Audience | Academic |
Author | Sundman, Ann-Sofie Jensen, Per Halldén, Lise-Lotte Trottier, Agaia J Persson, Mia E |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Mia E orcidid: 0000-0002-6115-7517 surname: Persson fullname: Persson, Mia E organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden – sequence: 2 givenname: Ann-Sofie surname: Sundman fullname: Sundman, Ann-Sofie organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden – sequence: 3 givenname: Lise-Lotte surname: Halldén fullname: Halldén, Lise-Lotte organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden – sequence: 4 givenname: Agaia J surname: Trottier fullname: Trottier, Agaia J organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden – sequence: 5 givenname: Per surname: Jensen fullname: Jensen, Per organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30416887$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed https://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-153536$$DView record from Swedish Publication Index |
BookMark | eNptkl2L3CAUhkPZ0t1u96Y_oAiFUgqZxhij3iwM26-FgV7041aMniQOGZ1qMsv--5qZ7XamVC-Uc57zqsf3eXbmvIMse4mLBWOYvd8ChPWCci6eZBclrlnOCRVnR_vz7CrGdZEGL-uCk2fZOSkqXHPOLjL7zWurBjveow4cRKQCIBXjHB3BoDs79qifNsrlxgbQc2yfHFADvdpZPwVkHer8YMAh5QxaqSYo4wMKMAYLOwjI-C6-yJ62aohw9bBeZj8-ffx-8yVfff18e7Nc5TpdbsyZILyoBKkNb01NeFMzwitDTVtQQSllhHBBNK1EiQuoRNGkNK9KIjiHplXkMrs96Bqv1nIb7EaFe-mVlfuAD51UYbR6AGk0I0y3RGlFKqMbJRpTNNiohjRCcEha-UEr3sF2ak7UPtify73aYCeJKaGkTvz1gU_wBowGNwY1nJSdZpztZed3si5LyglLAm8fBIL_NUEc5cZGDcOgHPgpyhKTshSlwDihr_9B1-kvXGrtTBUJYzX9S3UqPdi61qdz9Swql7TGZUXr_b0X_6HSNLCxOhmutSl-UvDmqKAHNYx99MM0Wu_iKfjuAOrgYwzQPjYDF3I2sNwbWM4GTvCr4_Y9on_sSn4DQoHsZg |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2020_1791682 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani12212895 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_021_01501_8 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2019_104860 crossref_primary_10_3390_genes12060832 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_41849_0 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.09.023 10.1111/gbb.12194 10.1016/J.Anbehav.2011.07.039 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005 10.1371/journal.pone.0006584 10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.173 10.1126/science.1261022 10.4088/JCP.10m06491 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.12.017 10.1007/s10071-011-0446-x 10.1371/journal.pone.0065296 10.1038/srep46636 10.2527/jas.2007-0616 10.1126/sciadv.1700398 10.1038/sj.mp.4001586 10.1371/journal.pone.0083993 10.1038/Nature04338 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01582 10.4236/ojas.2013.31008 10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.122 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0489 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.012 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.055 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00093-2 10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.219 10.1177/0963721416657050 10.1101/gr.3147604 10.1016/J.Anbehav.2005.03.025 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.07.016 10.1007/s00439-015-1585-y 10.1002/jeab.191 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003 10.1007/S10071-013-0627-X 10.1002/ajmg.b.31073 10.1007/s10071-017-1093-7 10.1111/gbb.12317 10.2174/1570159x13666150612225836 10.1007/s100710000072 10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.017 10.1186/s12864-018-4916-2 10.1093/jhered/esy012 10.1038/srep33439 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | COPYRIGHT 2018 PeerJ. Ltd. 2018 Persson et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. 2018 Persson et al. 2018 Persson et al. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: COPYRIGHT 2018 PeerJ. Ltd. – notice: 2018 Persson et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. – notice: 2018 Persson et al. 2018 Persson et al. |
DBID | NPM AAYXX CITATION 3V. 7XB 88I 8FE 8FH 8FK ABUWG AFKRA AZQEC BBNVY BENPR BHPHI CCPQU DWQXO GNUQQ HCIFZ LK8 M2P M7P PIMPY PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS Q9U 7X8 5PM ABXSW ADTPV AOWAS D8T DG8 ZZAVC DOA |
DOI | 10.7717/peerj.5889 |
DatabaseName | PubMed CrossRef ProQuest Central (Corporate) ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Science Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Natural Science Collection ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central ProQuest Central Essentials Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central Natural Science Collection ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Central Student SciTech Premium Collection Biological Sciences Science Database Biological Science Database Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central Basic MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) SWEPUB Linköpings universitet full text SwePub SwePub Articles SWEPUB Freely available online SWEPUB Linköpings universitet SwePub Articles full text Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | PubMed CrossRef Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Biological Science Collection ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Science Journals ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) SciTech Premium Collection ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Natural Science Collection Biological Science Database ProQuest SciTech Collection ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition Natural Science Collection ProQuest Central Korea Biological Science Collection ProQuest One Academic ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef PubMed Publicly Available Content Database |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Zoology |
EISSN | 2167-8359 |
EndPage | e5889 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_dc737cf3aca34dcba9bd0b1dab3b998e oai_DiVA_org_liu_153536 A561245636 10_7717_peerj_5889 30416887 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Advanced Research Grant from the European Research Council (ERC), project 322206 “GENEWELL” |
GroupedDBID | 3V. 53G 5VS 88I 8FE 8FH AAFWJ ABUWG ADBBV ADRAZ AENEX AFKRA AFPKN ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS AZQEC BAWUL BBNVY BCNDV BENPR BHPHI BPHCQ CCPQU DIK DWQXO ECGQY GNUQQ GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 H13 HCIFZ HYE IAO IEA IHR IHW ITC KQ8 LK8 M2P M48 M7P M~E NPM OK1 PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC RPM W2D YAO AAYXX CITATION 7XB 8FK PQEST PQUKI PRINS Q9U 7X8 5PM ABXSW ADTPV AOWAS D8T DG8 ZZAVC |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c608t-793804936d8fd638b67384d5df059555733893c549210e490b3848423988ebfa3 |
IEDL.DBID | RPM |
ISSN | 2167-8359 |
IngestDate | Tue Oct 22 15:15:05 EDT 2024 Sat Aug 24 00:48:40 EDT 2024 Tue Sep 17 20:46:39 EDT 2024 Sat Aug 17 03:32:31 EDT 2024 Thu Oct 10 18:15:39 EDT 2024 Wed Aug 14 18:52:12 EDT 2024 Tue Aug 13 05:21:16 EDT 2024 Tue Aug 20 22:14:31 EDT 2024 Fri Aug 23 02:11:00 EDT 2024 Sat Sep 28 08:22:29 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | Dog genetics Behavior genetics Dog behavior Golden retrievers Human-dog communication Labrador retrievers Domestication Genetics Human-directed social behavior Wolf |
Language | English |
License | This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c608t-793804936d8fd638b67384d5df059555733893c549210e490b3848423988ebfa3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-6115-7517 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6225837/ |
PMID | 30416887 |
PQID | 2130229765 |
PQPubID | 2045935 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_dc737cf3aca34dcba9bd0b1dab3b998e swepub_primary_oai_DiVA_org_liu_153536 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6225837 proquest_miscellaneous_2132292911 proquest_journals_2130229765 gale_infotracmisc_A561245636 gale_infotracacademiconefile_A561245636 gale_healthsolutions_A561245636 crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_5889 pubmed_primary_30416887 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2018-11-06 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2018-11-06 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 11 year: 2018 text: 2018-11-06 day: 06 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: San Diego – name: San Diego, USA |
PublicationTitle | PeerJ (San Francisco, CA) |
PublicationTitleAlternate | PeerJ |
PublicationYear | 2018 |
Publisher | PeerJ. Ltd PeerJ, Inc PeerJ Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: PeerJ. Ltd – name: PeerJ, Inc – name: PeerJ Inc |
References | Brubaker (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-2) 2017; 20 Sim (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-31) 2012; 73 Topal (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-36) 2005; 70 Nagasawa (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-23) 2015; 348 Bentosela (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-1) 2016; 105 Sutter (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-35) 2004; 14 Sundman (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-33) 2018; 201 Range (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-29) 2015; 5 Wilsson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-41) 1998; 58 Kis (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-12) 2014; 9 Caniglia (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-3) 2018; 19 Miklósi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-21) 2013; 17 Marshall-Pescini (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-17) 2017; 7 Persson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-25) 2015; 14 Miklosi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-20) 2000; 3 Sanders (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-30) 2005; 10 Miklósi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-19) 2003; 13 Persson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-26) 2017; 95 Persson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-27) 2016; 6 Horn (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-10) 2013; 8 Soproni (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-32) 2001; 115 Udell (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-39) 2014; 89 Jensen (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-11) 2016; 25 Heberlein (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-8) 2016; 122 VonHoldt (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-42) 2017; 3 Konno (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-13) 2018; 109 Lakatos (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-14) 2012; 15 Lindblad-Toh (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-15) 2005; 438 Mas (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-18) 2010; 153B Muller (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-22) 2015; 25 Sundman (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-34) 2016; 15 Topal (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-37) 1998; 112 Udell (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-38) 2015; 11 Hare (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-6) 1999; 113 Van Der Waaij (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-40) 2008; 86 Qayyum (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-28) 2015; 13 Passalacqua (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-24) 2011; 82 Chapman (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-4) 2015; 134 Marshall-Pescini (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-16) 2013; 16 Hare (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-7) 2005; 9 Gacsi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-5) 2009; 4 Hori (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-9) 2013; 3 Xu (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-43) 2013; 145 |
References_xml | – volume: 122 start-page: 59 year: 2016 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-8 article-title: A comparison between wolves, Canis lupus, and dogs, Canis familiaris, in showing behaviour towards humans publication-title: Animal Behaviour doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.09.023 contributor: fullname: Heberlein – volume: 14 start-page: 337 issue: 4 year: 2015 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-25 article-title: Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability publication-title: Genes Brain and Behavior doi: 10.1111/gbb.12194 contributor: fullname: Persson – volume: 82 start-page: 1043 issue: 5 year: 2011 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-24 article-title: Human-directed gazing behaviour in puppies and adult dogs, Canis lupus familiaris publication-title: Animal Behaviour doi: 10.1016/J.Anbehav.2011.07.039 contributor: fullname: Passalacqua – volume: 17 start-page: 287 issue: 6 year: 2013 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-21 article-title: What does it take to become ‘best friends’? Evolutionary changes in canine social competence publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005 contributor: fullname: Miklósi – volume: 4 start-page: e6584 issue: 8 year: 2009 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-5 article-title: Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills publication-title: PLOS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006584 contributor: fullname: Gacsi – volume: 113 start-page: 173 issue: 12 year: 1999 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-6 article-title: Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.173 contributor: fullname: Hare – volume: 348 start-page: 333 issue: 6232 year: 2015 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-23 article-title: Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.1261022 contributor: fullname: Nagasawa – volume: 73 start-page: 320 issue: 3 year: 2012 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-31 article-title: ARVCF genetic influences on neurocognitive and neuroanatomical intermediate phenotypes in Chinese patients with schizophrenia publication-title: Journal of Clinical Psychiatry doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06491 contributor: fullname: Sim – volume: 201 start-page: 46 year: 2018 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-33 article-title: Understanding of human referential gestures is not correlated to human-directed social behaviour in Labrador retrievers and German shepherd dogs publication-title: Applied Animal Behaviour Science doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.12.017 contributor: fullname: Sundman – volume: 15 start-page: 201 issue: 2 year: 2012 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-14 article-title: Comprehension and utilisation of pointing gestures and gazing in dog–human communication in relatively complex situations publication-title: Animal Cognition doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0446-x contributor: fullname: Lakatos – volume: 8 start-page: e65296 issue: 5 year: 2013 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-10 article-title: The importance of the secure base effect for domestic dogs—evidence from a manipulative problem-solving task publication-title: PLOS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065296 contributor: fullname: Horn – volume: 7 start-page: 46636 issue: 1 year: 2017 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-17 article-title: The role of domestication and experience in ‘looking back’ towards humans in an unsolvable task publication-title: Scientific Report doi: 10.1038/srep46636 contributor: fullname: Marshall-Pescini – volume: 86 start-page: 2853 issue: 11 year: 2008 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-40 article-title: Genetic analysis of results of a Swedish behavior test on German shepherd dogs and labrador retrievers publication-title: Journal of Animal Science doi: 10.2527/jas.2007-0616 contributor: fullname: Van Der Waaij – volume: 3 start-page: e1700398 issue: 7 year: 2017 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-42 article-title: Structural variants in genes associated with human Williams-Beuren syndrome underlie stereotypical hypersociability in domestic dogs publication-title: Science Advances doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700398 contributor: fullname: VonHoldt – volume: 10 start-page: 353 issue: 4 year: 2005 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-30 article-title: Haplotypic association spanning the 22q11.21 genes COMT and ARVCF with schizophrenia publication-title: Molecular Psychiatry doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001586 contributor: fullname: Sanders – volume: 9 start-page: e83993 issue: 1 year: 2014 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-12 article-title: Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris) publication-title: PLOS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083993 contributor: fullname: Kis – volume: 438 start-page: 803 issue: 7069 year: 2005 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-15 article-title: Genome sequence, comparative analysis and haplotype structure of the domestic dog publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/Nature04338 contributor: fullname: Lindblad-Toh – volume: 13 start-page: 763 issue: 9 year: 2003 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-19 article-title: A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X contributor: fullname: Miklósi – volume: 5 start-page: 1582 year: 2015 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-29 article-title: Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: the “Canine Cooperation Hypothesis” publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01582 contributor: fullname: Range – volume: 3 start-page: 54 issue: 1 year: 2013 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-9 article-title: Dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) is associated with gazing toward humans in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) publication-title: Open Journal of Animal Sciences doi: 10.4236/ojas.2013.31008 contributor: fullname: Hori – volume: 115 start-page: 122 issue: 2 year: 2001 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-32 article-title: Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris) publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.122 contributor: fullname: Soproni – volume: 11 start-page: 20150489 issue: 9 year: 2015 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-38 article-title: When dogs look back: inhibition of independent problem-solving behaviour in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) compared with wolves (Canis lupus) publication-title: Biology Letters doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0489 contributor: fullname: Udell – volume: 89 start-page: 99 year: 2014 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-39 article-title: Exploring breed differences in dogs (Canis familiaris): does exaggeration or inhibition of predatory response predict performance on human-guided tasks? publication-title: Animal Behaviour doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.012 contributor: fullname: Udell – volume: 25 start-page: 601 issue: 5 year: 2015 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-22 article-title: Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.055 contributor: fullname: Muller – volume: 58 start-page: 151 issue: 1–2 year: 1998 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-41 article-title: Behaviour test for eight-week old puppies—heritabilities of tested behaviour traits and its correspondence to later behaviour publication-title: Applied Animal Behaviour Science doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00093-2 contributor: fullname: Wilsson – volume: 112 start-page: 219 issue: 3 year: 1998 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-37 article-title: Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation Test publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.219 contributor: fullname: Topal – volume: 25 start-page: 334 issue: 5 year: 2016 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-11 article-title: The genetics of how dogs became our social allies publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0963721416657050 contributor: fullname: Jensen – volume: 14 start-page: 2388 issue: 12 year: 2004 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-35 article-title: Extensive and breed-specific linkage disequilibrium in Canis familiaris publication-title: Genome Research doi: 10.1101/gr.3147604 contributor: fullname: Sutter – volume: 70 start-page: 1367 issue: 6 year: 2005 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-36 article-title: Attachment to humans: a comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies publication-title: Animal Behaviour doi: 10.1016/J.Anbehav.2005.03.025 contributor: fullname: Topal – volume: 95 start-page: 85 year: 2017 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-26 article-title: Intranasal oxytocin and a polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene are associated with human-directed social behavior in golden retriever dogs publication-title: Hormones and Behavior doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.07.016 contributor: fullname: Persson – volume: 134 start-page: 1055 issue: 10 year: 2015 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-4 article-title: Whole exome sequencing in extended families with autism spectrum disorder implicates four candidate genes publication-title: Human Genetics doi: 10.1007/s00439-015-1585-y contributor: fullname: Chapman – volume: 105 start-page: 68 issue: 1 year: 2016 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-1 article-title: Sociability and gazing toward humans in dogs and wolves: simple behaviors with broad implications publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior doi: 10.1002/jeab.191 contributor: fullname: Bentosela – volume: 9 start-page: 439 issue: 9 year: 2005 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-7 article-title: Human-like social skills in dogs? publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003 contributor: fullname: Hare – volume: 16 start-page: 933 issue: 6 year: 2013 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-16 article-title: Gaze alternation in dogs and toddlers in an unsolvable task: evidence of an audience effect publication-title: Animal Cognition doi: 10.1007/S10071-013-0627-X contributor: fullname: Marshall-Pescini – volume: 153B start-page: 1052 issue: 5 year: 2010 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-18 article-title: A functional variant provided further evidence for the association of ARVCF with schizophrenia publication-title: American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31073 contributor: fullname: Mas – volume: 20 start-page: 717 issue: 4 year: 2017 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-2 article-title: Differences in problem-solving between canid populations: do domestication and lifetime experience affect persistence? publication-title: Animal Cognition doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1093-7 contributor: fullname: Brubaker – volume: 15 start-page: 750 issue: 8 year: 2016 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-34 article-title: Similar recent selection criteria associated with different behavioural effects in two dog breeds publication-title: Genes Brain and Behavior doi: 10.1111/gbb.12317 contributor: fullname: Sundman – volume: 13 start-page: 802 issue: 6 year: 2015 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-28 article-title: The role of the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) geneval158met in aggressive behavior, a review of genetic studies publication-title: Current Neuropharmacology doi: 10.2174/1570159x13666150612225836 contributor: fullname: Qayyum – volume: 3 start-page: 159 issue: 3 year: 2000 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-20 article-title: Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: an experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog publication-title: Animal Cognition doi: 10.1007/s100710000072 contributor: fullname: Miklosi – volume: 145 start-page: 95 issue: 1 year: 2013 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-43 article-title: Polymorphisms in seizure 6-like gene are associated with bipolar disorder I: evidence of gene x gender interaction publication-title: Journal of Affective Disorders doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.017 contributor: fullname: Xu – volume: 19 start-page: 533 issue: 1 year: 2018 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-3 article-title: Wolf outside, dog inside? The genomic make-up of the Czechoslovakian Wolfdog publication-title: BMC Genomics doi: 10.1186/s12864-018-4916-2 contributor: fullname: Caniglia – volume: 109 start-page: 566 issue: 5 year: 2018 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-13 article-title: Effect of canine oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism on the successful training of drug detection dogs publication-title: Journal of Heredity doi: 10.1093/jhered/esy012 contributor: fullname: Konno – volume: 6 start-page: 33439 issue: 1 year: 2016 ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-27 article-title: Genomic regions associated with interspecies communication in dogs contain genes related to human social disorders publication-title: Scientific Report doi: 10.1038/srep33439 contributor: fullname: Persson |
SSID | ssj0000826083 |
Score | 2.2438996 |
Snippet | Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26... Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on... Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on... BACKGROUNDDogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome... Background: Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on... |
SourceID | doaj swepub pubmedcentral proquest gale crossref pubmed |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | e5889 |
SubjectTerms | Alleles Analysis Animal Behavior Animal cognition Behavior Canidae Cooperation Dog behavior Dog genetics Dogs Domestication Ethics Gene frequency Gene polymorphism Genes Genetic aspects Genetic diversity Genetic engineering Genetics Genomes Genomics Golden retrievers Human-directed social behavior Human-dog communication Hypotheses Pedigree Principal components analysis Schizophrenia Single-nucleotide polymorphism Social behavior Social behavior in animals Social skills Studies Wolves Zoology |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Lb9QwELZQD4gL4k2ggBEITqHZdeI4x-VRVYhyoqg3y_ZMtkFVUqW7_58ZO11t4MCFazyWkhnPzDex_Y0Qb00NDnSLuVbQ5GXdhtxhVefQIKBG7yG2bzv9rk_Oyq_n1fleqy8-E5bogZPijiDUqg6tcsGpEoJ3jYfCL8B55alUwBh9i2avmIoxmFAzgYvER1pTyXJ0hTj--lAZ7ue-l4EiUf_f4XgvH_15VnLGKBqz0PE9cXeCj3KVXvu-uIX9A3H7dNogfyi6dN2WoLVccxSTbkTpJhMgSP7tKmNfvjwlM3qWfpvL6cL-dpRdL9fDJQUk6XqQ3xwV1DCMcozNt2jpSxjW14_E2fGXH59O8qmbQh5IFZucHNFQOaA0mBbI6zz3-yyhgpYQVlUxLyJhl8CMbYsCy6bwNGwiP6BB3zr1WBz0Q49PhTTQqMZ5xNqrkmzsyyIoHTiwLloKEpl4c6Nhe5VIMywVG2wHG-1g2Q6Z-MjK30kw0XV8QOa3k_ntv8yfiVdsOptuje7c1a646yeBQ6Uz8T5KsMOSBYOb7h3QpzD11UzycCZJjhbmwzfLw06Ofm2XvPG7JExXZeL1bphn8uG1HodtlCGRJaWVTDxJq2n30aogREyBPhP1bJ3NtDIf6buLSAOuKRQbRTPfpRU5m_K5-7mKirzstpayWqX0s_-h7-fiDkFGE29j6kNxsBm3-IJg2ca_jB74GzGJO2M priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – databaseName: ProQuest Central dbid: BENPR link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfR1db9Mw0IJOQrwgxtcCYxiB4CksrRMneULd2DQhNiHE0N4s2-eUoCkpafv_uXPcbAGJV_ssJfd9Z98dY2-LHDTIysVSQBmneWVj7bI8htKBk84Y8OPbzi_k2WX6-Sq7Cgm3VXhWudWJXlFDaylHfjijG7YZGs_s4_J3TFOj6HY1jNC4y3ZmGCkkE7ZzdHLx9duQZUEDJ9HJ6PuS5hi6HC6d6359yAqa637LEvmG_f-q5Vt26e83k6POot4anT5kD4Ibyec93XfZHdc8YvfOw0X5Y1b3ZbfoYvMFaTOuO8d1IIUDTulX7ufzxb1Rw7U-fc5D4f6m43XDF-01KiauG-BfNAbW0Ha880O4UAQ4tIvVE3Z5evL9-CwOUxVii6hYxyiQBYYFQkJRAUqfobmfKWRQoaeVZdQfEX0YS53bpolLy8TgduH7BBbOVFo8ZZOmbdwe4wWUotTGudyIFGlt0sQKaUnBTitUFhF7s8WwWvbNMxQGHUQH5emgiA4ROyLkDxDU8NovtN1CBflRYHOR20poq0UK1ujSQGKmoI0wGDG6iL0i0qm-enQQWzWn6Z_oJAoZsfceggQXKWh1qD_AX6EWWCPI_REkCpwdb2_ZQwWBX6kb9ozY62GbTtIjtsa1Gw-DIDM0LxF71nPT8NMiQc8YFX7E8hGfjbAy3mnqn74duESVXAg8-a7nyNGRT_WPuUfkdb1RaN0yIZ____tfsPvoFBa-3lLus8m627iX6HitzUGQrj8bzjGP priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest – databaseName: Scholars Portal Open Access Journals dbid: M48 link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lj9MwEB4ti4S4IN4EFjACwSmlrRPHOaHyWK0Qy4mivVm2xylBVbJkWwn-PTNOWpEFcY3HUjLPb2LPDMALXaBFVYVUSSzTrKh8akNepFgGDCo4h3F82-lndbLMPp7lZwewm985MPDin6kdz5NaduvJzx-_3pDBE36dFJSNvD4Pofs-ybUur8DVeUYZOl_hG2B-9MiEoQlq9N1JL20ZxaPYtv9v5_xHdLp8c3LUXzTGpOObcGMAk2LRS_8WHITmNlw7HY7L70DdF98S0BYr9mnCdkHYQSABBf-EFXFKX9qHNnrW_0QXQ_n-thN1I1btmtyTsA2KT5bSa2w70cVRXGQIAtvVxV1YHn_48u4kHWYrpJ5YsUnJLDUlB1KhrpBs0PH0zwxzrAhv5Tl3SSQk47l_22wasnLqaFnHboE6uMrKe3DYtE14AEJjKUvrQiiczEjiLpt6qTy72VlFLiOB5zsOm_O-hYah1IPlYKIcDMshgbfM_D0Ft72OD9puZQYrMugLWfhKWm9lht7Z0uHUzdA66ShvDAk8ZdGZvoZ0b7xmwTNACSpKlcCrSMEKRRL0dqhCoE_hRlgjyqMRJZmdHy_v1MPstNbM-Rh4TggvT-DZfpl38lW2JrTbSEMkcwoyCdzvtWn_0XJK-JjcfgLFSM9GXBmvNPW32BRckWPWkna-7DVytOV9_XURGbmut4ZiXC7Vw_-__yO4TtBQx6pLdQSHm24bHhP82rgn0bZ-A2bTMrw priority: 102 providerName: Scholars Portal |
Title | Sociality genes are associated with human-directed social behaviour in golden and Labrador retriever dogs |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30416887 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2130229765 https://search.proquest.com/docview/2132292911 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6225837 https://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-153536 https://doaj.org/article/dc737cf3aca34dcba9bd0b1dab3b998e |
Volume | 6 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEB61RSAuFRQoLiUsAsHJSZy11-tjWloqRKoKURRxWe3LwSi1Izf5_8yu7SiGGxcfvGMl3nmvZ74BeM9TIw3LbcioycI4zXUobZKGJrPGMquU8ePbZtfs6jb-Mk_me5B0vTC-aF-rYlgu74Zl8cvXVq7u9KirExvdzM4ZCiEmVqN92EcB3UnRvfnFgBnjigaKNMVsZbSytv49TDh3EKGYvkfMV9Dt-CEP1_-vUd7xSn9XTPZwRb0vunwCh20QSabNn30Ke7Y8gkez9jP5ETz8WfnT8mdQNO23GGqThbNqRNaWyJYl1hB3DEv8nL6wcW54rzlGJ20D_6YmRUkW1RINFJGlIV8lJtimqknth3GhKhBTLe6fw-3lxffzq7CdrhBq3J91iIrJMT2gzPDcoBYqN_8zNonJMeJKEoeTiLGMdghu0djG2VjhMvd4gdyqXNIXcFBWpX0JhJuMZlJZmyoaI89VPNaUaWdooxyNRgDvur0WqwZEQ2Dy4ZgjPHOEY04AZ44NWwoHfO1vVPVCtOwXRqc01TmVWtLYaCUzZcYqMlJRhZmjDeCNY6Jouki36iumbgooBouUBfDRUzgFRl5q2fYh4Ks4KKwe5WmPEhVP95c7QRGt4t-LifsQPMEYLwng7XbZPemK2UpbbTwNkkzQzQRw3MjV9qU78Qwg7Ulcb1f6K6glHha81YoAPjSy2XvkU_Fj6jdyWWwEermEspP__olX8BjjRu5bMtkpHKzrjX2NsdlaDeDB2cX1zbeBP9vA6-d5hNdZzAdeS_8ANgNAoA |
link.rule.ids | 230,315,730,783,787,867,888,2109,21400,24330,27936,27937,33756,33757,43817,53804,53806,74630 |
linkProvider | National Library of Medicine |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3db9MwED_BJgEviG8CgxmB4ClbWidO8oQ62FSgrRDa0N4sf6UUTUlJ2_-fO8ctC0i82mcpufN92v4dwJsit8qKysWC2zJO88rEymV5bEtnnXBaW9--bToT44v082V2GQpuq3CtcmsTvaG2jaEa-fGQTtiG6Dyz98tfMXWNotPV0ELjJuwTVBUmX_snp7Ov33ZVFnRwAoOMDpc0x9TleOlc-_MoK6iv-zVP5AH7_zXL1_zS33cme8ii3hud3YO7IYxko07u9-GGqx_ArWk4KH8Ii-7ZLYbYbE7WjKnWMRVE4Syj8ivz_fnizqnhWFc-Z-Hh_qZli5rNmys0TEzVlk0UJta2aVnrm3ChCjDbzFeP4OLs9PzDOA5dFWKDrFjHqJAFpgVc2KKyqH2a-n6mNrMVRlpZRviIGMMYQm4bJC4tE43ThccJLJyuFH8Me3VTu6fAClvyUmnncs1TlLVOE8OFIQM7qNBYRPB6y2G57MAzJCYdJAfp5SBJDhGcEPN3FAR47Qeadi6D_khrcp6biiujeGqNVqW2iR5YpbnGjNFFcEiik93r0Z3ayhF1_8QgkYsI3nkKUlyUoFHh_QH-CkFg9SgPepSocKY_vd0eMij8Sv7ZnhG82k3TSrrEVrtm42mQZIjuJYIn3W7a_TRPMDJGgx9B3ttnPa70Z-rFDw8HLtAkFxxXvu12ZG_Jx8X3kWfk1WIj0btlXDz7__cfwu3x-XQiJ59mX57DHQwQC__2UhzA3rrduBcYhK31y6BpvwHFDTSJ |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfR1db9Mw8ASdNPGC-CYwmBEInkLTOnGSJ9SxVQO2akIM7c3yV0rRlJS0_f_cOW5ZQOLVPkvJfZ99HwBvitwqKyoXC27LOM0rEyuX5bEtnXXCaW39-LbzmTi9TD9fZVch_2kV0iq3OtEratsYuiMfjumFbYzGMxtWIS3i4nj6YfkrpglS9NIaxmnchr08FTwZwN7Ryezi6-7GBY2dQIej61GaYxgzXDrX_nyfFTTj_YZV8s37_1XRN2zU3_mTvS6j3jJN78Hd4FKySccD9-GWqx_A_nl4NH8Ii64EF91tNifNxlTrmApkcZbRVSzzs_rizsDhWneVzkIR_6Zli5rNm2tUUkzVlp0pDLJt07LWD-RCcWC2ma8eweX05NvH0zhMWIgNomIdo3AWGCJwYYvKoiRqmgGa2sxW6HVlGfVKRH_GUBe3UeLSMtG4XfiegYXTleKPYVA3tXsKrLAlL5V2Ltc8RbrrNDFcGFK2owoVRwSvtxiWy66RhsQAhOggPR0k0SGCI0L-DoKaX_uFpp3LIEvSmpznpuLKKJ5ao1WpbaJHVmmuMXp0ERwS6WRXSboTYTmhSaDoMHIRwTsPQUKMFDQq1CLgr1A7rB7kQQ8Shc_0t7fsIYPwr-QfVo3g1W6bTlJCW-2ajYdBkDGamgiedNy0-2meoJeMyj-CvMdnPaz0d-rFD98aXKB6LjiefNtxZO_I8eL7xCPyerGRaOkyLp79__sPYR-FTJ59mn15DnfQVyx8GaY4gMG63bgX6I-t9csgaL8BHT44tw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sociality+genes+are+associated+with+human-directed+social+behaviour+in+golden+and+Labrador+retriever+dogs&rft.jtitle=PeerJ+%28San+Francisco%2C+CA%29&rft.au=Persson%2C+Mia+E&rft.au=Sundman%2C+Ann-Sofie&rft.au=Halld%C3%A9n%2C+Lise-Lotte&rft.au=Trottier%2C+Agaia+J&rft.date=2018-11-06&rft.pub=PeerJ%2C+Inc&rft.eissn=2167-8359&rft_id=info:doi/10.7717%2Fpeerj.5889&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2167-8359&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2167-8359&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2167-8359&client=summon |