Sociality genes are associated with human-directed social behaviour in golden and Labrador retriever dogs

Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the pres...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inPeerJ (San Francisco, CA) Vol. 6; p. e5889
Main Authors Persson, Mia E, Sundman, Ann-Sofie, Halldén, Lise-Lotte, Trottier, Agaia J, Jensen, Per
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States PeerJ. Ltd 06.11.2018
PeerJ, Inc
PeerJ Inc
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner ( = 4.389, = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers ( = 6.330, = 0.003) and Labradors ( = 5.209, = 0.025). Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
AbstractList BACKGROUNDDogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. METHODSDogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. RESULTSThere was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F 2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F 2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F 1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). DISCUSSIONOur results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F.sub.2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F.sub.2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F.sub.1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner ( F 2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers ( F 2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors ( F 1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F.sub.2,56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F.sub.2,55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F.sub.1,93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et al., 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP1 was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner ( = 4.389, = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers ( = 6.330, = 0.003) and Labradors ( = 5.209, = 0.025). Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
Background: Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26 associated with human contact-seeking behaviors during an unsolvable problem task in laboratory beagles (Persson et A, 2016). The aim of the present study was to verify the single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in additional dog breeds. We also studied how the allele frequencies have changed during domestication and recent selection. Methods: Dogs of two breeds, 61 golden retrievers and 100 Labrador retrievers, were phenotyped and genotyped, and 19 wolves were genotyped. The Labrador retrievers were divided into common and field type by pedigree data to make it possible to study the effects of recent selection. All dogs were tested in an unsolvable problem task where human-directed social behaviors were scored. DNA from dogs (buccal swabs) and wolves (blood or brain tissue) was analyzed for genotype on two of the previously identified SNP markers, BICF2G630798942 (SNP1) and BICF2S23712114 (SNP2), by pyrosequencing. Results: There was genetic variation for SNP1 in both dog breeds whereas the wolves were fixed for this polymorphism, and for SNP2 there was variation in both dogs and wolves. For both SNPs, Labrador retriever types differed significantly in allele frequencies. We found associations between SNPs and human-directed social behavior in both dog breeds. In golden retrievers, SNP I was associated with physical contact variables, for example, with the duration of physical contact with the owner (F-2,F-56 = 4.389, p = 0.017). SNP2 was associated with several behavioral variables in both breeds, among others owner gazing frequency in both golden retrievers (F-2,F-55 = 6.330, p = 0.003) and Labradors (F-1,F-93 = 5.209, p = 0.025). Discussion: Our results verify the association between the previously identified SNPs and human-directed social behavior scored in an unsolvable problem task. Differences in allele frequencies suggest that these loci have been affected by selection. The results indicate that these genomic regions are involved in human-directed social behavior in not only beagles but in other dog breeds as well. We hypothesize that they may have been important during dog domestication.
ArticleNumber e5889
Audience Academic
Author Sundman, Ann-Sofie
Jensen, Per
Halldén, Lise-Lotte
Trottier, Agaia J
Persson, Mia E
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Mia E
  orcidid: 0000-0002-6115-7517
  surname: Persson
  fullname: Persson, Mia E
  organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Ann-Sofie
  surname: Sundman
  fullname: Sundman, Ann-Sofie
  organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Lise-Lotte
  surname: Halldén
  fullname: Halldén, Lise-Lotte
  organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Agaia J
  surname: Trottier
  fullname: Trottier, Agaia J
  organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Per
  surname: Jensen
  fullname: Jensen, Per
  organization: AVIAN Behaviour Genomics and Physiology Group, IFM Biology, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30416887$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
https://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-153536$$DView record from Swedish Publication Index
BookMark eNptkl2L3CAUhkPZ0t1u96Y_oAiFUgqZxhij3iwM26-FgV7041aMniQOGZ1qMsv--5qZ7XamVC-Uc57zqsf3eXbmvIMse4mLBWOYvd8ChPWCci6eZBclrlnOCRVnR_vz7CrGdZEGL-uCk2fZOSkqXHPOLjL7zWurBjveow4cRKQCIBXjHB3BoDs79qifNsrlxgbQc2yfHFADvdpZPwVkHer8YMAh5QxaqSYo4wMKMAYLOwjI-C6-yJ62aohw9bBeZj8-ffx-8yVfff18e7Nc5TpdbsyZILyoBKkNb01NeFMzwitDTVtQQSllhHBBNK1EiQuoRNGkNK9KIjiHplXkMrs96Bqv1nIb7EaFe-mVlfuAD51UYbR6AGk0I0y3RGlFKqMbJRpTNNiohjRCcEha-UEr3sF2ak7UPtify73aYCeJKaGkTvz1gU_wBowGNwY1nJSdZpztZed3si5LyglLAm8fBIL_NUEc5cZGDcOgHPgpyhKTshSlwDihr_9B1-kvXGrtTBUJYzX9S3UqPdi61qdz9Swql7TGZUXr_b0X_6HSNLCxOhmutSl-UvDmqKAHNYx99MM0Wu_iKfjuAOrgYwzQPjYDF3I2sNwbWM4GTvCr4_Y9on_sSn4DQoHsZg
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2020_1791682
crossref_primary_10_3390_ani12212895
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_021_01501_8
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2019_104860
crossref_primary_10_3390_genes12060832
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_41849_0
Cites_doi 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.09.023
10.1111/gbb.12194
10.1016/J.Anbehav.2011.07.039
10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005
10.1371/journal.pone.0006584
10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.173
10.1126/science.1261022
10.4088/JCP.10m06491
10.1016/j.applanim.2017.12.017
10.1007/s10071-011-0446-x
10.1371/journal.pone.0065296
10.1038/srep46636
10.2527/jas.2007-0616
10.1126/sciadv.1700398
10.1038/sj.mp.4001586
10.1371/journal.pone.0083993
10.1038/Nature04338
10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X
10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01582
10.4236/ojas.2013.31008
10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.122
10.1098/rsbl.2015.0489
10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.012
10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.055
10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00093-2
10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.219
10.1177/0963721416657050
10.1101/gr.3147604
10.1016/J.Anbehav.2005.03.025
10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.07.016
10.1007/s00439-015-1585-y
10.1002/jeab.191
10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003
10.1007/S10071-013-0627-X
10.1002/ajmg.b.31073
10.1007/s10071-017-1093-7
10.1111/gbb.12317
10.2174/1570159x13666150612225836
10.1007/s100710000072
10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.017
10.1186/s12864-018-4916-2
10.1093/jhered/esy012
10.1038/srep33439
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright COPYRIGHT 2018 PeerJ. Ltd.
2018 Persson et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
2018 Persson et al. 2018 Persson et al.
Copyright_xml – notice: COPYRIGHT 2018 PeerJ. Ltd.
– notice: 2018 Persson et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
– notice: 2018 Persson et al. 2018 Persson et al.
DBID NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
3V.
7XB
88I
8FE
8FH
8FK
ABUWG
AFKRA
AZQEC
BBNVY
BENPR
BHPHI
CCPQU
DWQXO
GNUQQ
HCIFZ
LK8
M2P
M7P
PIMPY
PQEST
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
Q9U
7X8
5PM
ABXSW
ADTPV
AOWAS
D8T
DG8
ZZAVC
DOA
DOI 10.7717/peerj.5889
DatabaseName PubMed
CrossRef
ProQuest Central (Corporate)
ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)
Science Database (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest Central
ProQuest Central Essentials
Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central
Natural Science Collection
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Central Korea
ProQuest Central Student
SciTech Premium Collection
Biological Sciences
Science Database
Biological Science Database
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central China
ProQuest Central Basic
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
SWEPUB Linköpings universitet full text
SwePub
SwePub Articles
SWEPUB Freely available online
SWEPUB Linköpings universitet
SwePub Articles full text
Directory of Open Access Journals
DatabaseTitle PubMed
CrossRef
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest Science Journals (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central Basic
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest Science Journals
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
SciTech Premium Collection
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
Biological Science Database
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Central China
ProQuest Central
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Central Korea
Biological Science Collection
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic

CrossRef



PubMed

Publicly Available Content Database
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: DOA
  name: Directory of Open Access Journals
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: BENPR
  name: ProQuest Central
  url: https://www.proquest.com/central
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
Zoology
EISSN 2167-8359
EndPage e5889
ExternalDocumentID oai_doaj_org_article_dc737cf3aca34dcba9bd0b1dab3b998e
oai_DiVA_org_liu_153536
A561245636
10_7717_peerj_5889
30416887
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Advanced Research Grant from the European Research Council (ERC), project 322206 “GENEWELL”
GroupedDBID 3V.
53G
5VS
88I
8FE
8FH
AAFWJ
ABUWG
ADBBV
ADRAZ
AENEX
AFKRA
AFPKN
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
AZQEC
BAWUL
BBNVY
BCNDV
BENPR
BHPHI
BPHCQ
CCPQU
DIK
DWQXO
ECGQY
GNUQQ
GROUPED_DOAJ
GX1
H13
HCIFZ
HYE
IAO
IEA
IHR
IHW
ITC
KQ8
LK8
M2P
M48
M7P
M~E
NPM
OK1
PIMPY
PQQKQ
PROAC
RPM
W2D
YAO
AAYXX
CITATION
7XB
8FK
PQEST
PQUKI
PRINS
Q9U
7X8
5PM
ABXSW
ADTPV
AOWAS
D8T
DG8
ZZAVC
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c608t-793804936d8fd638b67384d5df059555733893c549210e490b3848423988ebfa3
IEDL.DBID RPM
ISSN 2167-8359
IngestDate Tue Oct 22 15:15:05 EDT 2024
Sat Aug 24 00:48:40 EDT 2024
Tue Sep 17 20:46:39 EDT 2024
Sat Aug 17 03:32:31 EDT 2024
Thu Oct 10 18:15:39 EDT 2024
Wed Aug 14 18:52:12 EDT 2024
Tue Aug 13 05:21:16 EDT 2024
Tue Aug 20 22:14:31 EDT 2024
Fri Aug 23 02:11:00 EDT 2024
Sat Sep 28 08:22:29 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Keywords Dog genetics
Behavior genetics
Dog behavior
Golden retrievers
Human-dog communication
Labrador retrievers
Domestication
Genetics
Human-directed social behavior
Wolf
Language English
License This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, reproduction and adaptation in any medium and for any purpose provided that it is properly attributed. For attribution, the original author(s), title, publication source (PeerJ) and either DOI or URL of the article must be cited.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c608t-793804936d8fd638b67384d5df059555733893c549210e490b3848423988ebfa3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0002-6115-7517
OpenAccessLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6225837/
PMID 30416887
PQID 2130229765
PQPubID 2045935
ParticipantIDs doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_dc737cf3aca34dcba9bd0b1dab3b998e
swepub_primary_oai_DiVA_org_liu_153536
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6225837
proquest_miscellaneous_2132292911
proquest_journals_2130229765
gale_infotracmisc_A561245636
gale_infotracacademiconefile_A561245636
gale_healthsolutions_A561245636
crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_5889
pubmed_primary_30416887
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2018-11-06
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2018-11-06
PublicationDate_xml – month: 11
  year: 2018
  text: 2018-11-06
  day: 06
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: San Diego
– name: San Diego, USA
PublicationTitle PeerJ (San Francisco, CA)
PublicationTitleAlternate PeerJ
PublicationYear 2018
Publisher PeerJ. Ltd
PeerJ, Inc
PeerJ Inc
Publisher_xml – name: PeerJ. Ltd
– name: PeerJ, Inc
– name: PeerJ Inc
References Brubaker (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-2) 2017; 20
Sim (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-31) 2012; 73
Topal (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-36) 2005; 70
Nagasawa (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-23) 2015; 348
Bentosela (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-1) 2016; 105
Sutter (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-35) 2004; 14
Sundman (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-33) 2018; 201
Range (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-29) 2015; 5
Wilsson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-41) 1998; 58
Kis (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-12) 2014; 9
Caniglia (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-3) 2018; 19
Miklósi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-21) 2013; 17
Marshall-Pescini (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-17) 2017; 7
Persson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-25) 2015; 14
Miklosi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-20) 2000; 3
Sanders (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-30) 2005; 10
Miklósi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-19) 2003; 13
Persson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-26) 2017; 95
Persson (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-27) 2016; 6
Horn (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-10) 2013; 8
Soproni (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-32) 2001; 115
Udell (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-39) 2014; 89
Jensen (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-11) 2016; 25
Heberlein (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-8) 2016; 122
VonHoldt (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-42) 2017; 3
Konno (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-13) 2018; 109
Lakatos (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-14) 2012; 15
Lindblad-Toh (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-15) 2005; 438
Mas (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-18) 2010; 153B
Muller (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-22) 2015; 25
Sundman (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-34) 2016; 15
Topal (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-37) 1998; 112
Udell (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-38) 2015; 11
Hare (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-6) 1999; 113
Van Der Waaij (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-40) 2008; 86
Qayyum (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-28) 2015; 13
Passalacqua (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-24) 2011; 82
Chapman (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-4) 2015; 134
Marshall-Pescini (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-16) 2013; 16
Hare (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-7) 2005; 9
Gacsi (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-5) 2009; 4
Hori (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-9) 2013; 3
Xu (10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-43) 2013; 145
References_xml – volume: 122
  start-page: 59
  year: 2016
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-8
  article-title: A comparison between wolves, Canis lupus, and dogs, Canis familiaris, in showing behaviour towards humans
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.09.023
  contributor:
    fullname: Heberlein
– volume: 14
  start-page: 337
  issue: 4
  year: 2015
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-25
  article-title: Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability
  publication-title: Genes Brain and Behavior
  doi: 10.1111/gbb.12194
  contributor:
    fullname: Persson
– volume: 82
  start-page: 1043
  issue: 5
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-24
  article-title: Human-directed gazing behaviour in puppies and adult dogs, Canis lupus familiaris
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
  doi: 10.1016/J.Anbehav.2011.07.039
  contributor:
    fullname: Passalacqua
– volume: 17
  start-page: 287
  issue: 6
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-21
  article-title: What does it take to become ‘best friends’? Evolutionary changes in canine social competence
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005
  contributor:
    fullname: Miklósi
– volume: 4
  start-page: e6584
  issue: 8
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-5
  article-title: Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills
  publication-title: PLOS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006584
  contributor:
    fullname: Gacsi
– volume: 113
  start-page: 173
  issue: 12
  year: 1999
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-6
  article-title: Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food
  publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.173
  contributor:
    fullname: Hare
– volume: 348
  start-page: 333
  issue: 6232
  year: 2015
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-23
  article-title: Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.1261022
  contributor:
    fullname: Nagasawa
– volume: 73
  start-page: 320
  issue: 3
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-31
  article-title: ARVCF genetic influences on neurocognitive and neuroanatomical intermediate phenotypes in Chinese patients with schizophrenia
  publication-title: Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
  doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06491
  contributor:
    fullname: Sim
– volume: 201
  start-page: 46
  year: 2018
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-33
  article-title: Understanding of human referential gestures is not correlated to human-directed social behaviour in Labrador retrievers and German shepherd dogs
  publication-title: Applied Animal Behaviour Science
  doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.12.017
  contributor:
    fullname: Sundman
– volume: 15
  start-page: 201
  issue: 2
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-14
  article-title: Comprehension and utilisation of pointing gestures and gazing in dog–human communication in relatively complex situations
  publication-title: Animal Cognition
  doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0446-x
  contributor:
    fullname: Lakatos
– volume: 8
  start-page: e65296
  issue: 5
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-10
  article-title: The importance of the secure base effect for domestic dogs—evidence from a manipulative problem-solving task
  publication-title: PLOS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065296
  contributor:
    fullname: Horn
– volume: 7
  start-page: 46636
  issue: 1
  year: 2017
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-17
  article-title: The role of domestication and experience in ‘looking back’ towards humans in an unsolvable task
  publication-title: Scientific Report
  doi: 10.1038/srep46636
  contributor:
    fullname: Marshall-Pescini
– volume: 86
  start-page: 2853
  issue: 11
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-40
  article-title: Genetic analysis of results of a Swedish behavior test on German shepherd dogs and labrador retrievers
  publication-title: Journal of Animal Science
  doi: 10.2527/jas.2007-0616
  contributor:
    fullname: Van Der Waaij
– volume: 3
  start-page: e1700398
  issue: 7
  year: 2017
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-42
  article-title: Structural variants in genes associated with human Williams-Beuren syndrome underlie stereotypical hypersociability in domestic dogs
  publication-title: Science Advances
  doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700398
  contributor:
    fullname: VonHoldt
– volume: 10
  start-page: 353
  issue: 4
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-30
  article-title: Haplotypic association spanning the 22q11.21 genes COMT and ARVCF with schizophrenia
  publication-title: Molecular Psychiatry
  doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001586
  contributor:
    fullname: Sanders
– volume: 9
  start-page: e83993
  issue: 1
  year: 2014
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-12
  article-title: Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris)
  publication-title: PLOS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083993
  contributor:
    fullname: Kis
– volume: 438
  start-page: 803
  issue: 7069
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-15
  article-title: Genome sequence, comparative analysis and haplotype structure of the domestic dog
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/Nature04338
  contributor:
    fullname: Lindblad-Toh
– volume: 13
  start-page: 763
  issue: 9
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-19
  article-title: A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X
  contributor:
    fullname: Miklósi
– volume: 5
  start-page: 1582
  year: 2015
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-29
  article-title: Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: the “Canine Cooperation Hypothesis”
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01582
  contributor:
    fullname: Range
– volume: 3
  start-page: 54
  issue: 1
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-9
  article-title: Dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) is associated with gazing toward humans in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris)
  publication-title: Open Journal of Animal Sciences
  doi: 10.4236/ojas.2013.31008
  contributor:
    fullname: Hori
– volume: 115
  start-page: 122
  issue: 2
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-32
  article-title: Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris)
  publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.122
  contributor:
    fullname: Soproni
– volume: 11
  start-page: 20150489
  issue: 9
  year: 2015
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-38
  article-title: When dogs look back: inhibition of independent problem-solving behaviour in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) compared with wolves (Canis lupus)
  publication-title: Biology Letters
  doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0489
  contributor:
    fullname: Udell
– volume: 89
  start-page: 99
  year: 2014
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-39
  article-title: Exploring breed differences in dogs (Canis familiaris): does exaggeration or inhibition of predatory response predict performance on human-guided tasks?
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.012
  contributor:
    fullname: Udell
– volume: 25
  start-page: 601
  issue: 5
  year: 2015
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-22
  article-title: Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.055
  contributor:
    fullname: Muller
– volume: 58
  start-page: 151
  issue: 1–2
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-41
  article-title: Behaviour test for eight-week old puppies—heritabilities of tested behaviour traits and its correspondence to later behaviour
  publication-title: Applied Animal Behaviour Science
  doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00093-2
  contributor:
    fullname: Wilsson
– volume: 112
  start-page: 219
  issue: 3
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-37
  article-title: Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation Test
  publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.3.219
  contributor:
    fullname: Topal
– volume: 25
  start-page: 334
  issue: 5
  year: 2016
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-11
  article-title: The genetics of how dogs became our social allies
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0963721416657050
  contributor:
    fullname: Jensen
– volume: 14
  start-page: 2388
  issue: 12
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-35
  article-title: Extensive and breed-specific linkage disequilibrium in Canis familiaris
  publication-title: Genome Research
  doi: 10.1101/gr.3147604
  contributor:
    fullname: Sutter
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1367
  issue: 6
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-36
  article-title: Attachment to humans: a comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
  doi: 10.1016/J.Anbehav.2005.03.025
  contributor:
    fullname: Topal
– volume: 95
  start-page: 85
  year: 2017
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-26
  article-title: Intranasal oxytocin and a polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene are associated with human-directed social behavior in golden retriever dogs
  publication-title: Hormones and Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.07.016
  contributor:
    fullname: Persson
– volume: 134
  start-page: 1055
  issue: 10
  year: 2015
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-4
  article-title: Whole exome sequencing in extended families with autism spectrum disorder implicates four candidate genes
  publication-title: Human Genetics
  doi: 10.1007/s00439-015-1585-y
  contributor:
    fullname: Chapman
– volume: 105
  start-page: 68
  issue: 1
  year: 2016
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-1
  article-title: Sociability and gazing toward humans in dogs and wolves: simple behaviors with broad implications
  publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
  doi: 10.1002/jeab.191
  contributor:
    fullname: Bentosela
– volume: 9
  start-page: 439
  issue: 9
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-7
  article-title: Human-like social skills in dogs?
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003
  contributor:
    fullname: Hare
– volume: 16
  start-page: 933
  issue: 6
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-16
  article-title: Gaze alternation in dogs and toddlers in an unsolvable task: evidence of an audience effect
  publication-title: Animal Cognition
  doi: 10.1007/S10071-013-0627-X
  contributor:
    fullname: Marshall-Pescini
– volume: 153B
  start-page: 1052
  issue: 5
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-18
  article-title: A functional variant provided further evidence for the association of ARVCF with schizophrenia
  publication-title: American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics
  doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31073
  contributor:
    fullname: Mas
– volume: 20
  start-page: 717
  issue: 4
  year: 2017
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-2
  article-title: Differences in problem-solving between canid populations: do domestication and lifetime experience affect persistence?
  publication-title: Animal Cognition
  doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1093-7
  contributor:
    fullname: Brubaker
– volume: 15
  start-page: 750
  issue: 8
  year: 2016
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-34
  article-title: Similar recent selection criteria associated with different behavioural effects in two dog breeds
  publication-title: Genes Brain and Behavior
  doi: 10.1111/gbb.12317
  contributor:
    fullname: Sundman
– volume: 13
  start-page: 802
  issue: 6
  year: 2015
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-28
  article-title: The role of the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT) geneval158met in aggressive behavior, a review of genetic studies
  publication-title: Current Neuropharmacology
  doi: 10.2174/1570159x13666150612225836
  contributor:
    fullname: Qayyum
– volume: 3
  start-page: 159
  issue: 3
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-20
  article-title: Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: an experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog
  publication-title: Animal Cognition
  doi: 10.1007/s100710000072
  contributor:
    fullname: Miklosi
– volume: 145
  start-page: 95
  issue: 1
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-43
  article-title: Polymorphisms in seizure 6-like gene are associated with bipolar disorder I: evidence of gene x gender interaction
  publication-title: Journal of Affective Disorders
  doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.017
  contributor:
    fullname: Xu
– volume: 19
  start-page: 533
  issue: 1
  year: 2018
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-3
  article-title: Wolf outside, dog inside? The genomic make-up of the Czechoslovakian Wolfdog
  publication-title: BMC Genomics
  doi: 10.1186/s12864-018-4916-2
  contributor:
    fullname: Caniglia
– volume: 109
  start-page: 566
  issue: 5
  year: 2018
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-13
  article-title: Effect of canine oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism on the successful training of drug detection dogs
  publication-title: Journal of Heredity
  doi: 10.1093/jhered/esy012
  contributor:
    fullname: Konno
– volume: 6
  start-page: 33439
  issue: 1
  year: 2016
  ident: 10.7717/peerj.5889/ref-27
  article-title: Genomic regions associated with interspecies communication in dogs contain genes related to human social disorders
  publication-title: Scientific Report
  doi: 10.1038/srep33439
  contributor:
    fullname: Persson
SSID ssj0000826083
Score 2.2438996
Snippet Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome 26...
Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on...
Background Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on...
BACKGROUNDDogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on chromosome...
Background: Dogs have human-directed social skills that allow them to communicate and cooperate with humans. We have previously identified two loci on...
SourceID doaj
swepub
pubmedcentral
proquest
gale
crossref
pubmed
SourceType Open Website
Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
StartPage e5889
SubjectTerms Alleles
Analysis
Animal Behavior
Animal cognition
Behavior
Canidae
Cooperation
Dog behavior
Dog genetics
Dogs
Domestication
Ethics
Gene frequency
Gene polymorphism
Genes
Genetic aspects
Genetic diversity
Genetic engineering
Genetics
Genomes
Genomics
Golden retrievers
Human-directed social behavior
Human-dog communication
Hypotheses
Pedigree
Principal components analysis
Schizophrenia
Single-nucleotide polymorphism
Social behavior
Social behavior in animals
Social skills
Studies
Wolves
Zoology
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: Directory of Open Access Journals
  dbid: DOA
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Lb9QwELZQD4gL4k2ggBEITqHZdeI4x-VRVYhyoqg3y_ZMtkFVUqW7_58ZO11t4MCFazyWkhnPzDex_Y0Qb00NDnSLuVbQ5GXdhtxhVefQIKBG7yG2bzv9rk_Oyq_n1fleqy8-E5bogZPijiDUqg6tcsGpEoJ3jYfCL8B55alUwBh9i2avmIoxmFAzgYvER1pTyXJ0hTj--lAZ7ue-l4EiUf_f4XgvH_15VnLGKBqz0PE9cXeCj3KVXvu-uIX9A3H7dNogfyi6dN2WoLVccxSTbkTpJhMgSP7tKmNfvjwlM3qWfpvL6cL-dpRdL9fDJQUk6XqQ3xwV1DCMcozNt2jpSxjW14_E2fGXH59O8qmbQh5IFZucHNFQOaA0mBbI6zz3-yyhgpYQVlUxLyJhl8CMbYsCy6bwNGwiP6BB3zr1WBz0Q49PhTTQqMZ5xNqrkmzsyyIoHTiwLloKEpl4c6Nhe5VIMywVG2wHG-1g2Q6Z-MjK30kw0XV8QOa3k_ntv8yfiVdsOptuje7c1a646yeBQ6Uz8T5KsMOSBYOb7h3QpzD11UzycCZJjhbmwzfLw06Ofm2XvPG7JExXZeL1bphn8uG1HodtlCGRJaWVTDxJq2n30aogREyBPhP1bJ3NtDIf6buLSAOuKRQbRTPfpRU5m_K5-7mKirzstpayWqX0s_-h7-fiDkFGE29j6kNxsBm3-IJg2ca_jB74GzGJO2M
  priority: 102
  providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals
– databaseName: ProQuest Central
  dbid: BENPR
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfR1db9Mw0IJOQrwgxtcCYxiB4CksrRMneULd2DQhNiHE0N4s2-eUoCkpafv_uXPcbAGJV_ssJfd9Z98dY2-LHDTIysVSQBmneWVj7bI8htKBk84Y8OPbzi_k2WX6-Sq7Cgm3VXhWudWJXlFDaylHfjijG7YZGs_s4_J3TFOj6HY1jNC4y3ZmGCkkE7ZzdHLx9duQZUEDJ9HJ6PuS5hi6HC6d6359yAqa637LEvmG_f-q5Vt26e83k6POot4anT5kD4Ibyec93XfZHdc8YvfOw0X5Y1b3ZbfoYvMFaTOuO8d1IIUDTulX7ufzxb1Rw7U-fc5D4f6m43XDF-01KiauG-BfNAbW0Ha880O4UAQ4tIvVE3Z5evL9-CwOUxVii6hYxyiQBYYFQkJRAUqfobmfKWRQoaeVZdQfEX0YS53bpolLy8TgduH7BBbOVFo8ZZOmbdwe4wWUotTGudyIFGlt0sQKaUnBTitUFhF7s8WwWvbNMxQGHUQH5emgiA4ROyLkDxDU8NovtN1CBflRYHOR20poq0UK1ujSQGKmoI0wGDG6iL0i0qm-enQQWzWn6Z_oJAoZsfceggQXKWh1qD_AX6EWWCPI_REkCpwdb2_ZQwWBX6kb9ozY62GbTtIjtsa1Gw-DIDM0LxF71nPT8NMiQc8YFX7E8hGfjbAy3mnqn74duESVXAg8-a7nyNGRT_WPuUfkdb1RaN0yIZ____tfsPvoFBa-3lLus8m627iX6HitzUGQrj8bzjGP
  priority: 102
  providerName: ProQuest
– databaseName: Scholars Portal Open Access Journals
  dbid: M48
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lj9MwEB4ti4S4IN4EFjACwSmlrRPHOaHyWK0Qy4mivVm2xylBVbJkWwn-PTNOWpEFcY3HUjLPb2LPDMALXaBFVYVUSSzTrKh8akNepFgGDCo4h3F82-lndbLMPp7lZwewm985MPDin6kdz5NaduvJzx-_3pDBE36dFJSNvD4Pofs-ybUur8DVeUYZOl_hG2B-9MiEoQlq9N1JL20ZxaPYtv9v5_xHdLp8c3LUXzTGpOObcGMAk2LRS_8WHITmNlw7HY7L70DdF98S0BYr9mnCdkHYQSABBf-EFXFKX9qHNnrW_0QXQ_n-thN1I1btmtyTsA2KT5bSa2w70cVRXGQIAtvVxV1YHn_48u4kHWYrpJ5YsUnJLDUlB1KhrpBs0PH0zwxzrAhv5Tl3SSQk47l_22wasnLqaFnHboE6uMrKe3DYtE14AEJjKUvrQiiczEjiLpt6qTy72VlFLiOB5zsOm_O-hYah1IPlYKIcDMshgbfM_D0Ft72OD9puZQYrMugLWfhKWm9lht7Z0uHUzdA66ShvDAk8ZdGZvoZ0b7xmwTNACSpKlcCrSMEKRRL0dqhCoE_hRlgjyqMRJZmdHy_v1MPstNbM-Rh4TggvT-DZfpl38lW2JrTbSEMkcwoyCdzvtWn_0XJK-JjcfgLFSM9GXBmvNPW32BRckWPWkna-7DVytOV9_XURGbmut4ZiXC7Vw_-__yO4TtBQx6pLdQSHm24bHhP82rgn0bZ-A2bTMrw
  priority: 102
  providerName: Scholars Portal
Title Sociality genes are associated with human-directed social behaviour in golden and Labrador retriever dogs
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30416887
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2130229765
https://search.proquest.com/docview/2132292911
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6225837
https://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-153536
https://doaj.org/article/dc737cf3aca34dcba9bd0b1dab3b998e
Volume 6
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEB61RSAuFRQoLiUsAsHJSZy11-tjWloqRKoKURRxWe3LwSi1Izf5_8yu7SiGGxcfvGMl3nmvZ74BeM9TIw3LbcioycI4zXUobZKGJrPGMquU8ePbZtfs6jb-Mk_me5B0vTC-aF-rYlgu74Zl8cvXVq7u9KirExvdzM4ZCiEmVqN92EcB3UnRvfnFgBnjigaKNMVsZbSytv49TDh3EKGYvkfMV9Dt-CEP1_-vUd7xSn9XTPZwRb0vunwCh20QSabNn30Ke7Y8gkez9jP5ETz8WfnT8mdQNO23GGqThbNqRNaWyJYl1hB3DEv8nL6wcW54rzlGJ20D_6YmRUkW1RINFJGlIV8lJtimqknth3GhKhBTLe6fw-3lxffzq7CdrhBq3J91iIrJMT2gzPDcoBYqN_8zNonJMeJKEoeTiLGMdghu0djG2VjhMvd4gdyqXNIXcFBWpX0JhJuMZlJZmyoaI89VPNaUaWdooxyNRgDvur0WqwZEQ2Dy4ZgjPHOEY04AZ44NWwoHfO1vVPVCtOwXRqc01TmVWtLYaCUzZcYqMlJRhZmjDeCNY6Jouki36iumbgooBouUBfDRUzgFRl5q2fYh4Ks4KKwe5WmPEhVP95c7QRGt4t-LifsQPMEYLwng7XbZPemK2UpbbTwNkkzQzQRw3MjV9qU78Qwg7Ulcb1f6K6glHha81YoAPjSy2XvkU_Fj6jdyWWwEermEspP__olX8BjjRu5bMtkpHKzrjX2NsdlaDeDB2cX1zbeBP9vA6-d5hNdZzAdeS_8ANgNAoA
link.rule.ids 230,315,730,783,787,867,888,2109,21400,24330,27936,27937,33756,33757,43817,53804,53806,74630
linkProvider National Library of Medicine
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3db9MwED_BJgEviG8CgxmB4ClbWidO8oQ62FSgrRDa0N4sf6UUTUlJ2_-fO8ctC0i82mcpufN92v4dwJsit8qKysWC2zJO88rEymV5bEtnnXBaW9--bToT44v082V2GQpuq3CtcmsTvaG2jaEa-fGQTtiG6Dyz98tfMXWNotPV0ELjJuwTVBUmX_snp7Ov33ZVFnRwAoOMDpc0x9TleOlc-_MoK6iv-zVP5AH7_zXL1_zS33cme8ii3hud3YO7IYxko07u9-GGqx_ArWk4KH8Ii-7ZLYbYbE7WjKnWMRVE4Syj8ivz_fnizqnhWFc-Z-Hh_qZli5rNmys0TEzVlk0UJta2aVnrm3ChCjDbzFeP4OLs9PzDOA5dFWKDrFjHqJAFpgVc2KKyqH2a-n6mNrMVRlpZRviIGMMYQm4bJC4tE43ThccJLJyuFH8Me3VTu6fAClvyUmnncs1TlLVOE8OFIQM7qNBYRPB6y2G57MAzJCYdJAfp5SBJDhGcEPN3FAR47Qeadi6D_khrcp6biiujeGqNVqW2iR5YpbnGjNFFcEiik93r0Z3ayhF1_8QgkYsI3nkKUlyUoFHh_QH-CkFg9SgPepSocKY_vd0eMij8Sv7ZnhG82k3TSrrEVrtm42mQZIjuJYIn3W7a_TRPMDJGgx9B3ttnPa70Z-rFDw8HLtAkFxxXvu12ZG_Jx8X3kWfk1WIj0btlXDz7__cfwu3x-XQiJ59mX57DHQwQC__2UhzA3rrduBcYhK31y6BpvwHFDTSJ
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfR1db9Mw8ASdNPGC-CYwmBEInkLTOnGSJ9SxVQO2akIM7c3yV0rRlJS0_f_cOW5ZQOLVPkvJfZ99HwBvitwqKyoXC27LOM0rEyuX5bEtnXXCaW39-LbzmTi9TD9fZVch_2kV0iq3OtEratsYuiMfjumFbYzGMxtWIS3i4nj6YfkrpglS9NIaxmnchr08FTwZwN7Ryezi6-7GBY2dQIej61GaYxgzXDrX_nyfFTTj_YZV8s37_1XRN2zU3_mTvS6j3jJN78Hd4FKySccD9-GWqx_A_nl4NH8Ii64EF91tNifNxlTrmApkcZbRVSzzs_rizsDhWneVzkIR_6Zli5rNm2tUUkzVlp0pDLJt07LWD-RCcWC2ma8eweX05NvH0zhMWIgNomIdo3AWGCJwYYvKoiRqmgGa2sxW6HVlGfVKRH_GUBe3UeLSMtG4XfiegYXTleKPYVA3tXsKrLAlL5V2Ltc8RbrrNDFcGFK2owoVRwSvtxiWy66RhsQAhOggPR0k0SGCI0L-DoKaX_uFpp3LIEvSmpznpuLKKJ5ao1WpbaJHVmmuMXp0ERwS6WRXSboTYTmhSaDoMHIRwTsPQUKMFDQq1CLgr1A7rB7kQQ8Shc_0t7fsIYPwr-QfVo3g1W6bTlJCW-2ajYdBkDGamgiedNy0-2meoJeMyj-CvMdnPaz0d-rFD98aXKB6LjiefNtxZO_I8eL7xCPyerGRaOkyLp79__sPYR-FTJ59mn15DnfQVyx8GaY4gMG63bgX6I-t9csgaL8BHT44tw
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sociality+genes+are+associated+with+human-directed+social+behaviour+in+golden+and+Labrador+retriever+dogs&rft.jtitle=PeerJ+%28San+Francisco%2C+CA%29&rft.au=Persson%2C+Mia+E&rft.au=Sundman%2C+Ann-Sofie&rft.au=Halld%C3%A9n%2C+Lise-Lotte&rft.au=Trottier%2C+Agaia+J&rft.date=2018-11-06&rft.pub=PeerJ%2C+Inc&rft.eissn=2167-8359&rft_id=info:doi/10.7717%2Fpeerj.5889&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2167-8359&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2167-8359&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2167-8359&client=summon