Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making

Significance Concern for the welfare of others is a key component of moral decision making and is disturbed in antisocial and criminal behavior. However, little is known about how people evaluate the costs of others’ suffering. Past studies have examined people’s judgments in hypothetical scenarios,...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS Vol. 111; no. 48; pp. 17320 - 17325
Main Authors Crockett, Molly J., Kurth-Nelson, Zeb, Siegel, Jenifer Z., Dayan, Peter, Dolan, Raymond J.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States National Academy of Sciences 02.12.2014
National Acad Sciences
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Significance Concern for the welfare of others is a key component of moral decision making and is disturbed in antisocial and criminal behavior. However, little is known about how people evaluate the costs of others’ suffering. Past studies have examined people’s judgments in hypothetical scenarios, but there is evidence that hypothetical judgments cannot accurately predict actual behavior. Here we addressed this issue by measuring how much money people will sacrifice to reduce the number of painful electric shocks delivered to either themselves or an anonymous stranger. Surprisingly, most people sacrifice more money to reduce a stranger’s pain than their own pain. This finding may help us better understand how people resolve moral dilemmas that commonly arise in medical, legal, and political decision making. Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others’ suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We investigated this question by inviting subjects to trade off profits for themselves against pain experienced either by themselves or an anonymous other person. Subjects made choices between different amounts of money and different numbers of painful electric shocks. We independently varied the recipient of the shocks (self vs. other) and whether the choice involved paying to decrease pain or profiting by increasing pain. We built computational models to quantify the relative values subjects ascribed to pain for themselves and others in this setting. In two studies we show that most people valued others’ pain more than their own pain. This was evident in a willingness to pay more to reduce others’ pain than their own and a requirement for more compensation to increase others’ pain relative to their own. This ”hyperaltruistic” valuation of others’ pain was linked to slower responding when making decisions that affected others, consistent with an engagement of deliberative processes in moral decision making. Subclinical psychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with reports of aversive processing deficits in psychopathy. Our results provide evidence for a circumstance in which people care more for others than themselves. Determining the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding human moral decision making and its disturbance in antisocial behavior.
AbstractList Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others’ suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We investigated this question by inviting subjects to trade off profits for themselves against pain experienced either by themselves or an anonymous other person. Subjects made choices between different amounts of money and different numbers of painful electric shocks. We independently varied the recipient of the shocks (self vs. other) and whether the choice involved paying to decrease pain or profiting by increasing pain. We built computational models to quantify the relative values subjects ascribed to pain for themselves and others in this setting. In two studies we show that most people valued others’ pain more than their own pain. This was evident in a willingness to pay more to reduce others’ pain than their own and a requirement for more compensation to increase others’ pain relative to their own. This ”hyperaltruistic” valuation of others’ pain was linked to slower responding when making decisions that affected others, consistent with an engagement of deliberative processes in moral decision making. Subclinical psychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with reports of aversive processing deficits in psychopathy. Our results provide evidence for a circumstance in which people care more for others than themselves. Determining the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding human moral decision making and its disturbance in antisocial behavior.
Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others' suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We investigated this question by inviting subjects to trade off profits for themselves against pain experienced either by themselves or an anonymous other person. Subjects made choices between different amounts of money and different numbers of painful electric shocks. We independently varied the recipient of the shocks (self vs. other) and whether the choice involved paying to decrease pain or profiting by increasing pain. We built computational models to quantify the relative values subjects ascribed to pain for themselves and others in this setting. In two studies we show that most people valued others' pain more than their own pain. This was evident in a willingness to pay more to reduce others' pain than their own and a requirement for more compensation to increase others' pain relative to their own. This "hyperaltruistic" valuation of others' pain was linked to slower responding when making decisions that affected others, consistent with an engagement of deliberative processes in moral decision making. Subclinical psychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with reports of eversive processing deficits in psychopathy. Our results provide evidence for a circumstance in which people care more for others than themselves. Determining the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding human moral decision making and its disturbance in antisocial behavior.
Concern for the welfare of others is a key component of moral decision making and is disturbed in antisocial and criminal behavior. However, little is known about how people evaluate the costs of others’ suffering. Past studies have examined people’s judgments in hypothetical scenarios, but there is evidence that hypothetical judgments cannot accurately predict actual behavior. Here we addressed this issue by measuring how much money people will sacrifice to reduce the number of painful electric shocks delivered to either themselves or an anonymous stranger. Surprisingly, most people sacrifice more money to reduce a stranger’s pain than their own pain. This finding may help us better understand how people resolve moral dilemmas that commonly arise in medical, legal, and political decision making. Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others’ suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We investigated this question by inviting subjects to trade off profits for themselves against pain experienced either by themselves or an anonymous other person. Subjects made choices between different amounts of money and different numbers of painful electric shocks. We independently varied the recipient of the shocks (self vs. other) and whether the choice involved paying to decrease pain or profiting by increasing pain. We built computational models to quantify the relative values subjects ascribed to pain for themselves and others in this setting. In two studies we show that most people valued others’ pain more than their own pain. This was evident in a willingness to pay more to reduce others’ pain than their own and a requirement for more compensation to increase others’ pain relative to their own. This ‟hyperaltruistic” valuation of others’ pain was linked to slower responding when making decisions that affected others, consistent with an engagement of deliberative processes in moral decision making. Subclinical psychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with reports of aversive processing deficits in psychopathy. Our results provide evidence for a circumstance in which people care more for others than themselves. Determining the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding human moral decision making and its disturbance in antisocial behavior.
Significance Concern for the welfare of others is a key component of moral decision making and is disturbed in antisocial and criminal behavior. However, little is known about how people evaluate the costs of others’ suffering. Past studies have examined people’s judgments in hypothetical scenarios, but there is evidence that hypothetical judgments cannot accurately predict actual behavior. Here we addressed this issue by measuring how much money people will sacrifice to reduce the number of painful electric shocks delivered to either themselves or an anonymous stranger. Surprisingly, most people sacrifice more money to reduce a stranger’s pain than their own pain. This finding may help us better understand how people resolve moral dilemmas that commonly arise in medical, legal, and political decision making. Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others’ suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We investigated this question by inviting subjects to trade off profits for themselves against pain experienced either by themselves or an anonymous other person. Subjects made choices between different amounts of money and different numbers of painful electric shocks. We independently varied the recipient of the shocks (self vs. other) and whether the choice involved paying to decrease pain or profiting by increasing pain. We built computational models to quantify the relative values subjects ascribed to pain for themselves and others in this setting. In two studies we show that most people valued others’ pain more than their own pain. This was evident in a willingness to pay more to reduce others’ pain than their own and a requirement for more compensation to increase others’ pain relative to their own. This ”hyperaltruistic” valuation of others’ pain was linked to slower responding when making decisions that affected others, consistent with an engagement of deliberative processes in moral decision making. Subclinical psychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with reports of aversive processing deficits in psychopathy. Our results provide evidence for a circumstance in which people care more for others than themselves. Determining the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding human moral decision making and its disturbance in antisocial behavior.
Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others' suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We investigated this question by inviting subjects to trade off profits for themselves against pain experienced either by themselves or an anonymous other person. Subjects made choices between different amounts of money and different numbers of painful electric shocks. We independently varied the recipient of the shocks (self vs. other) and whether the choice involved paying to decrease pain or profiting by increasing pain. We built computational models to quantify the relative values subjects ascribed to pain for themselves and others in this setting. In two studies we show that most people valued others' pain more than their own pain. This was evident in a willingness to pay more to reduce others' pain than their own and a requirement for more compensation to increase others' pain relative to their own. This "hyperaltruistic" valuation of others' pain was linked to slower responding when making decisions that affected others, consistent with an engagement of deliberative processes in moral decision making. Subclinical psychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with reports of aversive processing deficits in psychopathy. Our results provide evidence for a circumstance in which people care more for others than themselves. Determining the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding human moral decision making and its disturbance in antisocial behavior.Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others' suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We investigated this question by inviting subjects to trade off profits for themselves against pain experienced either by themselves or an anonymous other person. Subjects made choices between different amounts of money and different numbers of painful electric shocks. We independently varied the recipient of the shocks (self vs. other) and whether the choice involved paying to decrease pain or profiting by increasing pain. We built computational models to quantify the relative values subjects ascribed to pain for themselves and others in this setting. In two studies we show that most people valued others' pain more than their own pain. This was evident in a willingness to pay more to reduce others' pain than their own and a requirement for more compensation to increase others' pain relative to their own. This "hyperaltruistic" valuation of others' pain was linked to slower responding when making decisions that affected others, consistent with an engagement of deliberative processes in moral decision making. Subclinical psychopathic traits correlated negatively with aversion to pain for both self and others, in line with reports of aversive processing deficits in psychopathy. Our results provide evidence for a circumstance in which people care more for others than themselves. Determining the precise boundaries of this surprisingly prosocial disposition has implications for understanding human moral decision making and its disturbance in antisocial behavior.
Author Siegel, Jenifer Z.
Dayan, Peter
Dolan, Raymond J.
Crockett, Molly J.
Kurth-Nelson, Zeb
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Molly J.
  surname: Crockett
  fullname: Crockett, Molly J.
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Zeb
  surname: Kurth-Nelson
  fullname: Kurth-Nelson, Zeb
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Jenifer Z.
  surname: Siegel
  fullname: Siegel, Jenifer Z.
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Peter
  surname: Dayan
  fullname: Dayan, Peter
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Raymond J.
  surname: Dolan
  fullname: Dolan, Raymond J.
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25404350$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkc1v1DAQxS1URLeFMycgEhcuaWf8FftSCVVAkSpxgJ4tx3F2vWTjxc4W8d_jaJcFeoCTZb3fvHn2OyMnYxw9Ic8RLhAadrkdbb5ADkorhYiPyAJBYy25hhOyAKBNrTjlp-Qs5zUAaKHgCTmlggNnAhbk6samTTXFKk4rn3IVd9N3H5arXK0OQvZDX4Wx2sRkh6rzLuQQy9V-DePyKXnc2yH7Z4fznNy9f_fl-qa-_fTh4_Xb29oJjVPd245j5xtn2955Bp7KXnVMN1w6J7umcayjSlEEFLxtXctsKwXqVoOVBWTn5Grvu921G985P04ljdmmsLHph4k2mL-VMazMMt4bTiUI1RSDNweDFL_tfJ7MJmTnh8GOPu6yQQUMlJBc_R-VVGshQUNBXz9A13GXxvIThWK87BU4G778M_wx9a8WCnC5B1yKOSffHxEEM_ds5p7N757LhHgw4cJkp1JMeX0Y_jFXHaLMwnELouHKYMPoHObFHlnnKaYjwxltlBTza17t9d5GY5cpZHP3mQJKgLKII2M_ATqfyPg
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1002_ejp_1809
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep09916
crossref_primary_10_1093_jcr_ucaa018
crossref_primary_10_3390_ijerph19138174
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2023_119881
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhad144
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_74975_0
crossref_primary_10_1093_scan_nsab100
crossref_primary_10_1027_1618_3169_a000336
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2020_08_003
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01411
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_015_0959_4
crossref_primary_10_1177_19485506211027794
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976221122765
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dr_2024_101183
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1601280113
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1939_20_2021
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_13590
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_021_02049_z
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lmot_2021_101731
crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_020_01168_w
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1010010
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_isci_2020_101772
crossref_primary_10_1080_02699931_2019_1669536
crossref_primary_10_3390_ijerph192416605
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2024_104589
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2022_105104
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1385_19_2020
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijchp_2024_100533
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2016_10_052
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2429787
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_paid_2021_110714
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_00668
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_023_00509_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chbah_2024_100097
crossref_primary_10_12677_AP_2021_119225
crossref_primary_10_1002_ejp_1940
crossref_primary_10_3390_bs8010010
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_018_0372_x
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797618800042
crossref_primary_10_1287_mnsc_2018_3216
crossref_primary_10_3389_frobt_2021_603510
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976211042379
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2436750
crossref_primary_10_1002_pchj_822
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_021_26067_4
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneumeth_2018_03_002
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1603198113
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_018_0425_1
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1930297500002916
crossref_primary_10_3390_ijerph192416984
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2018_04_009
crossref_primary_10_1017_epi_2021_31
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jesp_2022_104371
crossref_primary_10_1080_1047840X_2020_1750925
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jesp_2020_104048
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_018_0572_x
crossref_primary_10_1017_apa_2021_1
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2617668
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2016_10_008
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2015_1004334
crossref_primary_10_1177_1948550617720275
crossref_primary_10_3724_SP_J_1041_2022_00613
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2021_0293
crossref_primary_10_1093_scan_nsy056
crossref_primary_10_4204_EPTCS_215_9
crossref_primary_10_1093_scan_nsy057
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976231221990
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0140580
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1911861117
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_767022
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10869_016_9484_3
crossref_primary_10_1111_jpr_12473
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2024_101884
crossref_primary_10_1177_0033294119896061
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2018_02_039
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2799850
crossref_primary_10_4236_psych_2015_68090
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_018_0308_5
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_12852
crossref_primary_10_14254_2071_789X_2023_16_1_3
crossref_primary_10_1093_scan_nsaa029
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bpsc_2019_07_008
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976231168777
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jesp_2021_104102
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0173405
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnbeh_2015_00287
crossref_primary_10_1002_pchj_651
crossref_primary_10_1093_jcr_ucad011
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyt_2021_706474
crossref_primary_10_1177_17456916211031926
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2014_12_007
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2024_101672
crossref_primary_10_1177_19485506231167231
crossref_primary_10_1177_10888683221114215
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_02194
crossref_primary_10_26599_BSA_2019_9050020
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_70199_4
crossref_primary_10_1177_0033294116685866
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_13653
crossref_primary_10_1111_tops_12382
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2209078119
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2725550
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797620948821
crossref_primary_10_3390_bs12010006
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2015_05_036
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0020818322000030
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2505920
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_15294
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1036624
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_024_01214_8
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_023_36807_3
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2018_1518834
crossref_primary_10_1177_19485506231168515
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_017_0131
crossref_primary_10_1360_TB_2022_0699
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0033291722002483
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1930297500006987
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_211983
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhac017
crossref_primary_10_1177_1745691618792261
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2018_09_007
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2018_09_001
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0117426
crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_631
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2022_103575
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_023_37283_5
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976221119727
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_021_01566_1
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2015_05_021
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2016_08_015
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1614111113
crossref_primary_10_1017_bpp_2019_4
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep15389
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0150523
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797616685771
crossref_primary_10_1177_0963721415624012
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psyneuen_2020_104860
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976231173900
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2015_08_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_joep_2018_09_008
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_010419_050830
crossref_primary_10_1177_0146167218757465
crossref_primary_10_1111_jgs_15804
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1237_20_2020
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_copsyc_2015_07_006
crossref_primary_10_1177_1043463119885102
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_160605
crossref_primary_10_1002_ejsp_2896
crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_721
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_2816543
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_19163_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2017_03_003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2020_03_009
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2015_02_009
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_29614_9
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12264_021_00808_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2023_105708
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ssci_2020_105116
crossref_primary_10_1186_s40359_023_01093_7
crossref_primary_10_1038_nn_4557
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2021_01_013
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyt_2023_1170150
crossref_primary_10_1080_10810730_2017_1329853
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnbeh_2015_00135
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_22226_9
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2021_118730
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2021_118211
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2017_05_004
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_3051673
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1706693114
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_geb_2021_10_005
crossref_primary_10_5964_jspp_5453
crossref_primary_10_3724_SP_J_1042_2022_00877
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12702
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2020_09_001
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jebo_2016_12_003
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_97617_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_appdev_2019_101069
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2023_105580
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00372_2017
crossref_primary_10_1111_puar_13190
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11098_015_0469_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_physa_2018_08_008
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0153577
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2301641120
crossref_primary_10_1111_josp_12250
crossref_primary_10_1111_spc3_12327
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_obhdp_2018_05_005
crossref_primary_10_1162_imag_a_00277
crossref_primary_10_1002_pmh_1395
Cites_doi 10.1038/ncomms4677
10.1521/soco.1986.4.4.353
10.1126/science.1182238
10.2307/1914185
10.1177/1948550612457688
10.1016/S1053-8119(18)31587-8
10.2307/2941024
10.1038/nature11467
10.1086/519249
10.1214/aos/1176344136
10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-P
10.2307/2234133
10.1176/ajp.121.6.584
10.1086/406755
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02304.x
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2513-09.2009
10.1038/nrn3577
10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.001
10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000254
10.1561/0700000047
10.1177/0963721413492764
10.1017/CBO9780511790850
10.1037/0022-3514.40.2.290
10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.007
10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7
10.1093/oseo/instance.00042831
10.1126/science.1093535
10.1073/pnas.1112324108
10.1023/A:1004241214825
10.1073/pnas.1102131108
10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003
10.1037/a0019737
10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.005
10.3386/w0042
10.1002/bdm.3960030404
10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.003
10.1093/clipsy.8.1.117
10.1038/nn.3279
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright copyright © 1993–2008 National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
Copyright National Academy of Sciences Dec 2, 2014
Copyright_xml – notice: copyright © 1993–2008 National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
– notice: Copyright National Academy of Sciences Dec 2, 2014
DBID FBQ
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QL
7QP
7QR
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TK
7TM
7TO
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
M7N
P64
RC3
7X8
7S9
L.6
5PM
DOI 10.1073/pnas.1408988111
DatabaseName AGRIS
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)
Immunology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
Engineering Research Database
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
AGRICOLA
AGRICOLA - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
Entomology Abstracts
Genetics Abstracts
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Immunology Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
AGRICOLA
AGRICOLA - Academic
DatabaseTitleList AGRICOLA

MEDLINE


CrossRef
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: FBQ
  name: AGRIS
  url: http://www.fao.org/agris/Centre.asp?Menu_1ID=DB&Menu_2ID=DB1&Language=EN&Content=http://www.fao.org/agris/search?Language=EN
  sourceTypes: Publisher
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Sciences (General)
DocumentTitleAlternate Harm to others outweighs harm to self
EISSN 1091-6490
EndPage 17325
ExternalDocumentID PMC4260587
3521676531
25404350
10_1073_pnas_1408988111
111_48_17320
43278658
US201600140413
Genre Randomized Controlled Trial
Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
Feature
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Wellcome Trust
  grantid: 098362
– fundername: Wellcome Trust
  grantid: 091593
– fundername: Wellcome Trust
  grantid: 078865/Z/05/Z
– fundername: Wellcome Trust
  grantid: 092217/Z/10/Z
– fundername: Wellcome Trust
  grantid: 091593/Z/10/Z
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-~X
.55
.GJ
0R~
123
29P
2AX
2FS
2WC
3O-
4.4
53G
5RE
5VS
692
6TJ
79B
85S
AACGO
AAFWJ
AANCE
AAYJJ
ABBHK
ABOCM
ABPLY
ABPPZ
ABPTK
ABTLG
ABZEH
ACGOD
ACIWK
ACKIV
ACNCT
ACPRK
ADULT
ADZLD
AENEX
AEUPB
AEXZC
AFDAS
AFFNX
AFOSN
AFRAH
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ASUFR
AS~
BKOMP
CS3
D0L
DCCCD
DIK
DNJUQ
DOOOF
DU5
DWIUU
E3Z
EBS
EJD
F20
F5P
FBQ
FRP
GX1
HGD
HH5
HQ3
HTVGU
HYE
JAAYA
JBMMH
JENOY
JHFFW
JKQEH
JLS
JLXEF
JPM
JSG
JSODD
JST
KQ8
L7B
LU7
MVM
N9A
NEJ
NHB
N~3
O9-
OK1
P-O
PNE
PQQKQ
R.V
RHF
RHI
RNA
RNS
RPM
RXW
SA0
SJN
TAE
TN5
UKR
VOH
VQA
W8F
WH7
WHG
WOQ
WOW
X7M
XFK
XSW
Y6R
YBH
YKV
YSK
ZA5
ZCA
ZCG
~02
~KM
ABXSQ
ACHIC
ADQXQ
ADXHL
AQVQM
H13
IPSME
-
02
0R
1AW
55
AAPBV
ABFLS
ADACO
DZ
KM
PQEST
X
XHC
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QL
7QP
7QR
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TK
7TM
7TO
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
M7N
P64
RC3
7X8
7S9
L.6
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c591t-fad41de7cabfce30e26f8d39746cc6d77c3d288210154bbcb3ab6519b90a68d33
ISSN 0027-8424
1091-6490
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:15:52 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 23:33:05 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 02:59:45 EDT 2025
Mon Jun 30 08:30:11 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 06:06:21 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:55:12 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:53:17 EDT 2025
Wed Nov 11 00:30:04 EST 2020
Thu May 29 08:43:08 EDT 2025
Wed Dec 27 19:18:15 EST 2023
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 48
Keywords social preferences
valuation
altruism
morality
decision making
Language English
License Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c591t-fad41de7cabfce30e26f8d39746cc6d77c3d288210154bbcb3ab6519b90a68d33
Notes http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1408988111
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ObjectType-Undefined-3
Author contributions: M.J.C., Z.K.-N., J.Z.S., P.D., and R.J.D. designed research; M.J.C. and J.Z.S. performed research; M.J.C., Z.K.-N., and P.D. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; M.J.C., Z.K.-N., J.Z.S., and P.D. analyzed data; and M.J.C., Z.K.-N., J.Z.S., P.D., and R.J.D. wrote the paper.
Edited by Daniel Kahneman, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved October 22, 2014 (received for review May 14, 2014)
OpenAccessLink https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/111/48/17320.full.pdf
PMID 25404350
PQID 1634873518
PQPubID 42026
PageCount 6
ParticipantIDs proquest_journals_1634873518
crossref_citationtrail_10_1073_pnas_1408988111
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4260587
jstor_primary_43278658
proquest_miscellaneous_1803085648
pubmed_primary_25404350
pnas_primary_111_48_17320
fao_agris_US201600140413
proquest_miscellaneous_1629956090
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1408988111
ProviderPackageCode RNA
PNE
CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2014-12-02
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2014-12-02
PublicationDate_xml – month: 12
  year: 2014
  text: 2014-12-02
  day: 02
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: Washington
PublicationTitle Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS
PublicationTitleAlternate Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
PublicationYear 2014
Publisher National Academy of Sciences
National Acad Sciences
Publisher_xml – name: National Academy of Sciences
– name: National Acad Sciences
References Kahneman D (e_1_3_3_10_2) 2013
e_1_3_3_17_2
e_1_3_3_16_2
e_1_3_3_19_2
e_1_3_3_38_2
e_1_3_3_39_2
e_1_3_3_13_2
e_1_3_3_36_2
e_1_3_3_12_2
e_1_3_3_15_2
e_1_3_3_34_2
e_1_3_3_14_2
e_1_3_3_35_2
e_1_3_3_32_2
e_1_3_3_11_2
e_1_3_3_30_2
e_1_3_3_31_2
e_1_3_3_40_2
Harsanyi JC (e_1_3_3_21_2) 1977; 44
Evans AM (e_1_3_3_33_2) 2014
e_1_3_3_6_2
Burnham KP (e_1_3_3_20_2) 2002
e_1_3_3_5_2
e_1_3_3_8_2
e_1_3_3_7_2
e_1_3_3_28_2
e_1_3_3_9_2
e_1_3_3_27_2
e_1_3_3_29_2
e_1_3_3_24_2
e_1_3_3_23_2
e_1_3_3_26_2
Thorndike EL (e_1_3_3_37_2) 1937; 51
e_1_3_3_25_2
e_1_3_3_2_2
e_1_3_3_43_2
e_1_3_3_1_2
e_1_3_3_44_2
e_1_3_3_4_2
Daw ND (e_1_3_3_18_2) 2011
e_1_3_3_22_2
e_1_3_3_41_2
e_1_3_3_3_2
e_1_3_3_42_2
20299588 - Science. 2010 Mar 19;327(5972):1480-4
22106300 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Dec 6;108(49):19761-6
22996558 - Nature. 2012 Sep 20;489(7416):427-30
23856025 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2013 Aug;17(8):413-25
23845564 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2013 Aug;17(8):363-6
20920798 - Neuron. 2010 Oct 6;68(1):149-60
23222911 - Nat Neurosci. 2013 Jan;16(1):105-10
7587017 - Cognition. 1995 Oct;57(1):1-29
19776285 - J Neurosci. 2009 Sep 23;29(38):11993-9
14239459 - Am J Psychiatry. 1964 Dec;121:584-5
24105343 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013 Nov;14(11):786-99
21788489 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Aug 9;108(32):13335-40
19254237 - Psychol Sci. 2009 Mar;20(3):309-17
14976305 - Science. 2004 Feb 20;303(5661):1157-62
19112488 - PLoS Comput Biol. 2008 Dec;4(12):e1000254
20658856 - Psychol Rev. 2010 Jul;117(3):864-901
25572965 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015 Jan 27;112(4):E381
15050515 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2004 Apr;8(4):185-90
24751464 - Nat Commun. 2014;5:3677
References_xml – ident: e_1_3_3_34_2
  doi: 10.1038/ncomms4677
– ident: e_1_3_3_26_2
  doi: 10.1521/soco.1986.4.4.353
– ident: e_1_3_3_5_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.1182238
– ident: e_1_3_3_17_2
  doi: 10.2307/1914185
– volume: 44
  start-page: 623
  year: 1977
  ident: e_1_3_3_21_2
  article-title: Morality and the theory of rational behavior
  publication-title: Soc Res (New York)
– ident: e_1_3_3_22_2
  doi: 10.1177/1948550612457688
– ident: e_1_3_3_44_2
  doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(18)31587-8
– ident: e_1_3_3_16_2
  doi: 10.2307/2941024
– ident: e_1_3_3_4_2
  doi: 10.1038/nature11467
– ident: e_1_3_3_41_2
  doi: 10.1086/519249
– ident: e_1_3_3_19_2
  doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136
– ident: e_1_3_3_1_2
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-P
– ident: e_1_3_3_39_2
  doi: 10.2307/2234133
– ident: e_1_3_3_7_2
  doi: 10.1176/ajp.121.6.584
– ident: e_1_3_3_42_2
  doi: 10.1086/406755
– volume: 51
  start-page: 227
  year: 1937
  ident: e_1_3_3_37_2
  article-title: Valuations of certain pains, deprivations, and frustrations
  publication-title: Pedagog Semin J Genet Psychol
– ident: e_1_3_3_14_2
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02304.x
– ident: e_1_3_3_23_2
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2513-09.2009
– ident: e_1_3_3_24_2
  doi: 10.1038/nrn3577
– ident: e_1_3_3_40_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.001
– ident: e_1_3_3_43_2
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000254
– ident: e_1_3_3_2_2
  doi: 10.1561/0700000047
– volume-title: Decision Making, Affect, and Learning: Attention and Performance XXIII
  year: 2011
  ident: e_1_3_3_18_2
– volume-title: Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach
  year: 2002
  ident: e_1_3_3_20_2
– ident: e_1_3_3_30_2
  doi: 10.1177/0963721413492764
– ident: e_1_3_3_27_2
  doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511790850
– volume-title: Thinking, Fast and Slow
  year: 2013
  ident: e_1_3_3_10_2
– ident: e_1_3_3_6_2
  doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.40.2.290
– ident: e_1_3_3_13_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.007
– ident: e_1_3_3_3_2
  doi: 10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7
– ident: e_1_3_3_12_2
  doi: 10.1093/oseo/instance.00042831
– ident: e_1_3_3_8_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.1093535
– ident: e_1_3_3_31_2
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112324108
– ident: e_1_3_3_15_2
  doi: 10.1023/A:1004241214825
– ident: e_1_3_3_35_2
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102131108
– ident: e_1_3_3_9_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003
– ident: e_1_3_3_29_2
  doi: 10.1037/a0019737
– ident: e_1_3_3_32_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.005
– ident: e_1_3_3_38_2
  doi: 10.3386/w0042
– ident: e_1_3_3_11_2
  doi: 10.1002/bdm.3960030404
– ident: e_1_3_3_36_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.003
– volume-title: Reaction Times and Reflection in Social Dilemmas: Extreme Responses are Fast, but Not Intuitive
  year: 2014
  ident: e_1_3_3_33_2
– ident: e_1_3_3_25_2
  doi: 10.1093/clipsy.8.1.117
– ident: e_1_3_3_28_2
  doi: 10.1038/nn.3279
– reference: 20920798 - Neuron. 2010 Oct 6;68(1):149-60
– reference: 14239459 - Am J Psychiatry. 1964 Dec;121:584-5
– reference: 20299588 - Science. 2010 Mar 19;327(5972):1480-4
– reference: 23222911 - Nat Neurosci. 2013 Jan;16(1):105-10
– reference: 23856025 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2013 Aug;17(8):413-25
– reference: 14976305 - Science. 2004 Feb 20;303(5661):1157-62
– reference: 19776285 - J Neurosci. 2009 Sep 23;29(38):11993-9
– reference: 20658856 - Psychol Rev. 2010 Jul;117(3):864-901
– reference: 15050515 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2004 Apr;8(4):185-90
– reference: 22106300 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Dec 6;108(49):19761-6
– reference: 24105343 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013 Nov;14(11):786-99
– reference: 25572965 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015 Jan 27;112(4):E381
– reference: 23845564 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2013 Aug;17(8):363-6
– reference: 7587017 - Cognition. 1995 Oct;57(1):1-29
– reference: 21788489 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Aug 9;108(32):13335-40
– reference: 22996558 - Nature. 2012 Sep 20;489(7416):427-30
– reference: 24751464 - Nat Commun. 2014;5:3677
– reference: 19254237 - Psychol Sci. 2009 Mar;20(3):309-17
– reference: 19112488 - PLoS Comput Biol. 2008 Dec;4(12):e1000254
SSID ssj0009580
Score 2.5701425
Snippet Significance Concern for the welfare of others is a key component of moral decision making and is disturbed in antisocial and criminal behavior. However,...
Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others' suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We...
Concern for the welfare of others is a key component of moral decision making and is disturbed in antisocial and criminal behavior. However, little is known...
Concern for the suffering of others is central to moral decision making. How humans evaluate others’ suffering, relative to their own suffering, is unknown. We...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
pnas
jstor
fao
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 17320
SubjectTerms Algorithms
Altruism
Behavior
Biological altruism
Biological Sciences
Choice Behavior - physiology
Decision Making
Economic value
Electroshock - adverse effects
Empathy
Experimentation
Female
Hardship
Humans
Loss aversion
Male
Models, Psychological
Moral judgment
Morality
Morals
Pain
Pain - etiology
Pain - psychology
Pain Threshold - psychology
Parametric models
people
politics
Prosocial behavior
Reaction Time - physiology
Self
Self destructive behavior
Sex Factors
Social Sciences
Surveys and Questionnaires
Valuation
Young Adult
Title Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making
URI https://www.jstor.org/stable/43278658
http://www.pnas.org/content/111/48/17320.abstract
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25404350
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1634873518
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1629956090
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1803085648
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4260587
Volume 111
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Rb9MwELa68cILYsBYYKAg8TAUZSSx4zgvSNMAVZNWTWKVpr1ETuLQiTVFayoE7_xv7hzHSaduAl6iNravqe9yd7bvviPkbVWElUx56acqFz5aKHjnqsKXMacyCGRFdd7a6YSPp-zkIr4YjX4PopZWTX5Y_NqYV_I_XIV7wFfMkv0HzlqicAM-A3_hChyG61_xeCxv5ug86iyqpbdYNT9wpxMrpLQNS3WNiBDeXOfhl6aejjfXJaiGfumZtWPLLmpg0m0THvVJJ0YTLD3fO5v0JYyPYfQ3E_B7isWrvZPDwRlRM_MnqkOFvFS53dXRB_YmxgZDbLxLO-yj_NluzfYBxGZvItQIiEG_kr3vQYdKOQIBYW0qtVXKRgW30teCcRodGyY0CgYGG7_HG60BqC8sYVzLJRiEQKRCdGTXIbYZjRIBrtgWeRDBakPHh46H2M2izWQyz9khRCX0_S3aa87NViUXXZQrQudC103LmNvRuAP35vwxeWTWJe5RK2Q7ZKTqJ2Snm0b3wMCTv3tKPqDUuc3CbaXOtVLnzkwDSp17Vbta6txO6txW6p6R6edP58dj31Th8Is4DRu_kiULS5UUMq8KRQMV8UqU4MYyXhS8TJKClhGs00L0xvO8yKnMOawL8jSQHDrSXbJdL2q1R9ySsVjkXIJxjlisVArmMihKzlKGqWTcIYfd9GWFgajHSinXmQ6VSGiGk5j18-2QAzvge4vOcnfXPeBHJr-C7cymXyJEVtTYUiF1yK5mkiXRyYNDHE3FkobFMhOZFkCH7HeszIxKgJ_jlImExiEMfWObQWHjKZys1WKFfSLMJg_S4J4-AmGkYs6AzvNWOuxDRDECYsUwOlmTG9sBAePXW-qrmQaOx2oUsUhe3PV_X5KH_Vu8T7abm5V6BT53k7_Wb8QfGGbSnA
linkProvider ABC ChemistRy
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Harm+to+others+outweighs+harm+to+self+in+moral+decision+making&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+National+Academy+of+Sciences+-+PNAS&rft.au=Crockett%2C+Molly+J.&rft.au=Kurth-Nelson%2C+Zeb&rft.au=Siegel%2C+Jenifer+Z.&rft.au=Dayan%2C+Peter&rft.date=2014-12-02&rft.pub=National+Academy+of+Sciences&rft.issn=0027-8424&rft.volume=111&rft.issue=48&rft.spage=17320&rft.epage=17325&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073%2Fpnas.1408988111&rft.externalDocID=43278658
thumbnail_m http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/image/custom?url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.pnas.org%2Fcontent%2F111%2F48.cover.gif
thumbnail_s http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/image/custom?url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.pnas.org%2Fcontent%2F111%2F48.cover.gif