Star finches Neochmia ruficauda have a visual preference for white dot patterns: a possible case of trypophilia
Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals,...
Saved in:
Published in | Animal cognition Vol. 25; no. 5; pp. 1271 - 1279 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Berlin/Heidelberg
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
01.10.2022
Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1435-9448 1435-9456 1435-9456 |
DOI | 10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch
Neochmia ruficauda
) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis. Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda ) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis. Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis.Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis. |
Author | Mizuno, Ayumi Soma, Masayo |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Ayumi orcidid: 0000-0003-0822-5637 surname: Mizuno fullname: Mizuno, Ayumi organization: Biosystems Science Course, The Graduate School of Life Science, Hokkaido University – sequence: 2 givenname: Masayo orcidid: 0000-0002-8596-1956 surname: Soma fullname: Soma, Masayo email: masayo.soma@sci.hokudai.ac.jp organization: Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35294684$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kU9vFSEUxYmpsX_0C7gwJG7cjALDwODCpGlsNWl0oa7JfbxLh2beMALzTL-9PF9btYtugITfOTn3nmNyMMUJCXnJ2VvOmH6XdydvmBAN44qZpntCjrhsu8bITh3cv2V_SI5zvmaM9dLwZ-Sw7YSRqpdHJH4rkKgPkxsw0y8Y3bAJQNPig4NlDXSALVKg25AXGOmc0GPCySH1MdFfQyhI17HQGUrBNOX3lZ1jzmE1InWQkUZPS7qZ4zyEMcBz8tTDmPHF7X1Cfpx__H72qbn8evH57PSycV0vS-ORtUyt1Iq1QutO94YZ3XLN_VpIbzovhNQeeq6d5IJ7UGjWCBx8byR0sj0hH_a-87La4NrhVBKMdk5hA-nGRgj2_58pDPYqbq1RXPeSV4M3twYp_lwwF7sJ2eE4woRxyVYoWbNJo7uKvn6AXsclTXU8K7Qwxiit-kq9-jfRfZS7LirQ7wGX6gLroq0LBUqIu4BhtJzZXeF2X7uttds_tdtdAvFAeuf-qKjdi3KFpytMf2M_ovoNkKS_8w |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2023_08_006 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_231057 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8 10.1098/rstb.1993.0057 10.1111/eth.12071 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00190.x 10.1080/03949370.2020.1803414 10.1016/s0169-5347(98)01471-2 10.1038/nature03363 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.024 10.1098/rspb.1999.0900 10.1098/rspb.2001.1891 10.1086/663197 10.1093/cz/zoy054 10.1038/343066a0 10.7717/peerj.2658 10.1093/beheco/10.6.688 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1990.tb05200.x 10.1111/brv.12171 10.2502/janip.68.2.2 10.1038/srep16614 10.1371/journal.pone.0172655 10.1007/s00265-007-0370-x 10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747 10.1126/science.250.4982.808 10.1098/rspb.1999.0598 10.1093/oso/9780199694532.001.0001 10.1007/s10336-020-01851-z 10.1007/s00114-012-0914-4 10.1093/beheco/ary021 10.1007/s002650050581 10.1006/anbe.2003.2126 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00612.x 10.1086/285303 10.1007/BF00604008 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 10.1111/eea.12176 10.1098/rspb.1994.0172 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01806.x 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.031 10.1080/02699931.2017.1345721 10.1371/journal.pone.0026485 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8 10.1098/rspb.1995.0045 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x 10.1073/pnas.0600625103 10.1006/anbe.2002.1999 10.1098/rstb.2017.0207 10.1086/444443 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb00540.x 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01986.x 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00454.x 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1958.tb00695.x 10.1126/science.281.5385.1999 10.1242/jeb.01047 10.1093/beheco/arw057 10.1007/s10336-007-0213-6 10.1073/pnas.0903327106 10.3389/fevo.2015.00004 10.1093/beheco/3.1.53 10.1007/s10336-012-0855-x 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.015 10.1098/rstb.2016.0352 10.1016/0022-5193(77)90061-3 10.1007/s00265-010-1067-0 10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001 10.1371/journal.pone.0003335 10.1098/rsos.160195 10.1093/cz/zoy052 10.1086/285308 10.1177/0956797613484937 10.1111/jeb.13102 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00174.x 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0131 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author(s) 2022 2022. The Author(s). Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Oct 2022 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author(s) 2022 – notice: 2022. The Author(s). – notice: Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Oct 2022 |
DBID | C6C AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 3V. 7QG 7SN 7TK 7X7 7XB 88E 88G 8FI 8FJ 8FK 8G5 ABUWG AEUYN AFKRA AZQEC BENPR C1K CCPQU DWQXO FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ GUQSH K9. M0S M1P M2M M2O MBDVC PHGZM PHGZT PJZUB PKEHL PPXIY PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PSYQQ Q9U 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5 |
DatabaseName | Springer Nature Link CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed ProQuest Central (Corporate) Animal Behavior Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) Psychology Database (Alumni) Hospital Premium Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) Research Library (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest One Sustainability ProQuest Central UK/Ireland ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest One ProQuest Central ProQuest Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Research Library ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Health & Medical Collection Medical Database Psychology Database Proquest Research Library Research Library (Corporate) ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Basic MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest One Psychology Research Library Prep ProQuest Central Student ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest One Health & Nursing Research Library (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central China Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management ProQuest Central ProQuest One Sustainability ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection Health Research Premium Collection Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest Research Library ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni) Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest Medical Library ProQuest Psychology Journals ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition Animal Behavior Abstracts ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef ProQuest One Psychology |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: C6C name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals url: http://www.springeropen.com/ sourceTypes: Publisher – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 4 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Zoology Psychology |
EISSN | 1435-9456 |
EndPage | 1279 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC9617841 35294684 10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science grantid: 23680027; 16H06177; 20K06809 funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001691 – fundername: The Japan Science Society grantid: 2020–5012 – fundername: Sumitomo Foundation grantid: 190272 funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100008608 – fundername: The Japan Science Society grantid: 2020-5012 – fundername: Sumitomo Foundation grantid: 190272 – fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science grantid: 20K06809 – fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science grantid: 16H06177 – fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science grantid: 23680027 – fundername: ; grantid: 190272 – fundername: ; grantid: 2020–5012 – fundername: ; grantid: 23680027; 16H06177; 20K06809 |
GroupedDBID | --- -56 -5G -BR -DZ -EM -Y2 -~C .86 06C 06D 0R~ 0VY 199 1N0 1SB 203 23M 29~ 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 2P1 2VQ 2~H 30V 36B 3V. 4.4 406 408 409 40D 40E 53G 5GY 5VS 67N 67Z 6NX 7X7 88E 8FI 8FJ 8G5 8TC 8UJ 95- 95. 95~ 96X AAAVM AABHQ AAGAY AAHBH AAHNG AAIAL AAJKR AAJSJ AAKKN AANXM AANZL AARHV AARTL AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYQN AAYTO AAYZH ABAKF ABBBX ABBXA ABDZT ABECU ABEEZ ABFTV ABHLI ABHQN ABIVO ABJNI ABJOX ABKCH ABKTR ABMNI ABMOR ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABQBU ABQSL ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTMW ABULA ABUWG ABWNU ABXPI ACACY ACBXY ACGFS ACHSB ACHXU ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACOKC ACOMO ACPRK ACSNA ACULB ACZOJ ADBBV ADHHG ADHIR ADINQ ADKNI ADKPE ADRFC ADTPH ADURQ ADYFF ADZKW AEBTG AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEKMD AENEX AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETLH AEUYN AEVLU AEXYK AFBBN AFGCZ AFGXO AFKRA AFLOW AFPKN AFQWF AFRAH AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGGDS AGJBK AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGRTI AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHAVH AHBYD AHKAY AHMBA AHSBF AHYZX AIAKS AIIXL AILAN AITGF AJBLW AJRNO AJZVZ AKMHD ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALWAN AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AMYQR AOCGG ARMRJ ASPBG AVWKF AXYYD AZFZN AZQEC B-. BA0 BDATZ BENPR BGNMA BPHCQ BVXVI C24 C6C CAG CCPQU COF CS3 CSCUP DDRTE DL5 DNIVK DPUIP DWQXO EBD EBLON EBS EIOEI EJD EMB EMOBN EN4 ESBYG F5P FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FINBP FNLPD FRRFC FSGXE FWDCC FYUFA G-Y G-Z GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNUQQ GNWQR GQ6 GQ7 GQ8 GROUPED_DOAJ GUQSH GXS H13 HF~ HG5 HG6 HMCUK HMJXF HQYDN HRMNR HVGLF HZ~ I09 IHE IJ- IKXTQ ITM IWAJR IXC IXE IZIGR IZQ I~X I~Z J-C J0Z JBSCW JCJTX JZLTJ KDC KOV KPH LAS LLZTM M1P M2M M2O M4Y MA- MQGED N2Q NB0 NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O9- O93 O9I O9J OAM OVD P2P PF0 PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ PT5 Q2X QOR QOS R89 R9I RNI ROL RPX RRX RSV RZK S16 S1Z S27 S3A S3B SAP SBL SDH SHX SISQX SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SV3 SZN T13 TEORI TSG TSK TSV TUC U2A U9L UG4 UKHRP UOJIU UTJUX UZXMN VC2 VFIZW W23 W48 WJK WK8 XJT YLTOR Z45 Z81 Z83 ZMTXR ZOVNA ~A9 AAFWJ AASML AAYXX ABDBE ABFSG ACSTC ADHKG AEZWR AFHIU AGQPQ AHPBZ AHWEU AIXLP AYFIA CITATION PHGZM PHGZT CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PJZUB PPXIY 7QG 7SN 7TK 7XB 8FK C1K K9. MBDVC PKEHL PQEST PQUKI PRINS Q9U 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c584t-fe0306b6b0327757890973171fd24f95f2247fa817c4121fa6e9dea1af894a543 |
IEDL.DBID | 7X7 |
ISSN | 1435-9448 1435-9456 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:38:41 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 16:46:39 EDT 2025 Wed Aug 13 06:33:34 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:58:16 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:27:45 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:48:47 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 21 02:47:00 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 5 |
Keywords | Estrildid finch Plumage dot pattern Sensory exploitation Sensory bias Visual attention Signal evolution |
Language | English |
License | 2022. The Author(s). Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c584t-fe0306b6b0327757890973171fd24f95f2247fa817c4121fa6e9dea1af894a543 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-8596-1956 0000-0003-0822-5637 |
OpenAccessLink | https://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5 |
PMID | 35294684 |
PQID | 2729996768 |
PQPubID | 55406 |
PageCount | 9 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9617841 proquest_miscellaneous_2640324975 proquest_journals_2729996768 pubmed_primary_35294684 crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5 springer_journals_10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2022-10-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2022-10-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 10 year: 2022 text: 2022-10-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | Berlin/Heidelberg |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Berlin/Heidelberg – name: Germany – name: Heidelberg |
PublicationTitle | Animal cognition |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Anim Cogn |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Anim Cogn |
PublicationYear | 2022 |
Publisher | Springer Berlin Heidelberg Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer Berlin Heidelberg – name: Springer Nature B.V |
References | Zanollo, Griggio, Robertson, Kleindorfer (CR84) 2013; 119 Darst, Cummings, Cannatella (CR14) 2006; 103 Crowhurst, Zanollo, Griggio, Robertson, Kleindorfer (CR13) 2012; 118 McGlothlin, Duff, Henry-Freeman, Ketterson (CR43) 2007; 61 Rodríguez, Snedden (CR59) 2004; 68 Endler (CR16) 1992; 139 Proctor (CR52) 1991; 42 Kose, Møller (CR33) 1999; 45 Basolo (CR4) 1990; 250 Ryan, Cummings (CR85) 2013; 44 Soma (CR72) 2018; 68 Summers, Symula, Clough, Cronin (CR78) 1999; 266 Soma, Iwama (CR75) 2017; 12 Swaddle, Cuthill (CR79) 1994; 258 Geberzahn, Gahr (CR25) 2011; 6 Soma, Garamszegi (CR73) 2015; 3 Bates, Mächler, Bolker, Walker (CR6) 2015; 67 Endler, Basolo (CR17) 1998; 13 Zanollo, Griggio, Robertson, Kleindorfer (CR83) 2012; 153 Payne, del Hoyo (CR50) 2010 Crothers, Gering, Cummings (CR12) 2011; 65 Kupfer, Fessler (CR34) 2018; 373 Goodwin (CR27) 1982 Martin (CR41) 2007; 148 McGraw, Mackillop, Dale, Hauber (CR44) 2002; 205 Basolo (CR3) 1990; 30 Madden, Tanner (CR39) 2003; 65 Maan, Cummings (CR37) 2009; 106 Borgia, Keagy (CR8) 2006; 72 Proctor (CR53) 1992; 44 Griggio, Valera, Casas-Crivillé, Hoi, Barbosa (CR29) 2011; 65 Ota, Gahr, Soma (CR49) 2015; 5 Rodd, Hughes, Grether, Baril (CR58) 2002; 269 Caves, Brandley, Johnsen (CR9) 2018; 33 Cole, Wilkins (CR10) 2013; 24 Ryan, Fox, Wilczynski, Rand (CR69) 1990; 343 Bischof (CR7) 1988; 163 Roulin (CR62) 1999; 10 Ryan, Rand (CR68) 1993; 340 Ryan, Keddy-Hector (CR66) 1992; 139 Basolo (CR5) 1995; 259 Marques, Batalha, Cardoso (CR40) 2016; 3 Dawkins (CR15) 2002; 63 Gomes, Funghi, Soma, Sorenson, Cardoso (CR26) 2017; 30 Prum, Torres, Williamson, Dyck (CR55) 1999; 266 Fuller, Endler (CR20) 2018; 64 Maan, Cummings (CR36) 2008; 62 Ruiz-de-Castañeda, Burtt, Gonzalez-Braojos, Moreno (CR64) 2012; 105 Zahavi (CR81) 1975; 53 CR56 Andersson (CR2) 1986; 40 Ryan (CR65) 1998; 281 Alatalo, Höglund, Lundberg, Sutherland (CR1) 1992; 3 Somveille, Marshall, Gluckman (CR76) 2016; 4 Průchová, Nedvěd, Veselý, Ernestová, Fuchs (CR54) 2014; 151 Ryan, Rand (CR67) 1990; 44 Reynolds, Fitzpatrick (CR57) 2007; 61 Rosenthal (CR61) 2018; 64 Garcia, Ramirez (CR24) 2005; 434 Moreno, López-Arrabé (CR46) 2021; 162 Siddiqi, Cronin, Loew, Vorobyev, Summers (CR70) 2004; 207 Soulsbury, Kervinen, Lebigre (CR77) 2016; 27 Zahavi (CR82) 1977; 67 Galván, Alonso-Alvarez (CR23) 2008; 3 Ron (CR60) 2008; 76 Endler, Mappes (CR18) 2017; 372 Roulin (CR63) 2016; 91 Gahr, Güttinger (CR22) 1986; 72 Guindre-Parker, Love (CR30) 2014; 45 Mizuno, Soma (CR45) 2020; 33 Martin (CR42) 2017 Maan, Cummings (CR38) 2012; 179 Fuller, Houle, Travis (CR21) 2005; 166 Kupfer, Le (CR35) 2018; 32 Soma, Garamszegi (CR74) 2018; 29 Morris (CR47) 1958; 131 Grafen (CR28) 1990; 144 Hanssen, Hasselquist, Folstad, Erikstad (CR31) 2008; 4 Piault, van den Brink, Roulin (CR51) 2012; 99 RO Prum (1609_CR55) 1999; 266 EM Caves (1609_CR9) 2018; 33 M Soma (1609_CR74) 2018; 29 TR Kupfer (1609_CR35) 2018; 32 RC Fuller (1609_CR21) 2005; 166 S Guindre-Parker (1609_CR30) 2014; 45 JA Endler (1609_CR18) 2017; 372 HJ Bischof (1609_CR7) 1988; 163 GR Martin (1609_CR41) 2007; 148 RV Alatalo (1609_CR1) 1992; 3 AL Basolo (1609_CR5) 1995; 259 D Goodwin (1609_CR27) 1982 CD Soulsbury (1609_CR77) 2016; 27 SA Hanssen (1609_CR31) 2008; 4 ACR Gomes (1609_CR26) 2017; 30 RB Payne (1609_CR50) 2010 HC Proctor (1609_CR53) 1992; 44 D Morris (1609_CR47) 1958; 131 ME Maan (1609_CR37) 2009; 106 TR Kupfer (1609_CR34) 2018; 373 CI Marques (1609_CR40) 2016; 3 RC Fuller (1609_CR20) 2018; 64 A Roulin (1609_CR63) 2016; 91 R Ruiz-de-Castañeda (1609_CR64) 2012; 105 C Garcia (1609_CR24) 2005; 434 MJ Ryan (1609_CR65) 1998; 281 CJ Crowhurst (1609_CR13) 2012; 118 J Moreno (1609_CR46) 2021; 162 SR Ron (1609_CR60) 2008; 76 JP Swaddle (1609_CR79) 1994; 258 JR Madden (1609_CR39) 2003; 65 GG Rosenthal (1609_CR61) 2018; 64 A Grafen (1609_CR28) 1990; 144 MJ Ryan (1609_CR69) 1990; 343 M Soma (1609_CR75) 2017; 12 V Zanollo (1609_CR83) 2012; 153 A Mizuno (1609_CR45) 2020; 33 M Soma (1609_CR72) 2018; 68 JA Endler (1609_CR16) 1992; 139 R Piault (1609_CR51) 2012; 99 MS Dawkins (1609_CR15) 2002; 63 MJ Ryan (1609_CR85) 2013; 44 M Somveille (1609_CR76) 2016; 4 N Ota (1609_CR49) 2015; 5 M Griggio (1609_CR29) 2011; 65 ME Maan (1609_CR36) 2008; 62 JA Endler (1609_CR17) 1998; 13 MJ Ryan (1609_CR67) 1990; 44 N Geberzahn (1609_CR25) 2011; 6 K Summers (1609_CR78) 1999; 266 G Borgia (1609_CR8) 2006; 72 GG Cole (1609_CR10) 2013; 24 V Zanollo (1609_CR84) 2013; 119 GR Martin (1609_CR42) 2017 A Siddiqi (1609_CR70) 2004; 207 AL Basolo (1609_CR4) 1990; 250 JW McGlothlin (1609_CR43) 2007; 61 1609_CR56 M Gahr (1609_CR22) 1986; 72 AL Basolo (1609_CR3) 1990; 30 M Kose (1609_CR33) 1999; 45 CR Darst (1609_CR14) 2006; 103 ME Maan (1609_CR38) 2012; 179 KJ McGraw (1609_CR44) 2002; 205 A Zahavi (1609_CR82) 1977; 67 RL Rodríguez (1609_CR59) 2004; 68 I Galván (1609_CR23) 2008; 3 HC Proctor (1609_CR52) 1991; 42 M Soma (1609_CR73) 2015; 3 FH Rodd (1609_CR58) 2002; 269 MJ Ryan (1609_CR68) 1993; 340 A Zahavi (1609_CR81) 1975; 53 M Andersson (1609_CR2) 1986; 40 D Bates (1609_CR6) 2015; 67 A Průchová (1609_CR54) 2014; 151 RG Reynolds (1609_CR57) 2007; 61 A Roulin (1609_CR62) 1999; 10 L Crothers (1609_CR12) 2011; 65 MJ Ryan (1609_CR66) 1992; 139 |
References_xml | – volume: 144 start-page: 517 year: 1990 end-page: 546 ident: CR28 article-title: Biological signals as handicaps publication-title: J Theor Biol doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8 – volume: 340 start-page: 187 year: 1993 end-page: 195 ident: CR68 article-title: Sexual selection and signal evolution: the ghost of biases past publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B doi: 10.1098/rstb.1993.0057 – volume: 119 start-page: 344 year: 2013 end-page: 352 ident: CR84 article-title: Males with a faster courtship display have more white spots and higher pairing success in the Diamond Firetail, publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/eth.12071 – volume: 45 start-page: 29 year: 2014 end-page: 33 ident: CR30 article-title: Revisiting the condition-dependence of melanin-based plumage publication-title: J Avian Biol doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00190.x – volume: 33 start-page: 49 year: 2020 end-page: 61 ident: CR45 article-title: Potential role of sensory bias in plumage pattern evolution: termite-eating and polka-dots in estrildid finches publication-title: Ethol Ecol Evol doi: 10.1080/03949370.2020.1803414 – volume: 13 start-page: 415 year: 1998 end-page: 420 ident: CR17 article-title: Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(98)01471-2 – volume: 434 start-page: 501 year: 2005 end-page: 505 ident: CR24 article-title: Evidence that sensory traps can evolve into honest signals publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/nature03363 – volume: 76 start-page: 1783 year: 2008 end-page: 1794 ident: CR60 article-title: The evolution of female mate choice for complex calls in túngara frogs publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.024 – volume: 266 start-page: 2141 year: 1999 end-page: 2145 ident: CR78 article-title: Visual mate choice in poison frogs publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0900 – volume: 269 start-page: 475 year: 2002 end-page: 481 ident: CR58 article-title: A possible non-sexual origin of mate preference: are male guppies mimicking fruit? publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1891 – volume: 179 start-page: E1 year: 2012 end-page: E14 ident: CR38 article-title: Poison frog colors are honest signals of toxicity, particularly for bird predators publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/663197 – volume: 64 start-page: 485 year: 2018 end-page: 492 ident: CR61 article-title: Evaluation and hedonic value in mate choice publication-title: Curr Zool doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy054 – volume: 343 start-page: 66 year: 1990 end-page: 67 ident: CR69 article-title: Sexual selection for sensory exploitation in the frog publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/343066a0 – start-page: 234 year: 2010 end-page: 377 ident: CR50 article-title: Handbook of the birds of the world publication-title: Barcelona – volume: 4 year: 2016 ident: CR76 article-title: A global analysis of bird plumage patterns reveals no association between habitat and camouflage publication-title: PeerJ doi: 10.7717/peerj.2658 – volume: 10 start-page: 688 year: 1999 end-page: 695 ident: CR62 article-title: Nonrandom pairing by male barn owls ( ) with respect to a female plumage trait publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.6.688 – volume: 44 start-page: 305 year: 1990 end-page: 314 ident: CR67 article-title: The sensory basis of sexual selection for complex calls in the túngara frog, (sexual selection for sensory exploitation) publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1990.tb05200.x – volume: 91 start-page: 328 year: 2016 end-page: 348 ident: CR63 article-title: Condition-dependence, pleiotropy and the handicap principle of sexual selection in melanin-based colouration publication-title: Biol Rev doi: 10.1111/brv.12171 – volume: 68 start-page: 121 year: 2018 end-page: 130 ident: CR72 article-title: Sexual selection in Estrildid finches, with further review of the evolution of nesting material holding display in relation to cooperative parental nesting publication-title: Jan J Anim Psychol doi: 10.2502/janip.68.2.2 – volume: 5 start-page: 16614 year: 2015 ident: CR49 article-title: Tap dancing birds: the multimodal mutual courtship display of males and females in a socially monogamous songbird publication-title: Sci Rep doi: 10.1038/srep16614 – volume: 12 year: 2017 ident: CR75 article-title: Mating success follows duet dancing in the Java sparrow publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172655 – volume: 61 start-page: 1391 year: 2007 end-page: 1399 ident: CR43 article-title: Diet quality affects an attractive white plumage pattern in dark-eyed juncos ( ) publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0370-x – volume: 205 start-page: 3747 year: 2002 end-page: 3755 ident: CR44 article-title: Different colors reveal different information: how nutritional stress affects the expression of melanin-and structurally based ornamental plumage publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747 – volume: 250 start-page: 808 year: 1990 end-page: 810 ident: CR4 article-title: Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.250.4982.808 – volume: 266 start-page: 13 year: 1999 end-page: 22 ident: CR55 article-title: Two-dimensional Fourier analysis of the spongy medullary keratin of structurally coloured feather barbs publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0598 – year: 2017 ident: CR42 publication-title: The sensory ecology of birds doi: 10.1093/oso/9780199694532.001.0001 – volume: 162 start-page: 511 year: 2021 end-page: 520 ident: CR46 article-title: The extent of white plumage patches in female Pied Flycatchers is negatively associated with corticosterone concentration in partly unpigmented feathers publication-title: J Ornithol doi: 10.1007/s10336-020-01851-z – volume: 99 start-page: 391 year: 2012 end-page: 396 ident: CR51 article-title: Condition-dependent expression of melanin-based coloration in the Eurasian kestrel publication-title: Naturwissenschaften doi: 10.1007/s00114-012-0914-4 – volume: 29 start-page: 676 year: 2018 end-page: 685 ident: CR74 article-title: Evolution of patterned plumage as a sexual signal in Estrildid finches publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/ary021 – year: 1982 ident: CR27 publication-title: Estrildid finches of the world – volume: 45 start-page: 430 year: 1999 end-page: 436 ident: CR33 article-title: Sexual selection, feather breakage and parasites: the importance of white spots in the tail of the barn swallow ( ) publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s002650050581 – volume: 65 start-page: 1077 year: 2003 end-page: 1083 ident: CR39 article-title: Preferences for coloured bower decorations can be explained in a nonsexual context publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2126 – volume: 42 start-page: 589 year: 1991 end-page: 598 ident: CR52 article-title: Courtship in the water mite : males capitalize on female adaptations for predation publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8 – volume: 72 start-page: 123 year: 1986 end-page: 131 ident: CR22 article-title: Functional aspects of singing in male and female (Estrildidae) publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00612.x – volume: 139 start-page: S4 year: 1992 end-page: S35 ident: CR66 article-title: Directional patterns of female mate choice and the role of sensory biases publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/285303 – volume: 163 start-page: 329 year: 1988 end-page: 337 ident: CR7 article-title: The visual field and visually guided behavior in the zebra finch ( ) publication-title: J Comp Physiol doi: 10.1007/BF00604008 – volume: 67 start-page: 1 year: 2015 end-page: 48 ident: CR6 article-title: Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 publication-title: J Stat Softw doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 – volume: 151 start-page: 128 year: 2014 end-page: 134 ident: CR54 article-title: Visual warning signals of the ladybird : the avian predators’ point of view publication-title: Entomol Exp Appl doi: 10.1111/eea.12176 – volume: 258 start-page: 267 year: 1994 end-page: 271 ident: CR79 article-title: Female Zebra Finches prefer males with symmetric chest plumage publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0172 – volume: 105 start-page: 409 year: 2012 end-page: 419 ident: CR64 article-title: Bacterial degradability of an intrafeather unmelanized ornament: a role for feather-degrading bacteria in sexual selection? publication-title: Biol J Linn Soc doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01806.x – volume: 68 start-page: 427 year: 2004 end-page: 432 ident: CR59 article-title: On the functional design of mate preferences and receiver biases publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.031 – volume: 32 start-page: 729 year: 2018 end-page: 741 ident: CR35 article-title: Disgusting clusters: trypophobia as an overgeneralised disease avoidance response publication-title: Cogn Emot doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1345721 – volume: 6 year: 2011 ident: CR25 article-title: Undirected (solitary) birdsong in female and male blue-capped cordon-bleus ( ) and its endocrine correlates publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026485 – volume: 44 start-page: 745 year: 1992 end-page: 752 ident: CR53 article-title: Sensory exploitation and the evolution of male mating behaviour: a cladistic test using water mites (Acari: Parasitengona) publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8 – volume: 259 start-page: 307 year: 1995 end-page: 311 ident: CR5 article-title: Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-existing bias in sexual selection publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0045 – volume: 65 start-page: 599 year: 2011 end-page: 605 ident: CR12 article-title: Aposematic signal variation predicts male–male interactions in a polymorphic poison frog publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x – volume: 103 start-page: 5852 year: 2006 end-page: 5857 ident: CR14 article-title: A mechanism for diversity in warning signals: conspicuousness versus toxicity in poison frogs publication-title: PNAS doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600625103 – volume: 63 start-page: 991 year: 2002 end-page: 998 ident: CR15 article-title: What are birds looking at? Head movements and eye use in chickens publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.1999 – volume: 373 start-page: 20170207 year: 2018 ident: CR34 article-title: Ectoparasite defence in humans: relationships to pathogen avoidance and clinical implications publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0207 – volume: 166 start-page: 437 year: 2005 end-page: 446 ident: CR21 article-title: Sensory bias as an explanation for the evolution of mate preferences publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/444443 – ident: CR56 – volume: 53 start-page: 205 year: 1975 end-page: 214 ident: CR81 article-title: Mate selection—a selection for a handicap publication-title: J Theor Biol doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3 – volume: 44 start-page: 437 issue: 1 year: 2013 end-page: 459 ident: CR85 article-title: Perceptual biases and mate choice publication-title: Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901 – volume: 40 start-page: 804 year: 1986 end-page: 816 ident: CR2 article-title: Evolution of condition-dependent sex ornaments and mating preferences: sexual selection based on viability differences publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb00540.x – volume: 118 start-page: 63 year: 2012 end-page: 75 ident: CR13 article-title: White flank spots signal feeding dominance in female diamond firetails, publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01986.x – volume: 62 start-page: 2334 year: 2008 end-page: 2345 ident: CR36 article-title: Female preferences for aposematic signal components in a polymorphic poison frog publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00454.x – volume: 131 start-page: 389 year: 1958 end-page: 439 ident: CR47 article-title: The comparative ethology of grassfinches (Erythrurae) and mannikins (Amadinae) publication-title: Proc Zool Soc Lond doi: 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1958.tb00695.x – volume: 281 start-page: 1999 year: 1998 end-page: 2003 ident: CR65 article-title: Sexual selection, receiver biases, and the evolution of sex differences publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.281.5385.1999 – volume: 207 start-page: 2471 year: 2004 end-page: 2485 ident: CR70 article-title: Interspecific and intraspecific views of color signals in the strawberry poison frog publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.01047 – volume: 27 start-page: 1362 year: 2016 end-page: 1369 ident: CR77 article-title: Curse of the black spot: spotting negatively correlates with fitness in black grouse publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/arw057 – volume: 148 start-page: 547 year: 2007 end-page: 562 ident: CR41 article-title: Visual fields and their functions in birds publication-title: J Ornithol doi: 10.1007/s10336-007-0213-6 – volume: 106 start-page: 19072 year: 2009 end-page: 19077 ident: CR37 article-title: Sexual dimorphism and directional sexual selection on aposematic signals in a poison frog publication-title: PNAS doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903327106 – volume: 3 start-page: 4 year: 2015 ident: CR73 article-title: Evolution of courtship display in Estrildid finches: dance in relation to female song and plumage ornamentation publication-title: Front Ecol Evol doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00004 – volume: 3 start-page: 53 year: 1992 end-page: 59 ident: CR1 article-title: Evolution of black grouse leks: female preferences benefit males in larger leks publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/3.1.53 – volume: 30 start-page: A80 year: 1990 ident: CR3 article-title: Preexisting mating biases and the evolution of the sword in the genus Xiphophorus publication-title: Am Zool – volume: 153 start-page: 1233 year: 2012 end-page: 1244 ident: CR83 article-title: The number and coloration of white flank spots predict the strength of a cutaneous immune response in female Diamond Firetails, publication-title: J Ornithol doi: 10.1007/s10336-012-0855-x – volume: 72 start-page: 1125 year: 2006 end-page: 1133 ident: CR8 article-title: An inverse relationship between decoration and food colour preferences in satin bowerbirds does not support the sensory drive hypothesis publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.015 – volume: 372 start-page: 20160352 year: 2017 ident: CR18 article-title: The current and future state of animal coloration research publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0352 – volume: 67 start-page: 603 year: 1977 end-page: 605 ident: CR82 article-title: The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle) publication-title: J Theor Biol doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(77)90061-3 – volume: 65 start-page: 655 year: 2011 end-page: 664 ident: CR29 article-title: White tail markings are an indicator of quality and affect mate preference in rock sparrows publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1067-0 – volume: 33 start-page: 358 year: 2018 end-page: 372 ident: CR9 article-title: Visual acuity and the evolution of signals publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001 – volume: 3 year: 2008 ident: CR23 article-title: An intracellular antioxidant determines the expression of a melanin-based signal in a bird publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003335 – volume: 3 year: 2016 ident: CR40 article-title: Signalling with a cryptic trait: the regularity of barred plumage in common waxbills publication-title: R Soc Open Sci doi: 10.1098/rsos.160195 – volume: 64 start-page: 465 year: 2018 end-page: 470 ident: CR20 article-title: A perspective on sensory drive publication-title: Curr Zool doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy052 – volume: 139 start-page: S125 year: 1992 end-page: S153 ident: CR16 article-title: Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/285308 – volume: 24 start-page: 1980 year: 2013 end-page: 1985 ident: CR10 article-title: Fear of holes publication-title: Psychol Sci doi: 10.1177/0956797613484937 – volume: 30 start-page: 1336 year: 2017 end-page: 1346 ident: CR26 article-title: Multimodal signalling in estrildid finches: song, dance and colour are associated with different ecological and life-history traits publication-title: J Evol Biol doi: 10.1111/jeb.13102 – volume: 61 start-page: 2253 year: 2007 end-page: 2259 ident: CR57 article-title: Assortative mating in poison-dart frogs based on an ecologically important trait publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00174.x – volume: 4 start-page: 379 year: 2008 end-page: 381 ident: CR31 article-title: A label of health: a previous immune challenge is reflected in the expression of a female plumage trait publication-title: Biol Lett doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0131 – start-page: 234 volume-title: Barcelona year: 2010 ident: 1609_CR50 – volume-title: The sensory ecology of birds year: 2017 ident: 1609_CR42 doi: 10.1093/oso/9780199694532.001.0001 – volume: 4 start-page: 379 year: 2008 ident: 1609_CR31 publication-title: Biol Lett doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0131 – volume: 68 start-page: 121 year: 2018 ident: 1609_CR72 publication-title: Jan J Anim Psychol doi: 10.2502/janip.68.2.2 – volume: 65 start-page: 655 year: 2011 ident: 1609_CR29 publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1067-0 – volume: 45 start-page: 29 year: 2014 ident: 1609_CR30 publication-title: J Avian Biol doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00190.x – volume: 68 start-page: 427 year: 2004 ident: 1609_CR59 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.031 – volume: 343 start-page: 66 year: 1990 ident: 1609_CR69 publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/343066a0 – volume: 27 start-page: 1362 year: 2016 ident: 1609_CR77 publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/arw057 – volume: 269 start-page: 475 year: 2002 ident: 1609_CR58 publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1891 – volume: 3 year: 2008 ident: 1609_CR23 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003335 – volume: 61 start-page: 1391 year: 2007 ident: 1609_CR43 publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0370-x – volume: 3 start-page: 53 year: 1992 ident: 1609_CR1 publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/3.1.53 – volume: 373 start-page: 20170207 year: 2018 ident: 1609_CR34 publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0207 – volume: 29 start-page: 676 year: 2018 ident: 1609_CR74 publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/ary021 – volume: 105 start-page: 409 year: 2012 ident: 1609_CR64 publication-title: Biol J Linn Soc doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01806.x – volume: 144 start-page: 517 year: 1990 ident: 1609_CR28 publication-title: J Theor Biol doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8 – volume: 205 start-page: 3747 year: 2002 ident: 1609_CR44 publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747 – volume: 281 start-page: 1999 year: 1998 ident: 1609_CR65 publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.281.5385.1999 – volume: 44 start-page: 305 year: 1990 ident: 1609_CR67 publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1990.tb05200.x – volume: 67 start-page: 1 year: 2015 ident: 1609_CR6 publication-title: J Stat Softw doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 – volume: 33 start-page: 358 year: 2018 ident: 1609_CR9 publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001 – volume: 64 start-page: 465 year: 2018 ident: 1609_CR20 publication-title: Curr Zool doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy052 – volume: 63 start-page: 991 year: 2002 ident: 1609_CR15 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.1999 – volume: 72 start-page: 1125 year: 2006 ident: 1609_CR8 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.015 – volume: 162 start-page: 511 year: 2021 ident: 1609_CR46 publication-title: J Ornithol doi: 10.1007/s10336-020-01851-z – volume: 207 start-page: 2471 year: 2004 ident: 1609_CR70 publication-title: J Exp Biol doi: 10.1242/jeb.01047 – volume: 103 start-page: 5852 year: 2006 ident: 1609_CR14 publication-title: PNAS doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600625103 – volume: 33 start-page: 49 year: 2020 ident: 1609_CR45 publication-title: Ethol Ecol Evol doi: 10.1080/03949370.2020.1803414 – volume: 340 start-page: 187 year: 1993 ident: 1609_CR68 publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B doi: 10.1098/rstb.1993.0057 – volume: 106 start-page: 19072 year: 2009 ident: 1609_CR37 publication-title: PNAS doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903327106 – volume: 119 start-page: 344 year: 2013 ident: 1609_CR84 publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/eth.12071 – volume-title: Estrildid finches of the world year: 1982 ident: 1609_CR27 – volume: 53 start-page: 205 year: 1975 ident: 1609_CR81 publication-title: J Theor Biol doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3 – volume: 434 start-page: 501 year: 2005 ident: 1609_CR24 publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/nature03363 – volume: 32 start-page: 729 year: 2018 ident: 1609_CR35 publication-title: Cogn Emot doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1345721 – volume: 5 start-page: 16614 year: 2015 ident: 1609_CR49 publication-title: Sci Rep doi: 10.1038/srep16614 – volume: 266 start-page: 13 year: 1999 ident: 1609_CR55 publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0598 – volume: 166 start-page: 437 year: 2005 ident: 1609_CR21 publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/444443 – volume: 6 year: 2011 ident: 1609_CR25 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026485 – volume: 99 start-page: 391 year: 2012 ident: 1609_CR51 publication-title: Naturwissenschaften doi: 10.1007/s00114-012-0914-4 – volume: 3 year: 2016 ident: 1609_CR40 publication-title: R Soc Open Sci doi: 10.1098/rsos.160195 – volume: 10 start-page: 688 year: 1999 ident: 1609_CR62 publication-title: Behav Ecol doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.6.688 – volume: 30 start-page: A80 year: 1990 ident: 1609_CR3 publication-title: Am Zool – volume: 42 start-page: 589 year: 1991 ident: 1609_CR52 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8 – volume: 4 year: 2016 ident: 1609_CR76 publication-title: PeerJ doi: 10.7717/peerj.2658 – volume: 163 start-page: 329 year: 1988 ident: 1609_CR7 publication-title: J Comp Physiol doi: 10.1007/BF00604008 – volume: 65 start-page: 1077 year: 2003 ident: 1609_CR39 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2126 – volume: 131 start-page: 389 year: 1958 ident: 1609_CR47 publication-title: Proc Zool Soc Lond doi: 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1958.tb00695.x – volume: 67 start-page: 603 year: 1977 ident: 1609_CR82 publication-title: J Theor Biol doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(77)90061-3 – volume: 44 start-page: 745 year: 1992 ident: 1609_CR53 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8 – volume: 372 start-page: 20160352 year: 2017 ident: 1609_CR18 publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0352 – ident: 1609_CR56 – volume: 65 start-page: 599 year: 2011 ident: 1609_CR12 publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x – volume: 64 start-page: 485 year: 2018 ident: 1609_CR61 publication-title: Curr Zool doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy054 – volume: 30 start-page: 1336 year: 2017 ident: 1609_CR26 publication-title: J Evol Biol doi: 10.1111/jeb.13102 – volume: 45 start-page: 430 year: 1999 ident: 1609_CR33 publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol doi: 10.1007/s002650050581 – volume: 250 start-page: 808 year: 1990 ident: 1609_CR4 publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.250.4982.808 – volume: 12 year: 2017 ident: 1609_CR75 publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172655 – volume: 139 start-page: S4 year: 1992 ident: 1609_CR66 publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/285303 – volume: 118 start-page: 63 year: 2012 ident: 1609_CR13 publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01986.x – volume: 62 start-page: 2334 year: 2008 ident: 1609_CR36 publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00454.x – volume: 91 start-page: 328 year: 2016 ident: 1609_CR63 publication-title: Biol Rev doi: 10.1111/brv.12171 – volume: 3 start-page: 4 year: 2015 ident: 1609_CR73 publication-title: Front Ecol Evol doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00004 – volume: 72 start-page: 123 year: 1986 ident: 1609_CR22 publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00612.x – volume: 148 start-page: 547 year: 2007 ident: 1609_CR41 publication-title: J Ornithol doi: 10.1007/s10336-007-0213-6 – volume: 179 start-page: E1 year: 2012 ident: 1609_CR38 publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/663197 – volume: 40 start-page: 804 year: 1986 ident: 1609_CR2 publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb00540.x – volume: 61 start-page: 2253 year: 2007 ident: 1609_CR57 publication-title: Evolution doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00174.x – volume: 76 start-page: 1783 year: 2008 ident: 1609_CR60 publication-title: Anim Behav doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.024 – volume: 44 start-page: 437 issue: 1 year: 2013 ident: 1609_CR85 publication-title: Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901 – volume: 13 start-page: 415 year: 1998 ident: 1609_CR17 publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(98)01471-2 – volume: 266 start-page: 2141 year: 1999 ident: 1609_CR78 publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0900 – volume: 258 start-page: 267 year: 1994 ident: 1609_CR79 publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0172 – volume: 139 start-page: S125 year: 1992 ident: 1609_CR16 publication-title: Am Nat doi: 10.1086/285308 – volume: 259 start-page: 307 year: 1995 ident: 1609_CR5 publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0045 – volume: 24 start-page: 1980 year: 2013 ident: 1609_CR10 publication-title: Psychol Sci doi: 10.1177/0956797613484937 – volume: 151 start-page: 128 year: 2014 ident: 1609_CR54 publication-title: Entomol Exp Appl doi: 10.1111/eea.12176 – volume: 153 start-page: 1233 year: 2012 ident: 1609_CR83 publication-title: J Ornithol doi: 10.1007/s10336-012-0855-x |
SSID | ssj0008491 |
Score | 2.3403766 |
Snippet | Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref springer |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 1271 |
SubjectTerms | Animals Aposematism Behavioral Sciences Bias Biological evolution Biomedical and Life Sciences Comparative studies Dietary restrictions Evolution Feathers - chemistry Feathers - physiology Finches - physiology Food Food availability Foraging behavior Humans Hunger Hypotheses Life Sciences Natural selection Neochmia ruficauda Original Paper Passeriformes Phylogeny Pigmentation - physiology Plumage Preferences Prey Psychology Research Signaling Zoology |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: SpringerLink Journals (ICM) dbid: U2A link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Na9wwEB3alEIuJU0_4iQtKvTWCipblq3cQmkIhebUhdCLkWSJXUhts7tOyb_PjNZ2sk1b6M2gsWx5JM0bz-gNwHttPVpNW3JV1I7LLNRcG624rUPIbJapMma7f7tQ5zP59TK_HA6FrcZs9zEkGXfqe4fd0Bxyyj4nVjTN88fwJEffneb1LD2d9t9Sxjp5BAS4Ru9jOCrz5z62zdEDjPkwVfK3eGk0Q2d78GzAj-x0o_Dn8Mg3-7A7bWM3-_D0RxuvXkCLSHLJwqKholjswrdu_nNh2LIPqJm-Nmxurj0z7Hqx6rHPbio5whDJsl8UYGDotLIucnA2qxOU7VpaRFeeOTR_rA1svbzp2o5-y5iXMDv78v3zOR8KLHCHuGPNgyePwSr7KUsLIrbXRN4jChHqVAadB7TvRTClKJwUqQhGeV17I0wotTS5zF7BTtM2_gBY6lxR1uj7WFFL73ObOY99KIs3WwQNCYjxO1duYB-nIhhX1R1vMummQt1UUTdVnsCH6Z5uw73xT-njUX3VsA5XVYq-A3p06FMl8G5qxhVEYRHT-LZHGSVx_FIX2MXrjbanxyE81VKVMoFiax5MAsTOvd3SLOaRpVvT4UspEvg4zpi71_r7KA7_T_wIdlOazTG78Bh21svev0GUtLZv46K4BXIPCMQ priority: 102 providerName: Springer Nature |
Title | Star finches Neochmia ruficauda have a visual preference for white dot patterns: a possible case of trypophilia |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35294684 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2729996768 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2640324975 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9617841 |
Volume | 25 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3fb9MwELZgE9Je0Bg_FjYmI_EGFkvi2DEvqFQdE4gKISoVXiLbsdVKIwltM7T_njs3TVUm9pJEsePEOdv3nX3-jpBXyjjQmiZnQpaW8dSXTGklmCm9T02aijx4u38Zi8sJ_zTNpt2E27Jzq9yMiWGgLmuLc-RvE0CBgM0BHb9vfjOMGoWrq10IjftkH6nL0PiS097gOs95iJiHkIApsEO6TTPd1jlQrgx92ZFjTbFsVzHdQpu3nSb_WTkNCunikDzskCQdrEX_iNxz1RE56Ae0myPy4Gcdrh6TGjDlgvp5heGx6NjVdvZrrumi9SCjttR0pq8d1fR6vmyhzKYPPkIB09I_uNRAwXylTWDjrJbvIG9TY3e6ctSCIqS1p6vFTVM3OEGjn5DJxej78JJ1oRaYBQSyYt6h7WCEOU8TiRT3Cml8Yhn7MuFeZR40vfQ6j6XlcRJ7LZwqnY61zxXXGU-fkr2qrtwxoYm1Mi_BCjJxyZ3LTGodlCEMPGwAPkQk3vznwnY85BgO46rYMiijbAqQTRFkU2QRed0_06xZOO7MfboRX9H1yGWxbT8RedknQ1_CBRJdubqFPIJD_bmSUMSztbT71wFQVVzkPCJypx30GZCnezelms8CX7fCbZg8jsibTYvZftb_a_H87lqckIMEW2_wKzwle6tF614APlqZs9AJzsj-4OOPzyM4fxiNv36Du0MxhOMkGfwF1OARFQ |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9QwEB6VIkQvCMorUMBIcAKrm8R5GAkhBFRb2u6plVZcgp3Y2pVKEnY3rfZP8RuZ8SZZLRW99RbJj8SZsecbe_wNwBupDVpNnfI4KXIuQltwqWTMdWFtqMMwTl20-8koHp6J7-NovAV_urswFFbZrYluoS6qnPbI9wNEgYjNER1_qn9zyhpFp6tdCo2VWhyZ5SW6bPOPh19Rvm-D4ODb6Zchb7MK8ByN7YJbQzBZx3oQBgmxuUtirPET3xaBsDKyaNQSq1I_yYUf-FbFRhZG-cqmUqhIhNjvLbiNhndAIYTJuHfwBqlwGfoIgnCJfk97Sae9qofGnFPsPHG6SR5tGsIr6PZqkOY_J7XOAB7ch3stcmWfV6r2ALZMuQs7_QK63IU7Pyr39BAqxLAzZqclpeNiI1Plk19TxWaNRZ1oCsUm6sIwxS6m8wb7rPtkJwwxNLukow2G7jKrHftnOf-AdeuKpu-5YTkaXlZZtpgt66qmDSH1CM5uRAiPYbusSvMUWJDnSVqg16X9QhgT6TA32EessbFGuOKB3_3nLG95zyn9xnm2Zmwm2WQom8zJJos8eNe3qVesH9fW3uvEl7UrwDxb66sHr_tinLt0IKNKUzVYJxY4fiET7OLJStr96xAYSxGnwoNkQw_6CsQLvllSTieOH1zStU_he_C-05j1Z_1_FM-uH8UruDs8PTnOjg9HR89hJyBNdjGNe7C9mDXmBWKzhX7pJgSDnzc9A_8C7hhHYg |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3fb9MwED6NIdBeJhi_MgYYCZ7A2pI4cYyEEGJUG4OKByZVvGS2Y6uVRhLaZlP_Nf46zm6SqkzsbW-R4jhx7s73nX3-DuCVUAa9pspoygtNWWwLKqRIqSqsjVUcp5nPdv82TI9O2ZdRMtqAP91ZGJdW2c2JfqIuKu3WyPcjRIGIzREd79s2LeL74eBD_Zu6ClJup7Urp7FUkROzuMTwbfb--BBl_TqKBp9_fDqibYUBqtHxzqk1DjKrVB3EEXfM7sKx14Q8tEXErEgsOjhuZRZyzcIotDI1ojAylDYTTCYsxn5vwW0eo9tEW-KjPtg7yJiv1ufgCBUYA7UHdtpje-jYqcujd_xugibrTvEK0r2asPnPrq13hoN7sN2iWPJxqXb3YcOUO7DVT6aLHbjzs_JXD6BCPDsldlK60lxkaCo9_jWRZNpY1I-mkGQsLwyR5GIya7DPui98QhBPk0u3zUEwdCa1ZwItZ--wbV05Uz43RKMTJpUl8-mirmq3OCQfwumNCOERbJZVaZ4AibTmWYERmAoLZkyiYm2wj1ThwwqhSwBh959z3XKgu1Ic5_mKvdnJJkfZ5F42eRLAm_6ZeskAcm3rvU58eTsbzPKV7gbwsr-Nduw2Z2RpqgbbpAzHzwTHLh4vpd2_DkGyYGnGAuBretA3cBzh63fKydhzhQt3BJSFAbztNGb1Wf8fxe71o3gBd9H28q_Hw5OnsBU5RfbpjXuwOZ825hnCtLl67u2BwNlNG-BfYehLmA |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Star+finches+Neochmia+ruficauda+have+a+visual+preference+for+white+dot+patterns%3A+a+possible+case+of+trypophilia&rft.jtitle=Animal+cognition&rft.date=2022-10-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.issn=1435-9448&rft.eissn=1435-9456&rft.volume=25&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=1271&rft.epage=1279&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs10071-022-01609-5&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1435-9448&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1435-9448&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1435-9448&client=summon |