Star finches Neochmia ruficauda have a visual preference for white dot patterns: a possible case of trypophilia

Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals,...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inAnimal cognition Vol. 25; no. 5; pp. 1271 - 1279
Main Authors Mizuno, Ayumi, Soma, Masayo
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Berlin/Heidelberg Springer Berlin Heidelberg 01.10.2022
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1435-9448
1435-9456
1435-9456
DOI10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda ) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis.
AbstractList Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis.
Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda ) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis.
Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis.Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain unclear. Dot patterns can trigger a fear response (trypophobia) in humans and are known to function as aposematic signals in non-human animals, suggesting that dots may deserve attention for biological reasons. Interestingly in many birds, plumage dot patterns serve for social/sexual signalling. To understand their evolution, we have focused on the sensory bias hypothesis, which predicts the role of pre-existing sensory preference driven by natural selection in shaping signal design. Our previous phylogenetic comparative study supported the hypothesis and showed that diet-driven visual preference promoted the evolution of plumage patterns, as there was an evolutionary correlation between termite-eating (white roundish gregarious prey) and the presence of plumage dot patterns in species of the family Estrildidae. This suggests that these species possess an intrinsic preference for dots. To test this, we compared the responses of an Estrildid species with dot plumage pattern (star finch Neochmia ruficauda) towards simultaneously presented monochrome-printed white dot vs white stripe patterns under both food-deprived and -supplied conditions. Overall, star finches preferred dots to stripes. They showed foraging-like behaviours almost only toward dots when hungry and gazed at dots frequently even when food was available, suggesting both hunger-related and hunger-neutral dot preferences. These results are rather surprising, given how strongly the subjects were attracted to abstract dot patterns without organic structure, but provided good support for the sensory bias hypothesis.
Author Mizuno, Ayumi
Soma, Masayo
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Ayumi
  orcidid: 0000-0003-0822-5637
  surname: Mizuno
  fullname: Mizuno, Ayumi
  organization: Biosystems Science Course, The Graduate School of Life Science, Hokkaido University
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Masayo
  orcidid: 0000-0002-8596-1956
  surname: Soma
  fullname: Soma, Masayo
  email: masayo.soma@sci.hokudai.ac.jp
  organization: Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35294684$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kU9vFSEUxYmpsX_0C7gwJG7cjALDwODCpGlsNWl0oa7JfbxLh2beMALzTL-9PF9btYtugITfOTn3nmNyMMUJCXnJ2VvOmH6XdydvmBAN44qZpntCjrhsu8bITh3cv2V_SI5zvmaM9dLwZ-Sw7YSRqpdHJH4rkKgPkxsw0y8Y3bAJQNPig4NlDXSALVKg25AXGOmc0GPCySH1MdFfQyhI17HQGUrBNOX3lZ1jzmE1InWQkUZPS7qZ4zyEMcBz8tTDmPHF7X1Cfpx__H72qbn8evH57PSycV0vS-ORtUyt1Iq1QutO94YZ3XLN_VpIbzovhNQeeq6d5IJ7UGjWCBx8byR0sj0hH_a-87La4NrhVBKMdk5hA-nGRgj2_58pDPYqbq1RXPeSV4M3twYp_lwwF7sJ2eE4woRxyVYoWbNJo7uKvn6AXsclTXU8K7Qwxiit-kq9-jfRfZS7LirQ7wGX6gLroq0LBUqIu4BhtJzZXeF2X7uttds_tdtdAvFAeuf-qKjdi3KFpytMf2M_ovoNkKS_8w
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2023_08_006
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_231057
Cites_doi 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8
10.1098/rstb.1993.0057
10.1111/eth.12071
10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00190.x
10.1080/03949370.2020.1803414
10.1016/s0169-5347(98)01471-2
10.1038/nature03363
10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.024
10.1098/rspb.1999.0900
10.1098/rspb.2001.1891
10.1086/663197
10.1093/cz/zoy054
10.1038/343066a0
10.7717/peerj.2658
10.1093/beheco/10.6.688
10.1111/j.1558-5646.1990.tb05200.x
10.1111/brv.12171
10.2502/janip.68.2.2
10.1038/srep16614
10.1371/journal.pone.0172655
10.1007/s00265-007-0370-x
10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747
10.1126/science.250.4982.808
10.1098/rspb.1999.0598
10.1093/oso/9780199694532.001.0001
10.1007/s10336-020-01851-z
10.1007/s00114-012-0914-4
10.1093/beheco/ary021
10.1007/s002650050581
10.1006/anbe.2003.2126
10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8
10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00612.x
10.1086/285303
10.1007/BF00604008
10.18637/jss.v067.i01
10.1111/eea.12176
10.1098/rspb.1994.0172
10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01806.x
10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.031
10.1080/02699931.2017.1345721
10.1371/journal.pone.0026485
10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8
10.1098/rspb.1995.0045
10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x
10.1073/pnas.0600625103
10.1006/anbe.2002.1999
10.1098/rstb.2017.0207
10.1086/444443
10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3
10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901
10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb00540.x
10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01986.x
10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00454.x
10.1111/j.1096-3642.1958.tb00695.x
10.1126/science.281.5385.1999
10.1242/jeb.01047
10.1093/beheco/arw057
10.1007/s10336-007-0213-6
10.1073/pnas.0903327106
10.3389/fevo.2015.00004
10.1093/beheco/3.1.53
10.1007/s10336-012-0855-x
10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.015
10.1098/rstb.2016.0352
10.1016/0022-5193(77)90061-3
10.1007/s00265-010-1067-0
10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001
10.1371/journal.pone.0003335
10.1098/rsos.160195
10.1093/cz/zoy052
10.1086/285308
10.1177/0956797613484937
10.1111/jeb.13102
10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00174.x
10.1098/rsbl.2008.0131
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright The Author(s) 2022
2022. The Author(s).
Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Oct 2022
Copyright_xml – notice: The Author(s) 2022
– notice: 2022. The Author(s).
– notice: Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Oct 2022
DBID C6C
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
3V.
7QG
7SN
7TK
7X7
7XB
88E
88G
8FI
8FJ
8FK
8G5
ABUWG
AEUYN
AFKRA
AZQEC
BENPR
C1K
CCPQU
DWQXO
FYUFA
GHDGH
GNUQQ
GUQSH
K9.
M0S
M1P
M2M
M2O
MBDVC
PHGZM
PHGZT
PJZUB
PKEHL
PPXIY
PQEST
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
PSYQQ
Q9U
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5
DatabaseName Springer Nature Link
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
ProQuest Central (Corporate)
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Health & Medical Collection
ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)
Medical Database (Alumni Edition)
Psychology Database (Alumni)
Hospital Premium Collection
Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)
Research Library (Alumni)
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest One Sustainability
ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest Central
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
ProQuest One
ProQuest Central
ProQuest Health Research Premium Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest Research Library
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
ProQuest Health & Medical Collection
Medical Database
Psychology Database
Proquest Research Library
Research Library (Corporate)
ProQuest Central Premium
ProQuest One Academic (New)
ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest One Health & Nursing
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central China
ProQuest One Psychology
ProQuest Central Basic
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest One Psychology
Research Library Prep
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest One Health & Nursing
Research Library (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central China
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
ProQuest Central
ProQuest One Sustainability
ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection
Health Research Premium Collection
Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central Korea
Health & Medical Research Collection
ProQuest Research Library
ProQuest Central (New)
ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni)
ProQuest Central Basic
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
ProQuest Hospital Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni)
Ecology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete
ProQuest Medical Library
ProQuest Psychology Journals
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
Animal Behavior Abstracts
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic (New)
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE

MEDLINE - Academic
CrossRef

ProQuest One Psychology
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: C6C
  name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals
  url: http://www.springeropen.com/
  sourceTypes: Publisher
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 4
  dbid: BENPR
  name: ProQuest
  url: https://www.proquest.com/central
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Zoology
Psychology
EISSN 1435-9456
EndPage 1279
ExternalDocumentID PMC9617841
35294684
10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science
  grantid: 23680027; 16H06177; 20K06809
  funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001691
– fundername: The Japan Science Society
  grantid: 2020–5012
– fundername: Sumitomo Foundation
  grantid: 190272
  funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100008608
– fundername: The Japan Science Society
  grantid: 2020-5012
– fundername: Sumitomo Foundation
  grantid: 190272
– fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science
  grantid: 20K06809
– fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science
  grantid: 16H06177
– fundername: Japan Society for the Promotion of Science
  grantid: 23680027
– fundername: ;
  grantid: 190272
– fundername: ;
  grantid: 2020–5012
– fundername: ;
  grantid: 23680027; 16H06177; 20K06809
GroupedDBID ---
-56
-5G
-BR
-DZ
-EM
-Y2
-~C
.86
06C
06D
0R~
0VY
199
1N0
1SB
203
23M
29~
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2P1
2VQ
2~H
30V
36B
3V.
4.4
406
408
409
40D
40E
53G
5GY
5VS
67N
67Z
6NX
7X7
88E
8FI
8FJ
8G5
8TC
8UJ
95-
95.
95~
96X
AAAVM
AABHQ
AAGAY
AAHBH
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJKR
AAJSJ
AAKKN
AANXM
AANZL
AARHV
AARTL
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
ABAKF
ABBBX
ABBXA
ABDZT
ABECU
ABEEZ
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABHQN
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABKCH
ABKTR
ABMNI
ABMOR
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABULA
ABUWG
ABWNU
ABXPI
ACACY
ACBXY
ACGFS
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACOKC
ACOMO
ACPRK
ACSNA
ACULB
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AENEX
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETLH
AEUYN
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFGCZ
AFGXO
AFKRA
AFLOW
AFPKN
AFQWF
AFRAH
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGGDS
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHKAY
AHMBA
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
AKMHD
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALWAN
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AZFZN
AZQEC
B-.
BA0
BDATZ
BENPR
BGNMA
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C24
C6C
CAG
CCPQU
COF
CS3
CSCUP
DDRTE
DL5
DNIVK
DPUIP
DWQXO
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMB
EMOBN
EN4
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FWDCC
FYUFA
G-Y
G-Z
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ6
GQ7
GQ8
GROUPED_DOAJ
GUQSH
GXS
H13
HF~
HG5
HG6
HMCUK
HMJXF
HQYDN
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
I09
IHE
IJ-
IKXTQ
ITM
IWAJR
IXC
IXE
IZIGR
IZQ
I~X
I~Z
J-C
J0Z
JBSCW
JCJTX
JZLTJ
KDC
KOV
KPH
LAS
LLZTM
M1P
M2M
M2O
M4Y
MA-
MQGED
N2Q
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9I
O9J
OAM
OVD
P2P
PF0
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT5
Q2X
QOR
QOS
R89
R9I
RNI
ROL
RPX
RRX
RSV
RZK
S16
S1Z
S27
S3A
S3B
SAP
SBL
SDH
SHX
SISQX
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TEORI
TSG
TSK
TSV
TUC
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UOJIU
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
W23
W48
WJK
WK8
XJT
YLTOR
Z45
Z81
Z83
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
~A9
AAFWJ
AASML
AAYXX
ABDBE
ABFSG
ACSTC
ADHKG
AEZWR
AFHIU
AGQPQ
AHPBZ
AHWEU
AIXLP
AYFIA
CITATION
PHGZM
PHGZT
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
PJZUB
PPXIY
7QG
7SN
7TK
7XB
8FK
C1K
K9.
MBDVC
PKEHL
PQEST
PQUKI
PRINS
Q9U
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c584t-fe0306b6b0327757890973171fd24f95f2247fa817c4121fa6e9dea1af894a543
IEDL.DBID 7X7
ISSN 1435-9448
1435-9456
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:38:41 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 16:46:39 EDT 2025
Wed Aug 13 06:33:34 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 05:58:16 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 02:27:45 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:48:47 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:47:00 EST 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 5
Keywords Estrildid finch
Plumage dot pattern
Sensory exploitation
Sensory bias
Visual attention
Signal evolution
Language English
License 2022. The Author(s).
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c584t-fe0306b6b0327757890973171fd24f95f2247fa817c4121fa6e9dea1af894a543
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0002-8596-1956
0000-0003-0822-5637
OpenAccessLink https://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5
PMID 35294684
PQID 2729996768
PQPubID 55406
PageCount 9
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9617841
proquest_miscellaneous_2640324975
proquest_journals_2729996768
pubmed_primary_35294684
crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5
springer_journals_10_1007_s10071_022_01609_5
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2022-10-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2022-10-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 10
  year: 2022
  text: 2022-10-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace Berlin/Heidelberg
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Berlin/Heidelberg
– name: Germany
– name: Heidelberg
PublicationTitle Animal cognition
PublicationTitleAbbrev Anim Cogn
PublicationTitleAlternate Anim Cogn
PublicationYear 2022
Publisher Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer Berlin Heidelberg
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Zanollo, Griggio, Robertson, Kleindorfer (CR84) 2013; 119
Darst, Cummings, Cannatella (CR14) 2006; 103
Crowhurst, Zanollo, Griggio, Robertson, Kleindorfer (CR13) 2012; 118
McGlothlin, Duff, Henry-Freeman, Ketterson (CR43) 2007; 61
Rodríguez, Snedden (CR59) 2004; 68
Endler (CR16) 1992; 139
Proctor (CR52) 1991; 42
Kose, Møller (CR33) 1999; 45
Basolo (CR4) 1990; 250
Ryan, Cummings (CR85) 2013; 44
Soma (CR72) 2018; 68
Summers, Symula, Clough, Cronin (CR78) 1999; 266
Soma, Iwama (CR75) 2017; 12
Swaddle, Cuthill (CR79) 1994; 258
Geberzahn, Gahr (CR25) 2011; 6
Soma, Garamszegi (CR73) 2015; 3
Bates, Mächler, Bolker, Walker (CR6) 2015; 67
Endler, Basolo (CR17) 1998; 13
Zanollo, Griggio, Robertson, Kleindorfer (CR83) 2012; 153
Payne, del Hoyo (CR50) 2010
Crothers, Gering, Cummings (CR12) 2011; 65
Kupfer, Fessler (CR34) 2018; 373
Goodwin (CR27) 1982
Martin (CR41) 2007; 148
McGraw, Mackillop, Dale, Hauber (CR44) 2002; 205
Basolo (CR3) 1990; 30
Madden, Tanner (CR39) 2003; 65
Maan, Cummings (CR37) 2009; 106
Borgia, Keagy (CR8) 2006; 72
Proctor (CR53) 1992; 44
Griggio, Valera, Casas-Crivillé, Hoi, Barbosa (CR29) 2011; 65
Ota, Gahr, Soma (CR49) 2015; 5
Rodd, Hughes, Grether, Baril (CR58) 2002; 269
Caves, Brandley, Johnsen (CR9) 2018; 33
Cole, Wilkins (CR10) 2013; 24
Ryan, Fox, Wilczynski, Rand (CR69) 1990; 343
Bischof (CR7) 1988; 163
Roulin (CR62) 1999; 10
Ryan, Rand (CR68) 1993; 340
Ryan, Keddy-Hector (CR66) 1992; 139
Basolo (CR5) 1995; 259
Marques, Batalha, Cardoso (CR40) 2016; 3
Dawkins (CR15) 2002; 63
Gomes, Funghi, Soma, Sorenson, Cardoso (CR26) 2017; 30
Prum, Torres, Williamson, Dyck (CR55) 1999; 266
Fuller, Endler (CR20) 2018; 64
Maan, Cummings (CR36) 2008; 62
Ruiz-de-Castañeda, Burtt, Gonzalez-Braojos, Moreno (CR64) 2012; 105
Zahavi (CR81) 1975; 53
CR56
Andersson (CR2) 1986; 40
Ryan (CR65) 1998; 281
Alatalo, Höglund, Lundberg, Sutherland (CR1) 1992; 3
Somveille, Marshall, Gluckman (CR76) 2016; 4
Průchová, Nedvěd, Veselý, Ernestová, Fuchs (CR54) 2014; 151
Ryan, Rand (CR67) 1990; 44
Reynolds, Fitzpatrick (CR57) 2007; 61
Rosenthal (CR61) 2018; 64
Garcia, Ramirez (CR24) 2005; 434
Moreno, López-Arrabé (CR46) 2021; 162
Siddiqi, Cronin, Loew, Vorobyev, Summers (CR70) 2004; 207
Soulsbury, Kervinen, Lebigre (CR77) 2016; 27
Zahavi (CR82) 1977; 67
Galván, Alonso-Alvarez (CR23) 2008; 3
Ron (CR60) 2008; 76
Endler, Mappes (CR18) 2017; 372
Roulin (CR63) 2016; 91
Gahr, Güttinger (CR22) 1986; 72
Guindre-Parker, Love (CR30) 2014; 45
Mizuno, Soma (CR45) 2020; 33
Martin (CR42) 2017
Maan, Cummings (CR38) 2012; 179
Fuller, Houle, Travis (CR21) 2005; 166
Kupfer, Le (CR35) 2018; 32
Soma, Garamszegi (CR74) 2018; 29
Morris (CR47) 1958; 131
Grafen (CR28) 1990; 144
Hanssen, Hasselquist, Folstad, Erikstad (CR31) 2008; 4
Piault, van den Brink, Roulin (CR51) 2012; 99
RO Prum (1609_CR55) 1999; 266
EM Caves (1609_CR9) 2018; 33
M Soma (1609_CR74) 2018; 29
TR Kupfer (1609_CR35) 2018; 32
RC Fuller (1609_CR21) 2005; 166
S Guindre-Parker (1609_CR30) 2014; 45
JA Endler (1609_CR18) 2017; 372
HJ Bischof (1609_CR7) 1988; 163
GR Martin (1609_CR41) 2007; 148
RV Alatalo (1609_CR1) 1992; 3
AL Basolo (1609_CR5) 1995; 259
D Goodwin (1609_CR27) 1982
CD Soulsbury (1609_CR77) 2016; 27
SA Hanssen (1609_CR31) 2008; 4
ACR Gomes (1609_CR26) 2017; 30
RB Payne (1609_CR50) 2010
HC Proctor (1609_CR53) 1992; 44
D Morris (1609_CR47) 1958; 131
ME Maan (1609_CR37) 2009; 106
TR Kupfer (1609_CR34) 2018; 373
CI Marques (1609_CR40) 2016; 3
RC Fuller (1609_CR20) 2018; 64
A Roulin (1609_CR63) 2016; 91
R Ruiz-de-Castañeda (1609_CR64) 2012; 105
C Garcia (1609_CR24) 2005; 434
MJ Ryan (1609_CR65) 1998; 281
CJ Crowhurst (1609_CR13) 2012; 118
J Moreno (1609_CR46) 2021; 162
SR Ron (1609_CR60) 2008; 76
JP Swaddle (1609_CR79) 1994; 258
JR Madden (1609_CR39) 2003; 65
GG Rosenthal (1609_CR61) 2018; 64
A Grafen (1609_CR28) 1990; 144
MJ Ryan (1609_CR69) 1990; 343
M Soma (1609_CR75) 2017; 12
V Zanollo (1609_CR83) 2012; 153
A Mizuno (1609_CR45) 2020; 33
M Soma (1609_CR72) 2018; 68
JA Endler (1609_CR16) 1992; 139
R Piault (1609_CR51) 2012; 99
MS Dawkins (1609_CR15) 2002; 63
MJ Ryan (1609_CR85) 2013; 44
M Somveille (1609_CR76) 2016; 4
N Ota (1609_CR49) 2015; 5
M Griggio (1609_CR29) 2011; 65
ME Maan (1609_CR36) 2008; 62
JA Endler (1609_CR17) 1998; 13
MJ Ryan (1609_CR67) 1990; 44
N Geberzahn (1609_CR25) 2011; 6
K Summers (1609_CR78) 1999; 266
G Borgia (1609_CR8) 2006; 72
GG Cole (1609_CR10) 2013; 24
V Zanollo (1609_CR84) 2013; 119
GR Martin (1609_CR42) 2017
A Siddiqi (1609_CR70) 2004; 207
AL Basolo (1609_CR4) 1990; 250
JW McGlothlin (1609_CR43) 2007; 61
1609_CR56
M Gahr (1609_CR22) 1986; 72
AL Basolo (1609_CR3) 1990; 30
M Kose (1609_CR33) 1999; 45
CR Darst (1609_CR14) 2006; 103
ME Maan (1609_CR38) 2012; 179
KJ McGraw (1609_CR44) 2002; 205
A Zahavi (1609_CR82) 1977; 67
RL Rodríguez (1609_CR59) 2004; 68
I Galván (1609_CR23) 2008; 3
HC Proctor (1609_CR52) 1991; 42
M Soma (1609_CR73) 2015; 3
FH Rodd (1609_CR58) 2002; 269
MJ Ryan (1609_CR68) 1993; 340
A Zahavi (1609_CR81) 1975; 53
M Andersson (1609_CR2) 1986; 40
D Bates (1609_CR6) 2015; 67
A Průchová (1609_CR54) 2014; 151
RG Reynolds (1609_CR57) 2007; 61
A Roulin (1609_CR62) 1999; 10
L Crothers (1609_CR12) 2011; 65
MJ Ryan (1609_CR66) 1992; 139
References_xml – volume: 144
  start-page: 517
  year: 1990
  end-page: 546
  ident: CR28
  article-title: Biological signals as handicaps
  publication-title: J Theor Biol
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8
– volume: 340
  start-page: 187
  year: 1993
  end-page: 195
  ident: CR68
  article-title: Sexual selection and signal evolution: the ghost of biases past
  publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.1993.0057
– volume: 119
  start-page: 344
  year: 2013
  end-page: 352
  ident: CR84
  article-title: Males with a faster courtship display have more white spots and higher pairing success in the Diamond Firetail,
  publication-title: Ethology
  doi: 10.1111/eth.12071
– volume: 45
  start-page: 29
  year: 2014
  end-page: 33
  ident: CR30
  article-title: Revisiting the condition-dependence of melanin-based plumage
  publication-title: J Avian Biol
  doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00190.x
– volume: 33
  start-page: 49
  year: 2020
  end-page: 61
  ident: CR45
  article-title: Potential role of sensory bias in plumage pattern evolution: termite-eating and polka-dots in estrildid finches
  publication-title: Ethol Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1080/03949370.2020.1803414
– volume: 13
  start-page: 415
  year: 1998
  end-page: 420
  ident: CR17
  article-title: Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection
  publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(98)01471-2
– volume: 434
  start-page: 501
  year: 2005
  end-page: 505
  ident: CR24
  article-title: Evidence that sensory traps can evolve into honest signals
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/nature03363
– volume: 76
  start-page: 1783
  year: 2008
  end-page: 1794
  ident: CR60
  article-title: The evolution of female mate choice for complex calls in túngara frogs
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.024
– volume: 266
  start-page: 2141
  year: 1999
  end-page: 2145
  ident: CR78
  article-title: Visual mate choice in poison frogs
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0900
– volume: 269
  start-page: 475
  year: 2002
  end-page: 481
  ident: CR58
  article-title: A possible non-sexual origin of mate preference: are male guppies mimicking fruit?
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1891
– volume: 179
  start-page: E1
  year: 2012
  end-page: E14
  ident: CR38
  article-title: Poison frog colors are honest signals of toxicity, particularly for bird predators
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/663197
– volume: 64
  start-page: 485
  year: 2018
  end-page: 492
  ident: CR61
  article-title: Evaluation and hedonic value in mate choice
  publication-title: Curr Zool
  doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy054
– volume: 343
  start-page: 66
  year: 1990
  end-page: 67
  ident: CR69
  article-title: Sexual selection for sensory exploitation in the frog
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/343066a0
– start-page: 234
  year: 2010
  end-page: 377
  ident: CR50
  article-title: Handbook of the birds of the world
  publication-title: Barcelona
– volume: 4
  year: 2016
  ident: CR76
  article-title: A global analysis of bird plumage patterns reveals no association between habitat and camouflage
  publication-title: PeerJ
  doi: 10.7717/peerj.2658
– volume: 10
  start-page: 688
  year: 1999
  end-page: 695
  ident: CR62
  article-title: Nonrandom pairing by male barn owls ( ) with respect to a female plumage trait
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.6.688
– volume: 44
  start-page: 305
  year: 1990
  end-page: 314
  ident: CR67
  article-title: The sensory basis of sexual selection for complex calls in the túngara frog, (sexual selection for sensory exploitation)
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1990.tb05200.x
– volume: 91
  start-page: 328
  year: 2016
  end-page: 348
  ident: CR63
  article-title: Condition-dependence, pleiotropy and the handicap principle of sexual selection in melanin-based colouration
  publication-title: Biol Rev
  doi: 10.1111/brv.12171
– volume: 68
  start-page: 121
  year: 2018
  end-page: 130
  ident: CR72
  article-title: Sexual selection in Estrildid finches, with further review of the evolution of nesting material holding display in relation to cooperative parental nesting
  publication-title: Jan J Anim Psychol
  doi: 10.2502/janip.68.2.2
– volume: 5
  start-page: 16614
  year: 2015
  ident: CR49
  article-title: Tap dancing birds: the multimodal mutual courtship display of males and females in a socially monogamous songbird
  publication-title: Sci Rep
  doi: 10.1038/srep16614
– volume: 12
  year: 2017
  ident: CR75
  article-title: Mating success follows duet dancing in the Java sparrow
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172655
– volume: 61
  start-page: 1391
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1399
  ident: CR43
  article-title: Diet quality affects an attractive white plumage pattern in dark-eyed juncos ( )
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0370-x
– volume: 205
  start-page: 3747
  year: 2002
  end-page: 3755
  ident: CR44
  article-title: Different colors reveal different information: how nutritional stress affects the expression of melanin-and structurally based ornamental plumage
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747
– volume: 250
  start-page: 808
  year: 1990
  end-page: 810
  ident: CR4
  article-title: Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.250.4982.808
– volume: 266
  start-page: 13
  year: 1999
  end-page: 22
  ident: CR55
  article-title: Two-dimensional Fourier analysis of the spongy medullary keratin of structurally coloured feather barbs
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0598
– year: 2017
  ident: CR42
  publication-title: The sensory ecology of birds
  doi: 10.1093/oso/9780199694532.001.0001
– volume: 162
  start-page: 511
  year: 2021
  end-page: 520
  ident: CR46
  article-title: The extent of white plumage patches in female Pied Flycatchers is negatively associated with corticosterone concentration in partly unpigmented feathers
  publication-title: J Ornithol
  doi: 10.1007/s10336-020-01851-z
– volume: 99
  start-page: 391
  year: 2012
  end-page: 396
  ident: CR51
  article-title: Condition-dependent expression of melanin-based coloration in the Eurasian kestrel
  publication-title: Naturwissenschaften
  doi: 10.1007/s00114-012-0914-4
– volume: 29
  start-page: 676
  year: 2018
  end-page: 685
  ident: CR74
  article-title: Evolution of patterned plumage as a sexual signal in Estrildid finches
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/ary021
– year: 1982
  ident: CR27
  publication-title: Estrildid finches of the world
– volume: 45
  start-page: 430
  year: 1999
  end-page: 436
  ident: CR33
  article-title: Sexual selection, feather breakage and parasites: the importance of white spots in the tail of the barn swallow ( )
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s002650050581
– volume: 65
  start-page: 1077
  year: 2003
  end-page: 1083
  ident: CR39
  article-title: Preferences for coloured bower decorations can be explained in a nonsexual context
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2126
– volume: 42
  start-page: 589
  year: 1991
  end-page: 598
  ident: CR52
  article-title: Courtship in the water mite : males capitalize on female adaptations for predation
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8
– volume: 72
  start-page: 123
  year: 1986
  end-page: 131
  ident: CR22
  article-title: Functional aspects of singing in male and female (Estrildidae)
  publication-title: Ethology
  doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00612.x
– volume: 139
  start-page: S4
  year: 1992
  end-page: S35
  ident: CR66
  article-title: Directional patterns of female mate choice and the role of sensory biases
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/285303
– volume: 163
  start-page: 329
  year: 1988
  end-page: 337
  ident: CR7
  article-title: The visual field and visually guided behavior in the zebra finch ( )
  publication-title: J Comp Physiol
  doi: 10.1007/BF00604008
– volume: 67
  start-page: 1
  year: 2015
  end-page: 48
  ident: CR6
  article-title: Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4
  publication-title: J Stat Softw
  doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
– volume: 151
  start-page: 128
  year: 2014
  end-page: 134
  ident: CR54
  article-title: Visual warning signals of the ladybird : the avian predators’ point of view
  publication-title: Entomol Exp Appl
  doi: 10.1111/eea.12176
– volume: 258
  start-page: 267
  year: 1994
  end-page: 271
  ident: CR79
  article-title: Female Zebra Finches prefer males with symmetric chest plumage
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0172
– volume: 105
  start-page: 409
  year: 2012
  end-page: 419
  ident: CR64
  article-title: Bacterial degradability of an intrafeather unmelanized ornament: a role for feather-degrading bacteria in sexual selection?
  publication-title: Biol J Linn Soc
  doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01806.x
– volume: 68
  start-page: 427
  year: 2004
  end-page: 432
  ident: CR59
  article-title: On the functional design of mate preferences and receiver biases
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.031
– volume: 32
  start-page: 729
  year: 2018
  end-page: 741
  ident: CR35
  article-title: Disgusting clusters: trypophobia as an overgeneralised disease avoidance response
  publication-title: Cogn Emot
  doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1345721
– volume: 6
  year: 2011
  ident: CR25
  article-title: Undirected (solitary) birdsong in female and male blue-capped cordon-bleus ( ) and its endocrine correlates
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026485
– volume: 44
  start-page: 745
  year: 1992
  end-page: 752
  ident: CR53
  article-title: Sensory exploitation and the evolution of male mating behaviour: a cladistic test using water mites (Acari: Parasitengona)
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8
– volume: 259
  start-page: 307
  year: 1995
  end-page: 311
  ident: CR5
  article-title: Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-existing bias in sexual selection
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0045
– volume: 65
  start-page: 599
  year: 2011
  end-page: 605
  ident: CR12
  article-title: Aposematic signal variation predicts male–male interactions in a polymorphic poison frog
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x
– volume: 103
  start-page: 5852
  year: 2006
  end-page: 5857
  ident: CR14
  article-title: A mechanism for diversity in warning signals: conspicuousness versus toxicity in poison frogs
  publication-title: PNAS
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600625103
– volume: 63
  start-page: 991
  year: 2002
  end-page: 998
  ident: CR15
  article-title: What are birds looking at? Head movements and eye use in chickens
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.1999
– volume: 373
  start-page: 20170207
  year: 2018
  ident: CR34
  article-title: Ectoparasite defence in humans: relationships to pathogen avoidance and clinical implications
  publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0207
– volume: 166
  start-page: 437
  year: 2005
  end-page: 446
  ident: CR21
  article-title: Sensory bias as an explanation for the evolution of mate preferences
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/444443
– ident: CR56
– volume: 53
  start-page: 205
  year: 1975
  end-page: 214
  ident: CR81
  article-title: Mate selection—a selection for a handicap
  publication-title: J Theor Biol
  doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3
– volume: 44
  start-page: 437
  issue: 1
  year: 2013
  end-page: 459
  ident: CR85
  article-title: Perceptual biases and mate choice
  publication-title: Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst
  doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901
– volume: 40
  start-page: 804
  year: 1986
  end-page: 816
  ident: CR2
  article-title: Evolution of condition-dependent sex ornaments and mating preferences: sexual selection based on viability differences
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb00540.x
– volume: 118
  start-page: 63
  year: 2012
  end-page: 75
  ident: CR13
  article-title: White flank spots signal feeding dominance in female diamond firetails,
  publication-title: Ethology
  doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01986.x
– volume: 62
  start-page: 2334
  year: 2008
  end-page: 2345
  ident: CR36
  article-title: Female preferences for aposematic signal components in a polymorphic poison frog
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00454.x
– volume: 131
  start-page: 389
  year: 1958
  end-page: 439
  ident: CR47
  article-title: The comparative ethology of grassfinches (Erythrurae) and mannikins (Amadinae)
  publication-title: Proc Zool Soc Lond
  doi: 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1958.tb00695.x
– volume: 281
  start-page: 1999
  year: 1998
  end-page: 2003
  ident: CR65
  article-title: Sexual selection, receiver biases, and the evolution of sex differences
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.281.5385.1999
– volume: 207
  start-page: 2471
  year: 2004
  end-page: 2485
  ident: CR70
  article-title: Interspecific and intraspecific views of color signals in the strawberry poison frog
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.01047
– volume: 27
  start-page: 1362
  year: 2016
  end-page: 1369
  ident: CR77
  article-title: Curse of the black spot: spotting negatively correlates with fitness in black grouse
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/arw057
– volume: 148
  start-page: 547
  year: 2007
  end-page: 562
  ident: CR41
  article-title: Visual fields and their functions in birds
  publication-title: J Ornithol
  doi: 10.1007/s10336-007-0213-6
– volume: 106
  start-page: 19072
  year: 2009
  end-page: 19077
  ident: CR37
  article-title: Sexual dimorphism and directional sexual selection on aposematic signals in a poison frog
  publication-title: PNAS
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903327106
– volume: 3
  start-page: 4
  year: 2015
  ident: CR73
  article-title: Evolution of courtship display in Estrildid finches: dance in relation to female song and plumage ornamentation
  publication-title: Front Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00004
– volume: 3
  start-page: 53
  year: 1992
  end-page: 59
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Evolution of black grouse leks: female preferences benefit males in larger leks
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/3.1.53
– volume: 30
  start-page: A80
  year: 1990
  ident: CR3
  article-title: Preexisting mating biases and the evolution of the sword in the genus Xiphophorus
  publication-title: Am Zool
– volume: 153
  start-page: 1233
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1244
  ident: CR83
  article-title: The number and coloration of white flank spots predict the strength of a cutaneous immune response in female Diamond Firetails,
  publication-title: J Ornithol
  doi: 10.1007/s10336-012-0855-x
– volume: 72
  start-page: 1125
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1133
  ident: CR8
  article-title: An inverse relationship between decoration and food colour preferences in satin bowerbirds does not support the sensory drive hypothesis
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.015
– volume: 372
  start-page: 20160352
  year: 2017
  ident: CR18
  article-title: The current and future state of animal coloration research
  publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0352
– volume: 67
  start-page: 603
  year: 1977
  end-page: 605
  ident: CR82
  article-title: The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle)
  publication-title: J Theor Biol
  doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(77)90061-3
– volume: 65
  start-page: 655
  year: 2011
  end-page: 664
  ident: CR29
  article-title: White tail markings are an indicator of quality and affect mate preference in rock sparrows
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1067-0
– volume: 33
  start-page: 358
  year: 2018
  end-page: 372
  ident: CR9
  article-title: Visual acuity and the evolution of signals
  publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001
– volume: 3
  year: 2008
  ident: CR23
  article-title: An intracellular antioxidant determines the expression of a melanin-based signal in a bird
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003335
– volume: 3
  year: 2016
  ident: CR40
  article-title: Signalling with a cryptic trait: the regularity of barred plumage in common waxbills
  publication-title: R Soc Open Sci
  doi: 10.1098/rsos.160195
– volume: 64
  start-page: 465
  year: 2018
  end-page: 470
  ident: CR20
  article-title: A perspective on sensory drive
  publication-title: Curr Zool
  doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy052
– volume: 139
  start-page: S125
  year: 1992
  end-page: S153
  ident: CR16
  article-title: Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/285308
– volume: 24
  start-page: 1980
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1985
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Fear of holes
  publication-title: Psychol Sci
  doi: 10.1177/0956797613484937
– volume: 30
  start-page: 1336
  year: 2017
  end-page: 1346
  ident: CR26
  article-title: Multimodal signalling in estrildid finches: song, dance and colour are associated with different ecological and life-history traits
  publication-title: J Evol Biol
  doi: 10.1111/jeb.13102
– volume: 61
  start-page: 2253
  year: 2007
  end-page: 2259
  ident: CR57
  article-title: Assortative mating in poison-dart frogs based on an ecologically important trait
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00174.x
– volume: 4
  start-page: 379
  year: 2008
  end-page: 381
  ident: CR31
  article-title: A label of health: a previous immune challenge is reflected in the expression of a female plumage trait
  publication-title: Biol Lett
  doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0131
– start-page: 234
  volume-title: Barcelona
  year: 2010
  ident: 1609_CR50
– volume-title: The sensory ecology of birds
  year: 2017
  ident: 1609_CR42
  doi: 10.1093/oso/9780199694532.001.0001
– volume: 4
  start-page: 379
  year: 2008
  ident: 1609_CR31
  publication-title: Biol Lett
  doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0131
– volume: 68
  start-page: 121
  year: 2018
  ident: 1609_CR72
  publication-title: Jan J Anim Psychol
  doi: 10.2502/janip.68.2.2
– volume: 65
  start-page: 655
  year: 2011
  ident: 1609_CR29
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1067-0
– volume: 45
  start-page: 29
  year: 2014
  ident: 1609_CR30
  publication-title: J Avian Biol
  doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00190.x
– volume: 68
  start-page: 427
  year: 2004
  ident: 1609_CR59
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.031
– volume: 343
  start-page: 66
  year: 1990
  ident: 1609_CR69
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/343066a0
– volume: 27
  start-page: 1362
  year: 2016
  ident: 1609_CR77
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/arw057
– volume: 269
  start-page: 475
  year: 2002
  ident: 1609_CR58
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.2001.1891
– volume: 3
  year: 2008
  ident: 1609_CR23
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003335
– volume: 61
  start-page: 1391
  year: 2007
  ident: 1609_CR43
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0370-x
– volume: 3
  start-page: 53
  year: 1992
  ident: 1609_CR1
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/3.1.53
– volume: 373
  start-page: 20170207
  year: 2018
  ident: 1609_CR34
  publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0207
– volume: 29
  start-page: 676
  year: 2018
  ident: 1609_CR74
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/ary021
– volume: 105
  start-page: 409
  year: 2012
  ident: 1609_CR64
  publication-title: Biol J Linn Soc
  doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01806.x
– volume: 144
  start-page: 517
  year: 1990
  ident: 1609_CR28
  publication-title: J Theor Biol
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8
– volume: 205
  start-page: 3747
  year: 2002
  ident: 1609_CR44
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.23.3747
– volume: 281
  start-page: 1999
  year: 1998
  ident: 1609_CR65
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.281.5385.1999
– volume: 44
  start-page: 305
  year: 1990
  ident: 1609_CR67
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1990.tb05200.x
– volume: 67
  start-page: 1
  year: 2015
  ident: 1609_CR6
  publication-title: J Stat Softw
  doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
– volume: 33
  start-page: 358
  year: 2018
  ident: 1609_CR9
  publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001
– volume: 64
  start-page: 465
  year: 2018
  ident: 1609_CR20
  publication-title: Curr Zool
  doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy052
– volume: 63
  start-page: 991
  year: 2002
  ident: 1609_CR15
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.1999
– volume: 72
  start-page: 1125
  year: 2006
  ident: 1609_CR8
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.03.015
– volume: 162
  start-page: 511
  year: 2021
  ident: 1609_CR46
  publication-title: J Ornithol
  doi: 10.1007/s10336-020-01851-z
– volume: 207
  start-page: 2471
  year: 2004
  ident: 1609_CR70
  publication-title: J Exp Biol
  doi: 10.1242/jeb.01047
– volume: 103
  start-page: 5852
  year: 2006
  ident: 1609_CR14
  publication-title: PNAS
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600625103
– volume: 33
  start-page: 49
  year: 2020
  ident: 1609_CR45
  publication-title: Ethol Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1080/03949370.2020.1803414
– volume: 340
  start-page: 187
  year: 1993
  ident: 1609_CR68
  publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.1993.0057
– volume: 106
  start-page: 19072
  year: 2009
  ident: 1609_CR37
  publication-title: PNAS
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903327106
– volume: 119
  start-page: 344
  year: 2013
  ident: 1609_CR84
  publication-title: Ethology
  doi: 10.1111/eth.12071
– volume-title: Estrildid finches of the world
  year: 1982
  ident: 1609_CR27
– volume: 53
  start-page: 205
  year: 1975
  ident: 1609_CR81
  publication-title: J Theor Biol
  doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3
– volume: 434
  start-page: 501
  year: 2005
  ident: 1609_CR24
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/nature03363
– volume: 32
  start-page: 729
  year: 2018
  ident: 1609_CR35
  publication-title: Cogn Emot
  doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1345721
– volume: 5
  start-page: 16614
  year: 2015
  ident: 1609_CR49
  publication-title: Sci Rep
  doi: 10.1038/srep16614
– volume: 266
  start-page: 13
  year: 1999
  ident: 1609_CR55
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0598
– volume: 166
  start-page: 437
  year: 2005
  ident: 1609_CR21
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/444443
– volume: 6
  year: 2011
  ident: 1609_CR25
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026485
– volume: 99
  start-page: 391
  year: 2012
  ident: 1609_CR51
  publication-title: Naturwissenschaften
  doi: 10.1007/s00114-012-0914-4
– volume: 3
  year: 2016
  ident: 1609_CR40
  publication-title: R Soc Open Sci
  doi: 10.1098/rsos.160195
– volume: 10
  start-page: 688
  year: 1999
  ident: 1609_CR62
  publication-title: Behav Ecol
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/10.6.688
– volume: 30
  start-page: A80
  year: 1990
  ident: 1609_CR3
  publication-title: Am Zool
– volume: 42
  start-page: 589
  year: 1991
  ident: 1609_CR52
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8
– volume: 4
  year: 2016
  ident: 1609_CR76
  publication-title: PeerJ
  doi: 10.7717/peerj.2658
– volume: 163
  start-page: 329
  year: 1988
  ident: 1609_CR7
  publication-title: J Comp Physiol
  doi: 10.1007/BF00604008
– volume: 65
  start-page: 1077
  year: 2003
  ident: 1609_CR39
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2126
– volume: 131
  start-page: 389
  year: 1958
  ident: 1609_CR47
  publication-title: Proc Zool Soc Lond
  doi: 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1958.tb00695.x
– volume: 67
  start-page: 603
  year: 1977
  ident: 1609_CR82
  publication-title: J Theor Biol
  doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(77)90061-3
– volume: 44
  start-page: 745
  year: 1992
  ident: 1609_CR53
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8
– volume: 372
  start-page: 20160352
  year: 2017
  ident: 1609_CR18
  publication-title: Philos Trans R Soc B
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0352
– ident: 1609_CR56
– volume: 65
  start-page: 599
  year: 2011
  ident: 1609_CR12
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x
– volume: 64
  start-page: 485
  year: 2018
  ident: 1609_CR61
  publication-title: Curr Zool
  doi: 10.1093/cz/zoy054
– volume: 30
  start-page: 1336
  year: 2017
  ident: 1609_CR26
  publication-title: J Evol Biol
  doi: 10.1111/jeb.13102
– volume: 45
  start-page: 430
  year: 1999
  ident: 1609_CR33
  publication-title: Behav Ecol Sociobiol
  doi: 10.1007/s002650050581
– volume: 250
  start-page: 808
  year: 1990
  ident: 1609_CR4
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.250.4982.808
– volume: 12
  year: 2017
  ident: 1609_CR75
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172655
– volume: 139
  start-page: S4
  year: 1992
  ident: 1609_CR66
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/285303
– volume: 118
  start-page: 63
  year: 2012
  ident: 1609_CR13
  publication-title: Ethology
  doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01986.x
– volume: 62
  start-page: 2334
  year: 2008
  ident: 1609_CR36
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00454.x
– volume: 91
  start-page: 328
  year: 2016
  ident: 1609_CR63
  publication-title: Biol Rev
  doi: 10.1111/brv.12171
– volume: 3
  start-page: 4
  year: 2015
  ident: 1609_CR73
  publication-title: Front Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00004
– volume: 72
  start-page: 123
  year: 1986
  ident: 1609_CR22
  publication-title: Ethology
  doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00612.x
– volume: 148
  start-page: 547
  year: 2007
  ident: 1609_CR41
  publication-title: J Ornithol
  doi: 10.1007/s10336-007-0213-6
– volume: 179
  start-page: E1
  year: 2012
  ident: 1609_CR38
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/663197
– volume: 40
  start-page: 804
  year: 1986
  ident: 1609_CR2
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1986.tb00540.x
– volume: 61
  start-page: 2253
  year: 2007
  ident: 1609_CR57
  publication-title: Evolution
  doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00174.x
– volume: 76
  start-page: 1783
  year: 2008
  ident: 1609_CR60
  publication-title: Anim Behav
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.024
– volume: 44
  start-page: 437
  issue: 1
  year: 2013
  ident: 1609_CR85
  publication-title: Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst
  doi: 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110512-135901
– volume: 13
  start-page: 415
  year: 1998
  ident: 1609_CR17
  publication-title: Trends Ecol Evol
  doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(98)01471-2
– volume: 266
  start-page: 2141
  year: 1999
  ident: 1609_CR78
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0900
– volume: 258
  start-page: 267
  year: 1994
  ident: 1609_CR79
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0172
– volume: 139
  start-page: S125
  year: 1992
  ident: 1609_CR16
  publication-title: Am Nat
  doi: 10.1086/285308
– volume: 259
  start-page: 307
  year: 1995
  ident: 1609_CR5
  publication-title: Proc R Soc Lond B
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0045
– volume: 24
  start-page: 1980
  year: 2013
  ident: 1609_CR10
  publication-title: Psychol Sci
  doi: 10.1177/0956797613484937
– volume: 151
  start-page: 128
  year: 2014
  ident: 1609_CR54
  publication-title: Entomol Exp Appl
  doi: 10.1111/eea.12176
– volume: 153
  start-page: 1233
  year: 2012
  ident: 1609_CR83
  publication-title: J Ornithol
  doi: 10.1007/s10336-012-0855-x
SSID ssj0008491
Score 2.3403766
Snippet Many animals have polka dot patterns on their body surface, some of which are known to have signalling functions; however, their evolutionary origins remain...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
springer
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 1271
SubjectTerms Animals
Aposematism
Behavioral Sciences
Bias
Biological evolution
Biomedical and Life Sciences
Comparative studies
Dietary restrictions
Evolution
Feathers - chemistry
Feathers - physiology
Finches - physiology
Food
Food availability
Foraging behavior
Humans
Hunger
Hypotheses
Life Sciences
Natural selection
Neochmia ruficauda
Original Paper
Passeriformes
Phylogeny
Pigmentation - physiology
Plumage
Preferences
Prey
Psychology Research
Signaling
Zoology
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: SpringerLink Journals (ICM)
  dbid: U2A
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Na9wwEB3alEIuJU0_4iQtKvTWCipblq3cQmkIhebUhdCLkWSJXUhts7tOyb_PjNZ2sk1b6M2gsWx5JM0bz-gNwHttPVpNW3JV1I7LLNRcG624rUPIbJapMma7f7tQ5zP59TK_HA6FrcZs9zEkGXfqe4fd0Bxyyj4nVjTN88fwJEffneb1LD2d9t9Sxjp5BAS4Ru9jOCrz5z62zdEDjPkwVfK3eGk0Q2d78GzAj-x0o_Dn8Mg3-7A7bWM3-_D0RxuvXkCLSHLJwqKholjswrdu_nNh2LIPqJm-Nmxurj0z7Hqx6rHPbio5whDJsl8UYGDotLIucnA2qxOU7VpaRFeeOTR_rA1svbzp2o5-y5iXMDv78v3zOR8KLHCHuGPNgyePwSr7KUsLIrbXRN4jChHqVAadB7TvRTClKJwUqQhGeV17I0wotTS5zF7BTtM2_gBY6lxR1uj7WFFL73ObOY99KIs3WwQNCYjxO1duYB-nIhhX1R1vMummQt1UUTdVnsCH6Z5uw73xT-njUX3VsA5XVYq-A3p06FMl8G5qxhVEYRHT-LZHGSVx_FIX2MXrjbanxyE81VKVMoFiax5MAsTOvd3SLOaRpVvT4UspEvg4zpi71_r7KA7_T_wIdlOazTG78Bh21svev0GUtLZv46K4BXIPCMQ
  priority: 102
  providerName: Springer Nature
Title Star finches Neochmia ruficauda have a visual preference for white dot patterns: a possible case of trypophilia
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10071-022-01609-5
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35294684
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2729996768
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2640324975
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9617841
Volume 25
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3fb9MwELZgE9Je0Bg_FjYmI_EGFkvi2DEvqFQdE4gKISoVXiLbsdVKIwltM7T_njs3TVUm9pJEsePEOdv3nX3-jpBXyjjQmiZnQpaW8dSXTGklmCm9T02aijx4u38Zi8sJ_zTNpt2E27Jzq9yMiWGgLmuLc-RvE0CBgM0BHb9vfjOMGoWrq10IjftkH6nL0PiS097gOs95iJiHkIApsEO6TTPd1jlQrgx92ZFjTbFsVzHdQpu3nSb_WTkNCunikDzskCQdrEX_iNxz1RE56Ae0myPy4Gcdrh6TGjDlgvp5heGx6NjVdvZrrumi9SCjttR0pq8d1fR6vmyhzKYPPkIB09I_uNRAwXylTWDjrJbvIG9TY3e6ctSCIqS1p6vFTVM3OEGjn5DJxej78JJ1oRaYBQSyYt6h7WCEOU8TiRT3Cml8Yhn7MuFeZR40vfQ6j6XlcRJ7LZwqnY61zxXXGU-fkr2qrtwxoYm1Mi_BCjJxyZ3LTGodlCEMPGwAPkQk3vznwnY85BgO46rYMiijbAqQTRFkU2QRed0_06xZOO7MfboRX9H1yGWxbT8RedknQ1_CBRJdubqFPIJD_bmSUMSztbT71wFQVVzkPCJypx30GZCnezelms8CX7fCbZg8jsibTYvZftb_a_H87lqckIMEW2_wKzwle6tF614APlqZs9AJzsj-4OOPzyM4fxiNv36Du0MxhOMkGfwF1OARFQ
linkProvider ProQuest
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9QwEB6VIkQvCMorUMBIcAKrm8R5GAkhBFRb2u6plVZcgp3Y2pVKEnY3rfZP8RuZ8SZZLRW99RbJj8SZsecbe_wNwBupDVpNnfI4KXIuQltwqWTMdWFtqMMwTl20-8koHp6J7-NovAV_urswFFbZrYluoS6qnPbI9wNEgYjNER1_qn9zyhpFp6tdCo2VWhyZ5SW6bPOPh19Rvm-D4ODb6Zchb7MK8ByN7YJbQzBZx3oQBgmxuUtirPET3xaBsDKyaNQSq1I_yYUf-FbFRhZG-cqmUqhIhNjvLbiNhndAIYTJuHfwBqlwGfoIgnCJfk97Sae9qofGnFPsPHG6SR5tGsIr6PZqkOY_J7XOAB7ch3stcmWfV6r2ALZMuQs7_QK63IU7Pyr39BAqxLAzZqclpeNiI1Plk19TxWaNRZ1oCsUm6sIwxS6m8wb7rPtkJwwxNLukow2G7jKrHftnOf-AdeuKpu-5YTkaXlZZtpgt66qmDSH1CM5uRAiPYbusSvMUWJDnSVqg16X9QhgT6TA32EessbFGuOKB3_3nLG95zyn9xnm2Zmwm2WQom8zJJos8eNe3qVesH9fW3uvEl7UrwDxb66sHr_tinLt0IKNKUzVYJxY4fiET7OLJStr96xAYSxGnwoNkQw_6CsQLvllSTieOH1zStU_he_C-05j1Z_1_FM-uH8UruDs8PTnOjg9HR89hJyBNdjGNe7C9mDXmBWKzhX7pJgSDnzc9A_8C7hhHYg
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3fb9MwED6NIdBeJhi_MgYYCZ7A2pI4cYyEEGJUG4OKByZVvGS2Y6uVRhLaZlP_Nf46zm6SqkzsbW-R4jhx7s73nX3-DuCVUAa9pspoygtNWWwLKqRIqSqsjVUcp5nPdv82TI9O2ZdRMtqAP91ZGJdW2c2JfqIuKu3WyPcjRIGIzREd79s2LeL74eBD_Zu6ClJup7Urp7FUkROzuMTwbfb--BBl_TqKBp9_fDqibYUBqtHxzqk1DjKrVB3EEXfM7sKx14Q8tEXErEgsOjhuZRZyzcIotDI1ojAylDYTTCYsxn5vwW0eo9tEW-KjPtg7yJiv1ufgCBUYA7UHdtpje-jYqcujd_xugibrTvEK0r2asPnPrq13hoN7sN2iWPJxqXb3YcOUO7DVT6aLHbjzs_JXD6BCPDsldlK60lxkaCo9_jWRZNpY1I-mkGQsLwyR5GIya7DPui98QhBPk0u3zUEwdCa1ZwItZ--wbV05Uz43RKMTJpUl8-mirmq3OCQfwumNCOERbJZVaZ4AibTmWYERmAoLZkyiYm2wj1ThwwqhSwBh959z3XKgu1Ic5_mKvdnJJkfZ5F42eRLAm_6ZeskAcm3rvU58eTsbzPKV7gbwsr-Nduw2Z2RpqgbbpAzHzwTHLh4vpd2_DkGyYGnGAuBretA3cBzh63fKydhzhQt3BJSFAbztNGb1Wf8fxe71o3gBd9H28q_Hw5OnsBU5RfbpjXuwOZ825hnCtLl67u2BwNlNG-BfYehLmA
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Star+finches+Neochmia+ruficauda+have+a+visual+preference+for+white+dot+patterns%3A+a+possible+case+of+trypophilia&rft.jtitle=Animal+cognition&rft.date=2022-10-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.issn=1435-9448&rft.eissn=1435-9456&rft.volume=25&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=1271&rft.epage=1279&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs10071-022-01609-5&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1435-9448&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1435-9448&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1435-9448&client=summon