Multiple memory systems as substrates for multiple decision systems

•We investigated the parallels between relational memory and decision strategies.•Model-based reinforcement learning correlated positively with acquired equivalence.•Model-free learning did not significantly relate to acquired equivalence. It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based d...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inNeurobiology of learning and memory Vol. 117; pp. 4 - 13
Main Authors Doll, Bradley B., Shohamy, Daphna, Daw, Nathaniel D.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Elsevier Inc 01.01.2015
Elsevier BV
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract •We investigated the parallels between relational memory and decision strategies.•Model-based reinforcement learning correlated positively with acquired equivalence.•Model-free learning did not significantly relate to acquired equivalence. It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human behavior in learning tasks appears to include habitual responses described by prominent model-free reinforcement learning (RL) theories, but also more deliberative or goal-directed actions that can be characterized by a different class of theories, model-based RL. The latter theories evaluate actions by using a representation of the contingencies of the task (as with a learned map of a spatial maze), called an “internal model.” Given the evidence of behavioral and neural dissociations between these approaches, they are often characterized as dissociable learning systems, though they likely interact and share common mechanisms. In many respects, this division parallels a longstanding dissociation in cognitive neuroscience between multiple memory systems, describing, at the broadest level, separate systems for declarative and procedural learning. Procedural learning has notable parallels with model-free RL: both involve learning of habits and both are known to depend on parts of the striatum. Declarative memory, by contrast, supports memory for single events or episodes and depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus is thought to support declarative memory by encoding temporal and spatial relations among stimuli and thus is often referred to as a relational memory system. Such relational encoding is likely to play an important role in learning an internal model, the representation that is central to model-based RL. Thus, insofar as the memory systems represent more general-purpose cognitive mechanisms that might subserve performance on many sorts of tasks including decision making, these parallels raise the question whether the multiple decision systems are served by multiple memory systems, such that one dissociation is grounded in the other. Here we investigated the relationship between model-based RL and relational memory by comparing individual differences across behavioral tasks designed to measure either capacity. Human subjects performed two tasks, a learning and generalization task (acquired equivalence) which involves relational encoding and depends on the hippocampus; and a sequential RL task that could be solved by either a model-based or model-free strategy. We assessed the correlation between subjects’ use of flexible, relational memory, as measured by generalization in the acquired equivalence task, and their differential reliance on either RL strategy in the decision task. We observed a significant positive relationship between generalization and model-based, but not model-free, choice strategies. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that model-based RL, like acquired equivalence, relies on a more general-purpose relational memory system.
AbstractList It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human behavior in learning tasks appears to include habitual responses described by prominent model-free reinforcement learning (RL) theories, but also more deliberative or goal-directed actions that can be characterized by a different class of theories, model-based RL. The latter theories evaluate actions by using a representation of the contingencies of the task (as with a learned map of a spatial maze), called an “internal model.” Given the evidence of behavioral and neural dissociations between these approaches, they are often characterized as dissociable learning systems, though they likely interact and share common mechanisms. In many respects, this division parallels a longstanding dissociation in cognitive neuroscience between multiple memory systems, describing, at the broadest level, separate systems for declarative and procedural learning. Procedural learning has notable parallels with model-free RL: both involve learning of habits and both are known to depend on parts of the striatum. Declarative memory, by contrast, supports memory for single events or episodes and depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus is thought to support declarative memory by encoding temporal and spatial relations among stimuli and thus is often referred to as a relational memory system. Such relational encoding is likely to play an important role in learning an internal model, the representation that is central to model-based RL. Thus, insofar as the memory systems represent more general-purpose cognitive mechanisms that might subserve performance on many sorts of tasks including decision making, these parallels raise the question whether the multiple decision systems are served by multiple memory systems, such that one dissociation is grounded in the other. Here we investigated the relationship between model-based RL and relational memory by comparing individual differences across behavioral tasks designed to measure either capacity. Human subjects performed two tasks, a learning and generalization task (acquired equivalence) which involves relational encoding and depends on the hippocampus; and a sequential RL task that could be solved by either a model-based or model-free strategy. We assessed the correlation between subjects’ use of flexible, relational memory, as measured by generalization in the acquired equivalence task, and their differential reliance on either RL strategy in the decision task. We observed a significant positive relationship between generalization and model-based, but not model-free, choice strategies. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that model-based RL, like acquired equivalence, relies on a more general-purpose relational memory system.
It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human behavior in learning tasks appears to include habitual responses described by prominent model-free reinforcement learning (RL) theories, but also more deliberative or goal-directed actions that can be characterized by a different class of theories, model-based RL. The latter theories evaluate actions by using a representation of the contingencies of the task (as with a learned map of a spatial maze), called an "internal model." Given the evidence of behavioral and neural dissociations between these approaches, they are often characterized as dissociable learning systems, though they likely interact and share common mechanisms. In many respects, this division parallels a longstanding dissociation in cognitive neuroscience between multiple memory systems, describing, at the broadest level, separate systems for declarative and procedural learning. Procedural learning has notable parallels with model-free RL: both involve learning of habits and both are known to depend on parts of the striatum. Declarative memory, by contrast, supports memory for single events or episodes and depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus is thought to support declarative memory by encoding temporal and spatial relations among stimuli and thus is often referred to as a relational memory system. Such relational encoding is likely to play an important role in learning an internal model, the representation that is central to model-based RL. Thus, insofar as the memory systems represent more general-purpose cognitive mechanisms that might subserve performance on many sorts of tasks including decision making, these parallels raise the question whether the multiple decision systems are served by multiple memory systems, such that one dissociation is grounded in the other. Here we investigated the relationship between model-based RL and relational memory by comparing individual differences across behavioral tasks designed to measure either capacity. Human subjects performed two tasks, a learning and generalization task (acquired equivalence) which involves relational encoding and depends on the hippocampus; and a sequential RL task that could be solved by either a model-based or model-free strategy. We assessed the correlation between subjects' use of flexible, relational memory, as measured by generalization in the acquired equivalence task, and their differential reliance on either RL strategy in the decision task. We observed a significant positive relationship between generalization and model-based, but not model-free, choice strategies. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that model-based RL, like acquired equivalence, relies on a more general-purpose relational memory system.It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human behavior in learning tasks appears to include habitual responses described by prominent model-free reinforcement learning (RL) theories, but also more deliberative or goal-directed actions that can be characterized by a different class of theories, model-based RL. The latter theories evaluate actions by using a representation of the contingencies of the task (as with a learned map of a spatial maze), called an "internal model." Given the evidence of behavioral and neural dissociations between these approaches, they are often characterized as dissociable learning systems, though they likely interact and share common mechanisms. In many respects, this division parallels a longstanding dissociation in cognitive neuroscience between multiple memory systems, describing, at the broadest level, separate systems for declarative and procedural learning. Procedural learning has notable parallels with model-free RL: both involve learning of habits and both are known to depend on parts of the striatum. Declarative memory, by contrast, supports memory for single events or episodes and depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus is thought to support declarative memory by encoding temporal and spatial relations among stimuli and thus is often referred to as a relational memory system. Such relational encoding is likely to play an important role in learning an internal model, the representation that is central to model-based RL. Thus, insofar as the memory systems represent more general-purpose cognitive mechanisms that might subserve performance on many sorts of tasks including decision making, these parallels raise the question whether the multiple decision systems are served by multiple memory systems, such that one dissociation is grounded in the other. Here we investigated the relationship between model-based RL and relational memory by comparing individual differences across behavioral tasks designed to measure either capacity. Human subjects performed two tasks, a learning and generalization task (acquired equivalence) which involves relational encoding and depends on the hippocampus; and a sequential RL task that could be solved by either a model-based or model-free strategy. We assessed the correlation between subjects' use of flexible, relational memory, as measured by generalization in the acquired equivalence task, and their differential reliance on either RL strategy in the decision task. We observed a significant positive relationship between generalization and model-based, but not model-free, choice strategies. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that model-based RL, like acquired equivalence, relies on a more general-purpose relational memory system.
It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human behavior in learning tasks appears to include habitual responses described by prominent model-free reinforcement learning (RL) theories, but also more deliberative or goal-directed actions that can be characterized by a different class of theories, model-based RL. The latter theories evaluate actions by using a representation of the contingencies of the task (as with a learned map of a spatial maze), called an "internal model." Given the evidence of behavioral and neural dissociations between these approaches, they are often characterized as dissociable learning systems, though they likely interact and share common mechanisms. In many respects, this division parallels a longstanding dissociation in cognitive neuroscience between multiple memory systems, describing, at the broadest level, separate systems for declarative and procedural learning. Procedural learning has notable parallels with model-free RL: both involve learning of habits and both are known to depend on parts of the striatum. Declarative memory, by contrast, supports memory for single events or episodes and depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus is thought to support declarative memory by encoding temporal and spatial relations among stimuli and thus is often referred to as a relational memory system. Such relational encoding is likely to play an important role in learning an internal model, the representation that is central to model-based RL. Thus, insofar as the memory systems represent more general-purpose cognitive mechanisms that might subserve performance on many sorts of tasks including decision making, these parallels raise the question whether the multiple decision systems are served by multiple memory systems, such that one dissociation is grounded in the other. Here we investigated the relationship between model-based RL and relational memory by comparing individual differences across behavioral tasks designed to measure either capacity. Human subjects performed two tasks, a learning and generalization task (acquired equivalence) which involves relational encoding and depends on the hippocampus; and a sequential RL task that could be solved by either a model-based or model-free strategy. We assessed the correlation between subjects' use of flexible, relational memory, as measured by generalization in the acquired equivalence task, and their differential reliance on either RL strategy in the decision task. We observed a significant positive relationship between generalization and model-based, but not model-free, choice strategies. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that model-based RL, like acquired equivalence, relies on a more general-purpose relational memory system.
•We investigated the parallels between relational memory and decision strategies.•Model-based reinforcement learning correlated positively with acquired equivalence.•Model-free learning did not significantly relate to acquired equivalence. It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human behavior in learning tasks appears to include habitual responses described by prominent model-free reinforcement learning (RL) theories, but also more deliberative or goal-directed actions that can be characterized by a different class of theories, model-based RL. The latter theories evaluate actions by using a representation of the contingencies of the task (as with a learned map of a spatial maze), called an “internal model.” Given the evidence of behavioral and neural dissociations between these approaches, they are often characterized as dissociable learning systems, though they likely interact and share common mechanisms. In many respects, this division parallels a longstanding dissociation in cognitive neuroscience between multiple memory systems, describing, at the broadest level, separate systems for declarative and procedural learning. Procedural learning has notable parallels with model-free RL: both involve learning of habits and both are known to depend on parts of the striatum. Declarative memory, by contrast, supports memory for single events or episodes and depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus is thought to support declarative memory by encoding temporal and spatial relations among stimuli and thus is often referred to as a relational memory system. Such relational encoding is likely to play an important role in learning an internal model, the representation that is central to model-based RL. Thus, insofar as the memory systems represent more general-purpose cognitive mechanisms that might subserve performance on many sorts of tasks including decision making, these parallels raise the question whether the multiple decision systems are served by multiple memory systems, such that one dissociation is grounded in the other. Here we investigated the relationship between model-based RL and relational memory by comparing individual differences across behavioral tasks designed to measure either capacity. Human subjects performed two tasks, a learning and generalization task (acquired equivalence) which involves relational encoding and depends on the hippocampus; and a sequential RL task that could be solved by either a model-based or model-free strategy. We assessed the correlation between subjects’ use of flexible, relational memory, as measured by generalization in the acquired equivalence task, and their differential reliance on either RL strategy in the decision task. We observed a significant positive relationship between generalization and model-based, but not model-free, choice strategies. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that model-based RL, like acquired equivalence, relies on a more general-purpose relational memory system.
* We investigated the parallels between relational memory and decision strategies. * Model-based reinforcement learning correlated positively with acquired equivalence. * Model-free learning did not significantly relate to acquired equivalence. It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human behavior in learning tasks appears to include habitual responses described by prominent model-free reinforcement learning (RL) theories, but also more deliberative or goal-directed actions that can be characterized by a different class of theories, model-based RL. The latter theories evaluate actions by using a representation of the contingencies of the task (as with a learned map of a spatial maze), called an "internal model.â[euro] Given the evidence of behavioral and neural dissociations between these approaches, they are often characterized as dissociable learning systems, though they likely interact and share common mechanisms. In many respects, this division parallels a longstanding dissociation in cognitive neuroscience between multiple memory systems, describing, at the broadest level, separate systems for declarative and procedural learning. Procedural learning has notable parallels with model-free RL: both involve learning of habits and both are known to depend on parts of the striatum. Declarative memory, by contrast, supports memory for single events or episodes and depends on the hippocampus. The hippocampus is thought to support declarative memory by encoding temporal and spatial relations among stimuli and thus is often referred to as a relational memory system. Such relational encoding is likely to play an important role in learning an internal model, the representation that is central to model-based RL. Thus, insofar as the memory systems represent more general-purpose cognitive mechanisms that might subserve performance on many sorts of tasks including decision making, these parallels raise the question whether the multiple decision systems are served by multiple memory systems, such that one dissociation is grounded in the other. Here we investigated the relationship between model-based RL and relational memory by comparing individual differences across behavioral tasks designed to measure either capacity. Human subjects performed two tasks, a learning and generalization task (acquired equivalence) which involves relational encoding and depends on the hippocampus; and a sequential RL task that could be solved by either a model-based or model-free strategy. We assessed the correlation between subjects' use of flexible, relational memory, as measured by generalization in the acquired equivalence task, and their differential reliance on either RL strategy in the decision task. We observed a significant positive relationship between generalization and model-based, but not model-free, choice strategies. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that model-based RL, like acquired equivalence, relies on a more general-purpose relational memory system.
Author Shohamy, Daphna
Doll, Bradley B.
Daw, Nathaniel D.
AuthorAffiliation c Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York
a Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York
b Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, New York
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: c Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, New York
– name: b Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, New York
– name: a Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Bradley B.
  surname: Doll
  fullname: Doll, Bradley B.
  email: bradley.doll@nyu.edu
  organization: Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, United States
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Daphna
  surname: Shohamy
  fullname: Shohamy, Daphna
  organization: Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Nathaniel D.
  surname: Daw
  fullname: Daw, Nathaniel D.
  organization: Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, United States
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24846190$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqNkVtr3DAQhUVJaS7tD-hLMfSlL95oZF0pFMrSG6TkJX0WsjxutdjWVrID---r7WZDm4cQGJBA35k5o3NOTqY4ISGvga6AgrzcrKZhXDEKfEVLAX9GzoAaURsh-cn-rnitOFOn5DznDaUAwugX5JRxzSUYekbW35dhDtsBqxHHmHZV3uUZx1y5XOWlzXNyM-aqj6kaj2SHPuQQpyP7kjzv3ZDx1d15QX58_nSz_lpfXX_5tv54VXuh2VyD4EoxaRrPWQOu62kvO9MZxbz0vjVUOd0i6zWnlFPXaq5ASAHgQXIhdHNBPhz6bpd2xM7jVNwNdpvC6NLORhfs_y9T-GV_xltb5jUUVGnw7q5Bir8XzLMdQ_Y4DG7CuGQLUnIGVBr6BJQL1jBtREHfPkA3cUlT-Ym_VFkbNBTqzb_m710foygAHACfYs4J-3sEqN3HbTe2xG33cVtaCnjRqAcaH2Y3l2zK_mF4VPn-oMQS2G3AZLMPOHnsQkI_2y6GR9R_AKisw3w
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1177_10888683221114215
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psc_2018_10_008
crossref_primary_10_1038_nrn_2016_56
crossref_primary_10_1002_acn3_51185
crossref_primary_10_1002_da_22857
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41539_024_00243_9
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_94530_9
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01447
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_25988
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12559_015_9340_1
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41583_020_0355_6
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01952_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2020_12_027
crossref_primary_10_1097_WCO_0000000000000218
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2022_105103
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pbio_3002934
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1004523
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nlm_2019_01_004
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_0763_16_2016
crossref_primary_10_1038_ncomms12438
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_020_15696_w
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_04919
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2017_08_009
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep43119
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1007443
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_573730
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep33124
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_00312
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2084_17_2018
crossref_primary_10_1186_s12993_023_00207_w
crossref_primary_10_3389_fcogn_2023_1326191
crossref_primary_10_1177_1073858414568317
crossref_primary_10_1038_nn_3981
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_0873_20_2020
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1901_15_2016
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11910_021_01155_6
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2019_00915
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3049_17_2018
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_019_00700_8
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2018_01_006
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_0185_14_2014
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_19804_2
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2104_19_2019
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_018_28176_5
crossref_primary_10_1111_eth_12655
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_017_0180_8
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_02004_w
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_12996
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2018_03_048
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2016_07_034
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_020_0905_y
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41583_024_00807_z
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2023_105385
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_023_00472_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2018_04_023
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2021_101378
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2021_118780
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nlm_2014_08_014
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_683885
Cites_doi 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-02-00582.1989
10.1093/brain/awh100
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04233.2000
10.1038/nature12112
10.1007/BF00993104
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07920.x
10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.016
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00525
10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.899
10.1073/pnas.1014269108
10.1073/pnas.1312011110
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04218.x
10.1901/jeab.2009.91-127
10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.87
10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.011
10.1162/089892903321208123
10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.023
10.1177/0956797612463080
10.1001/archneur.1984.04050200071020
10.1126/science.7414331
10.1016/0028-3932(68)90024-9
10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1156
10.1080/14640748208400878
10.1162/neco.1993.5.4.613
10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.030
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4647-10.2011
10.1126/science.273.5280.1399
10.1073/pnas.0610561104
10.1038/nn1560
10.1006/jmps.1998.1217
10.1037/h0061626
10.1016/S0893-6080(02)00045-X
10.1002/hipo.20009
10.1038/379255a0
10.1037/0735-7036.106.4.342
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-05-01936.1996
10.1126/science.1168878
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2806-12.2012
10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9
10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.195
10.1038/nn1279
10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027
10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10976.2002
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00070
10.1162/jocn.2009.21131
10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.024
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x
10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003387
10.1101/lm.1.2.106
10.1016/j.conb.2012.08.003
10.1017/S0140525X00063949
10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.023
10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.468
10.1073/pnas.0602659103
10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.042
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3761-07.2007
10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08017.x
10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.056
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06796.x
10.1126/science.1223252
10.1521/soco.2008.26.5.593
10.1038/nn.3413
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
10.1214/ss/1177011136
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2014 Elsevier Inc.
Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 2014
Copyright_xml – notice: 2014 Elsevier Inc.
– notice: Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
– notice: 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 2014
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QR
7TK
8FD
FR3
P64
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Engineering Research Database
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Chemoreception Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Technology Research Database
Neurosciences Abstracts
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList
MEDLINE - Academic
MEDLINE

Neurosciences Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Biology
Anatomy & Physiology
Zoology
Psychology
EISSN 1095-9564
EndPage 13
ExternalDocumentID PMC4233017
3556813771
24846190
10_1016_j_nlm_2014_04_014
S1074742714000823
Genre Journal Article
Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NINDS NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 NS078784
– fundername: NINDS NIH HHS
  grantid: R01NS078784-01
GroupedDBID ---
--K
--M
-DZ
-~X
.GJ
.~1
0R~
123
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29N
4.4
457
4G.
53G
5RE
5VS
7-5
71M
8P~
AACTN
AADFP
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAGJA
AAGUQ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABOYX
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACIWK
ACPRK
ACRLP
ACXNI
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADFGL
ADMUD
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AFDAS
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AFYLN
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
C45
CAG
COF
CS3
DM4
DU5
EBS
EFBJH
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-Q
GBLVA
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
IHE
J1W
KOM
LG5
M41
MO0
MOBAO
N9A
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OKEIE
OVD
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
PQQKQ
Q38
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SPCBC
SSB
SSN
SSY
SSZ
T5K
TEORI
TN5
UNMZH
WH7
XOL
XPP
XSW
YQT
ZGI
ZMT
ZU3
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADNMO
ADVLN
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
SSH
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
PKN
7QG
7QR
7TK
8FD
EFKBS
FR3
P64
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c582t-154772693c4231adf0f6d9d972c6ccb907a8be2f840040ab847156511c1645583
IEDL.DBID .~1
ISSN 1074-7427
1095-9564
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:04:21 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 15:32:53 EDT 2025
Tue Aug 05 10:23:38 EDT 2025
Wed Aug 13 06:04:23 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 01:51:55 EST 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:15:11 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:01:00 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:22:13 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Keywords Relational memory
Reinforcement learning
Language English
License Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c582t-154772693c4231adf0f6d9d972c6ccb907a8be2f840040ab847156511c1645583
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/4233017
PMID 24846190
PQID 1645154181
PQPubID 32945
PageCount 10
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4233017
proquest_miscellaneous_1664210690
proquest_miscellaneous_1645232895
proquest_journals_1645154181
pubmed_primary_24846190
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nlm_2014_04_014
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_nlm_2014_04_014
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_nlm_2014_04_014
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2015-01-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2015-01-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 01
  year: 2015
  text: 2015-01-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: New York
PublicationTitle Neurobiology of learning and memory
PublicationTitleAlternate Neurobiol Learn Mem
PublicationYear 2015
Publisher Elsevier Inc
Elsevier BV
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Inc
– name: Elsevier BV
References Meeter, Shohamy, Myers (b0220) 2009; 91
Yin, Ostlund, Knowlton, Balleine (b0390) 2005; 22
Sutton (b0325) 1990
Squire (b0305) 1992; 99
Daw, Shohamy (b0085) 2008; 26
Pfeiffer, Foster (b0260) 2013; 497
Greene, Gross, Elsinger, Rao (b0165) 2006; 18
Zeithamova, Schlichting, Preston (b0395) 2012; 6
Dayan (b0090) 1993; 5
Simon, Daw (b0295) 2011; 31
Johnson, Redish (b0190) 2007; 27
Shohamy, Myers, Grossman, Sage, Gluck, Poldrack (b0285) 2004; 127
Daw (b0070) 2011
Tricomi, Balleine, O’Doherty (b0350) 2009; 29
Steinberg, Keiflin, Boivin, Witten, Deisseroth, Janak (b0320) 2013; 16
Doll, Simon, Daw (b0110) 2012; 22
Bornstein, Daw (b0015) 2013; 9
Eichenbaum, Yonelinas, Ranganath (b0115) 2007; 30
Tanaka, Doya, Okada, Ueda, Okamoto, Yamawaki (b0335) 2004; 7
Bunsey, Eichenbaum (b0025) 1996; 379
Adams (b0005) 1982; 34
Tsai, Zhang, Adamantidis, Stuber, Bonci, de Lecea (b0355) 2009; 324
Wunderlich, Smittenaar, Dolan (b0380) 2012; 75
Burgess, Maguire, O’Keefe (b0030) 2002; 35
Addis, Wong, Schacter (b0010) 2007; 45
Dickinson, Smith, Mirenowicz (b0105) 2000; 114
Daw, Gershman, Seymour, Dayan, Dolan (b0075) 2011; 69
Honey, Hall (b0185) 1989; 15
Staresina, Davachi (b0315) 2009; 63
Lee, Ghim, Kim, Lee, Jung (b0210) 2012; 32
Daw, Niv, Dayan (b0080) 2005; 8
Knowlton, Squire, Gluck (b0200) 1994; 1
Myers, Shohamy, Gluck, Grossman, Kluger, Ferris (b0235) 2003; 15
Otto, Raio, Chiang, Phelps, Daw (b0255) 2013; 110
Camerer (b0035) 1998; 42
Martone, Butters, Payne, Becker, Sax (b0215) 1984; 41
Corbit, Ostlund, Balleine (b0055) 2002; 22
Gershman, Markman, Otto (b0150) 2012
Glimcher (b0160) 2011; 108
Sutton, Barto (b0330) 1998
Stan Development Team. Stan: A C++ Library for Probability and Sampling, Version 1.3.
Den Ouden, Daw, Fernandez, Elshout, Rijpkema, Hoogman, Franke, Cools (b0095) 2013; 80
Gelman, Carlin, Stern, Rubin (b0135) 2003
Corkin (b0060) 1968; 6
Kruschke (b0205) 2010
Moore, Atkeson (b0230) 1993; 13
Shohamy, Wagner (b0290) 2008; 60
Schapiro, Kustner, Turk-Browne (b0280) 2012; 22
Turk-Browne, Scholl, Chun, Johnson (b0360) 2009; 21
Wimmer, Daw, Shohamy (b0370) 2012; 35
Cohen, Squire (b0045) 1980; 210
Montague, Dayan, Sejnowski (b0225) 1996; 16
Reynolds, Wickens (b0270) 2002; 15
Davis (b0065) 1992; 106
Corbit, Balleine (b0050) 2000; 20
Tolman, Honzik (b0345) 1930; 4
Rummery, Niranjan (b0275) 1994
Yin, Knowlton, Balleine (b0385) 2004; 19
Knowlton, Mangels, Squire (b0195) 1996; 273
Wimmer, Shohamy (b0375) 2012; 338
Hassabis, Kumaran, Vann, Maguire (b0170) 2007; 104
Otto, Gershman, Markman, Daw (b0250) 2013; 24
Preston, Shrager, Dudukovic, Gabrieli (b0265) 2004; 14
Gelman, Rubin (b0145) 1992; 7
Hikida, Kimura, Wada, Funabiki, Nakanishi (b0180) 2010; 66
O’Keefe, Nadel (b0245) 1979; 2
Dickinson (b0100) 1980
Cohen, Eichenbaum (b0040) 1993
Glascher, Daw, Dayan, O’Doherty (b0155) 2010; 66
Tolman (b0340) 1948; 55
Heindel, Salmon, Shults, Walicke, Butters (b0175) 1989; 9
Nissen, Bullemer (b0240) 1987; 19
Gabrieli (b0125) 1998; 49
Gelman, Hill (b0140) 2007
van der Meer, Johnson, Schmitzer-Torbert, Redish (b0365) 2010; 67
Bornstein, Daw (b0020) 2012; 35
Foerde, Knowlton, Poldrack (b0120) 2006; 103
Gabrieli, Corkin, Mickel, Growdon (b0130) 1993; 107
Skatova, Chan, Daw (b0300) 2013; 7
Gelman (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0140) 2007
Bornstein (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0020) 2012; 35
Sutton (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0330) 1998
Corkin (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0060) 1968; 6
Martone (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0215) 1984; 41
Doll (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0110) 2012; 22
Glascher (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0155) 2010; 66
Shohamy (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0290) 2008; 60
Staresina (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0315) 2009; 63
Turk-Browne (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0360) 2009; 21
Bornstein (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0015) 2013; 9
Tolman (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0340) 1948; 55
Johnson (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0190) 2007; 27
Rummery (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0275) 1994
Corbit (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0050) 2000; 20
Bunsey (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0025) 1996; 379
Knowlton (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0200) 1994; 1
Meeter (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0220) 2009; 91
Daw (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0070) 2011
Eichenbaum (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0115) 2007; 30
Gabrieli (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0130) 1993; 107
Lee (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0210) 2012; 32
Daw (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0085) 2008; 26
O’Keefe (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0245) 1979; 2
Zeithamova (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0395) 2012; 6
Addis (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0010) 2007; 45
Wimmer (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0370) 2012; 35
Nissen (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0240) 1987; 19
Yin (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0385) 2004; 19
Corbit (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0055) 2002; 22
Knowlton (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0195) 1996; 273
Wimmer (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0375) 2012; 338
Wunderlich (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0380) 2012; 75
Yin (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0390) 2005; 22
Glimcher (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0160) 2011; 108
van der Meer (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0365) 2010; 67
Dickinson (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0100) 1980
Tricomi (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0350) 2009; 29
Dickinson (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0105) 2000; 114
10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0310
Steinberg (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0320) 2013; 16
Gershman (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0150) 2012
Daw (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0075) 2011; 69
Sutton (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0325) 1990
Cohen (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0040) 1993
Adams (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0005) 1982; 34
Otto (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0250) 2013; 24
Preston (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0265) 2004; 14
Montague (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0225) 1996; 16
Moore (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0230) 1993; 13
Hikida (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0180) 2010; 66
Honey (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0185) 1989; 15
Reynolds (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0270) 2002; 15
Greene (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0165) 2006; 18
Hassabis (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0170) 2007; 104
Daw (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0080) 2005; 8
Pfeiffer (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0260) 2013; 497
Schapiro (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0280) 2012; 22
Shohamy (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0285) 2004; 127
Davis (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0065) 1992; 106
Otto (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0255) 2013; 110
Tanaka (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0335) 2004; 7
Foerde (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0120) 2006; 103
Burgess (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0030) 2002; 35
Skatova (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0300) 2013; 7
Tsai (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0355) 2009; 324
Kruschke (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0205) 2010
Gelman (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0145) 1992; 7
Myers (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0235) 2003; 15
Cohen (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0045) 1980; 210
Den Ouden (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0095) 2013; 80
Squire (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0305) 1992; 99
Gabrieli (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0125) 1998; 49
Simon (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0295) 2011; 31
Camerer (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0035) 1998; 42
Gelman (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0135) 2003
Tolman (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0345) 1930; 4
Dayan (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0090) 1993; 5
Heindel (10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0175) 1989; 9
8703077 - Science. 1996 Sep 6;273(5280):1399-402
1594723 - Psychol Rev. 1992 Apr;99(2):195-231
2521896 - J Neurosci. 1989 Feb;9(2):582-7
10883798 - Behav Neurosci. 2000 Jun;114(3):468-83
23100426 - J Neurosci. 2012 Oct 24;32(43):15053-65
22884326 - Neuron. 2012 Aug 9;75(3):418-24
24324166 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013 Dec 24;110(52):20941-6
21389268 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Sep 13;108 Suppl 3:15647-54
17989284 - J Neurosci. 2007 Nov 7;27(45):12176-89
18870876 - Psychol Rev. 1948 Jul;55(4):189-208
9710553 - J Math Psychol. 1998 Jun;42(2/3):305-26
24339770 - PLoS Comput Biol. 2013;9(12):e1003387
24267657 - Neuron. 2013 Nov 20;80(4):1090-100
23066083 - Science. 2012 Oct 12;338(6104):270-3
19640484 - Neuron. 2009 Jul 30;63(2):267-76
15098720 - Hippocampus. 2004;14(2):148-52
15235607 - Nat Neurosci. 2004 Aug;7(8):887-93
12486193 - J Neurosci. 2002 Dec 15;22(24):10976-84
14750976 - Eur J Neurosci. 2004 Jan;19(1):181-9
16868087 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006 Aug 1;103(31):11778-83
21471389 - J Neurosci. 2011 Apr 6;31(14):5526-39
22959354 - Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2012 Dec;22(6):1075-81
12676056 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2003 Feb 15;15(2):185-93
22470333 - Front Hum Neurosci. 2012 Mar 26;6:70
23594744 - Nature. 2013 May 2;497(7447):74-9
22487039 - Eur J Neurosci. 2012 Apr;35(7):1092-104
22487032 - Eur J Neurosci. 2012 Apr;35(7):1011-23
8136066 - Behav Neurosci. 1993 Dec;107(6):899-910
19230516 - J Exp Anal Behav. 2009 Jan;91(1):127-41
8774460 - J Neurosci. 1996 Mar 1;16(5):1936-47
17126370 - Neuropsychologia. 2007 Apr 8;45(7):1363-77
20624589 - Neuron. 2010 Jul 15;67(1):25-32
19490086 - Eur J Neurosci. 2009 Jun;29(11):2225-32
15013954 - Brain. 2004 Apr;127(Pt 4):851-9
20510862 - Neuron. 2010 May 27;66(4):585-95
21435563 - Neuron. 2011 Mar 24;69(6):1204-15
16839289 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2006 Jul;18(7):1156-73
16286932 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Dec;8(12):1704-11
8538790 - Nature. 1996 Jan 18;379(6562):255-7
18957228 - Neuron. 2008 Oct 23;60(2):378-89
7414331 - Science. 1980 Oct 10;210(4466):207-10
1451416 - J Comp Psychol. 1992 Dec;106(4):342-9
2794870 - J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1989 Oct;15(4):338-46
6236779 - Arch Neurol. 1984 Sep;41(9):965-70
9496622 - Annu Rev Psychol. 1998;49:87-115
22885059 - Curr Biol. 2012 Sep 11;22(17):1622-7
12371508 - Neural Netw. 2002 Jun-Jul;15(4-6):507-21
10467589 - Learn Mem. 1994 Jul-Aug;1(2):106-20
16045504 - Eur J Neurosci. 2005 Jul;22(2):513-23
23558545 - Psychol Sci. 2013 May;24(5):751-61
17417939 - Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:123-52
19389999 - Science. 2009 May 22;324(5930):1080-4
23708143 - Nat Neurosci. 2013 Jul;16(7):966-73
10818159 - J Neurosci. 2000 Jun 1;20(11):4233-9
17229836 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007 Jan 30;104(5):1726-31
23230992 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014 Feb;143(1):182-94
20620875 - Neuron. 2010 Jun 24;66(6):896-907
18823241 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2009 Oct;21(10):1934-45
24027514 - Front Hum Neurosci. 2013 Sep 03;7:525
12194864 - Neuron. 2002 Aug 15;35(4):625-41
References_xml – volume: 60
  start-page: 378
  year: 2008
  end-page: 389
  ident: b0290
  article-title: Integrating memories in the human brain: Hippocampal–midbrain encoding of overlapping events
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 4
  start-page: 257
  year: 1930
  end-page: 275
  ident: b0345
  article-title: Introduction and removal of reward, and maze performance in rats
  publication-title: University of California Publications in Psychology
– volume: 1
  start-page: 106
  year: 1994
  end-page: 120
  ident: b0200
  article-title: Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia
  publication-title: Learning & Memory
– volume: 7
  start-page: 887
  year: 2004
  end-page: 893
  ident: b0335
  article-title: Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
– volume: 9
  year: 2013
  ident: b0015
  article-title: Cortical and hippocampal correlates of deliberation during model-based decisions for rewards in humans
  publication-title: PLoS Computational Biology.
– volume: 379
  start-page: 255
  year: 1996
  end-page: 257
  ident: b0025
  article-title: Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans
  publication-title: Nature
– volume: 22
  start-page: 10976
  year: 2002
  end-page: 10984
  ident: b0055
  article-title: Sensitivity to instrumental contingency degradation is mediated by the entorhinal cortex and its efferents via the dorsal hippocampus
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 6
  start-page: 225
  year: 1968
  end-page: 264
  ident: b0060
  article-title: Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 114
  start-page: 468
  year: 2000
  end-page: 483
  ident: b0105
  article-title: Dissociation of pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists
  publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience
– year: 2010
  ident: b0205
  article-title: Doing Bayesian data analysis, a tutorial introduction with R
– volume: 103
  start-page: 11778
  year: 2006
  end-page: 11783
  ident: b0120
  article-title: Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA
– volume: 26
  start-page: 593
  year: 2008
  end-page: 620
  ident: b0085
  article-title: The cognitive neuroscience of motivation and learning
  publication-title: Social Cognition
– volume: 21
  start-page: 1934
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1945
  ident: b0360
  article-title: Neural evidence of statistical learning: Efficient detection of visual regularities without awareness
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– year: 1980
  ident: b0100
  article-title: Contemporary animal learning theory
– volume: 19
  start-page: 181
  year: 2004
  end-page: 189
  ident: b0385
  article-title: Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– start-page: 3
  year: 2011
  end-page: 38
  ident: b0070
  article-title: Trial-by-trial data analysis using computational models
  publication-title: Decision making, affect, and learning: Attention and performance XXIII
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1622
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1627
  ident: b0280
  article-title: Shaping of object representations in the human medial temporal lobe based on temporal regularities
  publication-title: Current Biology
– year: 1993
  ident: b0040
  article-title: Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system
– volume: 99
  start-page: 195
  year: 1992
  end-page: 231
  ident: b0305
  article-title: Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 6
  start-page: 70
  year: 2012
  ident: b0395
  article-title: The hippocampus and inferential reasoning: Building memories to navigate future decisions
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– year: 2007
  ident: b0140
  article-title: Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models
– volume: 14
  start-page: 148
  year: 2004
  end-page: 152
  ident: b0265
  article-title: Hippocampal contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory
  publication-title: Hippocampus
– volume: 127
  start-page: 851
  year: 2004
  end-page: 859
  ident: b0285
  article-title: Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: Converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology
  publication-title: Brain
– volume: 35
  start-page: 625
  year: 2002
  end-page: 641
  ident: b0030
  article-title: The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 41
  start-page: 965
  year: 1984
  end-page: 970
  ident: b0215
  article-title: Dissociations between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia
  publication-title: Archives of Neurology
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1204
  year: 2011
  end-page: 1215
  ident: b0075
  article-title: Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 80
  start-page: 1090
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1100
  ident: b0095
  article-title: Dissociable effects of dopamine and serotonin on reversal learning
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 15
  start-page: 338
  year: 1989
  end-page: 346
  ident: b0185
  article-title: Acquired equivalence and distinctiveness of cues
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes
– volume: 42
  start-page: 305
  year: 1998
  end-page: 326
  ident: b0035
  article-title: Experience-weighted attraction learning in coordination games: Probability rules, heterogeneity, and time-variation
  publication-title: Journal of Mathematical Psychology
– volume: 31
  start-page: 5526
  year: 2011
  end-page: 5539
  ident: b0295
  article-title: Neural correlates of forward planning in a spatial decision task in humans
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 16
  start-page: 966
  year: 2013
  end-page: 973
  ident: b0320
  article-title: A causal link between prediction errors, dopamine neurons and learning
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
– year: 1994
  ident: b0275
  article-title: On-line Q-learning using connectionist systems. CUED/F-INFENG/TR 166
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1
  year: 1987
  end-page: 32
  ident: b0240
  article-title: Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 497
  start-page: 74
  year: 2013
  end-page: 79
  ident: b0260
  article-title: Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals
  publication-title: Nature
– volume: 15
  start-page: 185
  year: 2003
  end-page: 193
  ident: b0235
  article-title: Dissociating hippocampal versus basal ganglia contributions to learning and transfer
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 29
  start-page: 2225
  year: 2009
  end-page: 2232
  ident: b0350
  article-title: A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 7
  start-page: 457
  year: 1992
  end-page: 511
  ident: b0145
  article-title: Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences
  publication-title: Statistical Science
– volume: 16
  start-page: 1936
  year: 1996
  end-page: 1947
  ident: b0225
  article-title: A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 324
  start-page: 1080
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1084
  ident: b0355
  article-title: Phasic firing in dopaminergic neurons is sufficient for behavioral conditioning
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 2
  start-page: 487
  year: 1979
  end-page: 533
  ident: b0245
  article-title: The hippocampus as a cognitive map
  publication-title: Behavioral and Brain Sciences
– year: 1998
  ident: b0330
  article-title: Reinforcement learning: An introduction
– volume: 91
  start-page: 127
  year: 2009
  end-page: 141
  ident: b0220
  article-title: Acquired equivalence changes stimulus representations
  publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1363
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1377
  ident: b0010
  article-title: Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 63
  start-page: 267
  year: 2009
  end-page: 276
  ident: b0315
  article-title: Mind the gap: Binding experiences across space and time in the human hippocampus
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 338
  start-page: 270
  year: 2012
  end-page: 273
  ident: b0375
  article-title: Preference by association: How memory mechanisms in the hippocampus bias decisions
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 5
  start-page: 613
  year: 1993
  end-page: 624
  ident: b0090
  article-title: Improving generalization for temporal difference learning: The successor representation
  publication-title: Neural Computation
– volume: 67
  start-page: 25
  year: 2010
  end-page: 32
  ident: b0365
  article-title: Triple dissociation of information processing in dorsal striatum, ventral striatum, and hippocampus on a learned spatial decision task
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 8
  start-page: 1704
  year: 2005
  end-page: 1711
  ident: b0080
  article-title: Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
– start-page: 216
  year: 1990
  end-page: 224
  ident: b0325
  article-title: Integrated architectures for learning, planning, and reacting based on approximating dynamic programming
  publication-title: Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Machine Learning, GTE Laboratories Incorporated
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1156
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1173
  ident: b0165
  article-title: An FMRI analysis of the human hippocampus: Inference, context, and task awareness
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 108
  start-page: 15647
  year: 2011
  end-page: 15654
  ident: b0160
  article-title: Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: The dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science
– volume: 13
  start-page: 103
  year: 1993
  end-page: 130
  ident: b0230
  article-title: Prioritized sweeping: Reinforcement learning with less data and less time
  publication-title: Machine Learning
– volume: 66
  start-page: 585
  year: 2010
  end-page: 595
  ident: b0155
  article-title: States versus rewards: Dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 55
  start-page: 189
  year: 1948
  end-page: 208
  ident: b0340
  article-title: Cognitive maps in rats and men
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 27
  start-page: 12176
  year: 2007
  end-page: 12189
  ident: b0190
  article-title: Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 15
  start-page: 507
  year: 2002
  end-page: 521
  ident: b0270
  article-title: Dopamine-dependent plasticity of corticostriatal synapses
  publication-title: Neural Networks
– volume: 22
  start-page: 513
  year: 2005
  end-page: 523
  ident: b0390
  article-title: The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1011
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1023
  ident: b0020
  article-title: Dissociating hippocampal and striatal contributions to sequential prediction learning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– year: 2003
  ident: b0135
  article-title: Bayesian data analysis
– volume: 273
  start-page: 1399
  year: 1996
  end-page: 1402
  ident: b0195
  article-title: A neostriatal habit learning system in humans
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 110
  start-page: 20941
  year: 2013
  end-page: 20946
  ident: b0255
  article-title: Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA
– reference: Stan Development Team. Stan: A C++ Library for Probability and Sampling, Version 1.3.
– volume: 32
  start-page: 15053
  year: 2012
  end-page: 15065
  ident: b0210
  article-title: Hippocampal neural correlates for values of experienced events
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 20
  start-page: 4233
  year: 2000
  end-page: 4239
  ident: b0050
  article-title: The role of the hippocampus in instrumental conditioning
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 75
  start-page: 418
  year: 2012
  end-page: 424
  ident: b0380
  article-title: Dopamine enhances model-based over model-free choice behavior
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 49
  start-page: 87
  year: 1998
  end-page: 115
  ident: b0125
  article-title: Cognitive neuroscience of human memory
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
– volume: 210
  start-page: 207
  year: 1980
  end-page: 210
  ident: b0045
  article-title: Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 34
  start-page: 77
  year: 1982
  end-page: 98
  ident: b0005
  article-title: Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation
  publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B
– volume: 106
  start-page: 342
  year: 1992
  end-page: 349
  ident: b0065
  article-title: Transitive inference in rats (rattus norvegicus)
  publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology
– volume: 107
  start-page: 899
  year: 1993
  end-page: 910
  ident: b0130
  article-title: Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer’s disease and in global amnesia
  publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience
– volume: 30
  start-page: 123
  year: 2007
  end-page: 152
  ident: b0115
  article-title: The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory
  publication-title: Annual review of Neuroscience
– volume: 9
  start-page: 582
  year: 1989
  end-page: 587
  ident: b0175
  article-title: Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: A comparison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1092
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1104
  ident: b0370
  article-title: Generalization of value in reinforcement learning by humans
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1075
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1081
  ident: b0110
  article-title: The ubiquity of model-based reinforcement learning
  publication-title: Current Opinion in Neurobiology
– volume: 24
  start-page: 751
  year: 2013
  end-page: 761
  ident: b0250
  article-title: The curse of planning: Dissecting multiple reinforcement-learning systems by taxing the central executive
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 104
  start-page: 1726
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1731
  ident: b0170
  article-title: Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA
– volume: 66
  start-page: 896
  year: 2010
  end-page: 907
  ident: b0180
  article-title: Distinct roles of synaptic transmission in direct and indirect striatal pathways to reward and aversive behavior
  publication-title: Neuron
– year: 2012
  ident: b0150
  article-title: Retrospective revaluation in sequential decision making: A tale of two systems
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 7
  start-page: 525
  year: 2013
  ident: b0300
  article-title: Extraversion differentiates between model-based and model-free strategies in a reinforcement learning task
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– volume: 9
  start-page: 582
  year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0175
  article-title: Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: A comparison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-02-00582.1989
– volume: 127
  start-page: 851
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0285
  article-title: Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based learning: Converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology
  publication-title: Brain
  doi: 10.1093/brain/awh100
– volume: 20
  start-page: 4233
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0050
  article-title: The role of the hippocampus in instrumental conditioning
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04233.2000
– year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0140
– volume: 497
  start-page: 74
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0260
  article-title: Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/nature12112
– volume: 13
  start-page: 103
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0230
  article-title: Prioritized sweeping: Reinforcement learning with less data and less time
  publication-title: Machine Learning
  doi: 10.1007/BF00993104
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1011
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0020
  article-title: Dissociating hippocampal and striatal contributions to sequential prediction learning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07920.x
– year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0135
– volume: 66
  start-page: 585
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0155
  article-title: States versus rewards: Dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.016
– volume: 7
  start-page: 525
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0300
  article-title: Extraversion differentiates between model-based and model-free strategies in a reinforcement learning task
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00525
– volume: 107
  start-page: 899
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0130
  article-title: Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer’s disease and in global amnesia
  publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.899
– volume: 108
  start-page: 15647
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0160
  article-title: Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: The dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014269108
– volume: 110
  start-page: 20941
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0255
  article-title: Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312011110
– start-page: 216
  year: 1990
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0325
  article-title: Integrated architectures for learning, planning, and reacting based on approximating dynamic programming
– volume: 22
  start-page: 513
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0390
  article-title: The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04218.x
– volume: 91
  start-page: 127
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0220
  article-title: Acquired equivalence changes stimulus representations
  publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
  doi: 10.1901/jeab.2009.91-127
– volume: 49
  start-page: 87
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0125
  article-title: Cognitive neuroscience of human memory
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.87
– volume: 66
  start-page: 896
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0180
  article-title: Distinct roles of synaptic transmission in direct and indirect striatal pathways to reward and aversive behavior
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.011
– volume: 15
  start-page: 185
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0235
  article-title: Dissociating hippocampal versus basal ganglia contributions to learning and transfer
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/089892903321208123
– volume: 67
  start-page: 25
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0365
  article-title: Triple dissociation of information processing in dorsal striatum, ventral striatum, and hippocampus on a learned spatial decision task
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.023
– volume: 24
  start-page: 751
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0250
  article-title: The curse of planning: Dissecting multiple reinforcement-learning systems by taxing the central executive
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797612463080
– volume: 41
  start-page: 965
  year: 1984
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0215
  article-title: Dissociations between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia
  publication-title: Archives of Neurology
  doi: 10.1001/archneur.1984.04050200071020
– volume: 210
  start-page: 207
  year: 1980
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0045
  article-title: Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.7414331
– volume: 6
  start-page: 225
  year: 1968
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0060
  article-title: Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(68)90024-9
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1156
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0165
  article-title: An FMRI analysis of the human hippocampus: Inference, context, and task awareness
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1156
– volume: 34
  start-page: 77
  year: 1982
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0005
  article-title: Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation
  publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B
  doi: 10.1080/14640748208400878
– volume: 5
  start-page: 613
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0090
  article-title: Improving generalization for temporal difference learning: The successor representation
  publication-title: Neural Computation
  doi: 10.1162/neco.1993.5.4.613
– volume: 80
  start-page: 1090
  issue: 4
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0095
  article-title: Dissociable effects of dopamine and serotonin on reversal learning
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.030
– volume: 31
  start-page: 5526
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0295
  article-title: Neural correlates of forward planning in a spatial decision task in humans
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4647-10.2011
– volume: 273
  start-page: 1399
  year: 1996
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0195
  article-title: A neostriatal habit learning system in humans
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.273.5280.1399
– volume: 104
  start-page: 1726
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0170
  article-title: Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610561104
– year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0040
– start-page: 3
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0070
  article-title: Trial-by-trial data analysis using computational models
– volume: 8
  start-page: 1704
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0080
  article-title: Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/nn1560
– volume: 42
  start-page: 305
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0035
  article-title: Experience-weighted attraction learning in coordination games: Probability rules, heterogeneity, and time-variation
  publication-title: Journal of Mathematical Psychology
  doi: 10.1006/jmps.1998.1217
– year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0205
– volume: 55
  start-page: 189
  year: 1948
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0340
  article-title: Cognitive maps in rats and men
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/h0061626
– volume: 15
  start-page: 507
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0270
  article-title: Dopamine-dependent plasticity of corticostriatal synapses
  publication-title: Neural Networks
  doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(02)00045-X
– volume: 14
  start-page: 148
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0265
  article-title: Hippocampal contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory
  publication-title: Hippocampus
  doi: 10.1002/hipo.20009
– volume: 379
  start-page: 255
  year: 1996
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0025
  article-title: Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/379255a0
– volume: 106
  start-page: 342
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0065
  article-title: Transitive inference in rats (rattus norvegicus)
  publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.106.4.342
– volume: 16
  start-page: 1936
  year: 1996
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0225
  article-title: A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-05-01936.1996
– year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0150
  article-title: Retrospective revaluation in sequential decision making: A tale of two systems
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0310
– volume: 324
  start-page: 1080
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0355
  article-title: Phasic firing in dopaminergic neurons is sufficient for behavioral conditioning
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.1168878
– volume: 32
  start-page: 15053
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0210
  article-title: Hippocampal neural correlates for values of experienced events
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2806-12.2012
– volume: 35
  start-page: 625
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0030
  article-title: The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9
– year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0330
– volume: 99
  start-page: 195
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0305
  article-title: Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.195
– volume: 7
  start-page: 887
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0335
  article-title: Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/nn1279
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1204
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0075
  article-title: Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1
  year: 1987
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0240
  article-title: Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
– volume: 22
  start-page: 10976
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0055
  article-title: Sensitivity to instrumental contingency degradation is mediated by the entorhinal cortex and its efferents via the dorsal hippocampus
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10976.2002
– volume: 6
  start-page: 70
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0395
  article-title: The hippocampus and inferential reasoning: Building memories to navigate future decisions
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00070
– volume: 4
  start-page: 257
  year: 1930
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0345
  article-title: Introduction and removal of reward, and maze performance in rats
  publication-title: University of California Publications in Psychology
– volume: 21
  start-page: 1934
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0360
  article-title: Neural evidence of statistical learning: Efficient detection of visual regularities without awareness
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21131
– volume: 63
  start-page: 267
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0315
  article-title: Mind the gap: Binding experiences across space and time in the human hippocampus
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.024
– volume: 19
  start-page: 181
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0385
  article-title: Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x
– volume: 9
  issue: 12
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0015
  article-title: Cortical and hippocampal correlates of deliberation during model-based decisions for rewards in humans
  publication-title: PLoS Computational Biology.
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003387
– volume: 15
  start-page: 338
  year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0185
  article-title: Acquired equivalence and distinctiveness of cues
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes
– volume: 1
  start-page: 106
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0200
  article-title: Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia
  publication-title: Learning & Memory
  doi: 10.1101/lm.1.2.106
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1075
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0110
  article-title: The ubiquity of model-based reinforcement learning
  publication-title: Current Opinion in Neurobiology
  doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.08.003
– volume: 2
  start-page: 487
  year: 1979
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0245
  article-title: The hippocampus as a cognitive map
  publication-title: Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00063949
– volume: 60
  start-page: 378
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0290
  article-title: Integrating memories in the human brain: Hippocampal–midbrain encoding of overlapping events
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.023
– year: 1980
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0100
– volume: 114
  start-page: 468
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0105
  article-title: Dissociation of pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists
  publication-title: Behavioral Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.468
– volume: 103
  start-page: 11778
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0120
  article-title: Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602659103
– volume: 75
  start-page: 418
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0380
  article-title: Dopamine enhances model-based over model-free choice behavior
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.042
– volume: 27
  start-page: 12176
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0190
  article-title: Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point
  publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3761-07.2007
– volume: 30
  start-page: 123
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0115
  article-title: The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory
  publication-title: Annual review of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1092
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0370
  article-title: Generalization of value in reinforcement learning by humans
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08017.x
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1622
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0280
  article-title: Shaping of object representations in the human medial temporal lobe based on temporal regularities
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.056
– volume: 29
  start-page: 2225
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0350
  article-title: A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06796.x
– volume: 338
  start-page: 270
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0375
  article-title: Preference by association: How memory mechanisms in the hippocampus bias decisions
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.1223252
– volume: 26
  start-page: 593
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0085
  article-title: The cognitive neuroscience of motivation and learning
  publication-title: Social Cognition
  doi: 10.1521/soco.2008.26.5.593
– volume: 16
  start-page: 966
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0320
  article-title: A causal link between prediction errors, dopamine neurons and learning
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/nn.3413
– year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0275
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1363
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0010
  article-title: Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
– volume: 7
  start-page: 457
  year: 1992
  ident: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014_b0145
  article-title: Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences
  publication-title: Statistical Science
  doi: 10.1214/ss/1177011136
– reference: 12371508 - Neural Netw. 2002 Jun-Jul;15(4-6):507-21
– reference: 19490086 - Eur J Neurosci. 2009 Jun;29(11):2225-32
– reference: 1451416 - J Comp Psychol. 1992 Dec;106(4):342-9
– reference: 15235607 - Nat Neurosci. 2004 Aug;7(8):887-93
– reference: 12486193 - J Neurosci. 2002 Dec 15;22(24):10976-84
– reference: 16286932 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Dec;8(12):1704-11
– reference: 23708143 - Nat Neurosci. 2013 Jul;16(7):966-73
– reference: 21435563 - Neuron. 2011 Mar 24;69(6):1204-15
– reference: 18870876 - Psychol Rev. 1948 Jul;55(4):189-208
– reference: 8136066 - Behav Neurosci. 1993 Dec;107(6):899-910
– reference: 22487039 - Eur J Neurosci. 2012 Apr;35(7):1092-104
– reference: 21389268 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Sep 13;108 Suppl 3:15647-54
– reference: 24339770 - PLoS Comput Biol. 2013;9(12):e1003387
– reference: 16045504 - Eur J Neurosci. 2005 Jul;22(2):513-23
– reference: 9710553 - J Math Psychol. 1998 Jun;42(2/3):305-26
– reference: 15098720 - Hippocampus. 2004;14(2):148-52
– reference: 19389999 - Science. 2009 May 22;324(5930):1080-4
– reference: 12194864 - Neuron. 2002 Aug 15;35(4):625-41
– reference: 16839289 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2006 Jul;18(7):1156-73
– reference: 24324166 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013 Dec 24;110(52):20941-6
– reference: 8703077 - Science. 1996 Sep 6;273(5280):1399-402
– reference: 2521896 - J Neurosci. 1989 Feb;9(2):582-7
– reference: 14750976 - Eur J Neurosci. 2004 Jan;19(1):181-9
– reference: 10467589 - Learn Mem. 1994 Jul-Aug;1(2):106-20
– reference: 15013954 - Brain. 2004 Apr;127(Pt 4):851-9
– reference: 10883798 - Behav Neurosci. 2000 Jun;114(3):468-83
– reference: 23558545 - Psychol Sci. 2013 May;24(5):751-61
– reference: 23100426 - J Neurosci. 2012 Oct 24;32(43):15053-65
– reference: 19230516 - J Exp Anal Behav. 2009 Jan;91(1):127-41
– reference: 6236779 - Arch Neurol. 1984 Sep;41(9):965-70
– reference: 20620875 - Neuron. 2010 Jun 24;66(6):896-907
– reference: 24027514 - Front Hum Neurosci. 2013 Sep 03;7:525
– reference: 20510862 - Neuron. 2010 May 27;66(4):585-95
– reference: 18957228 - Neuron. 2008 Oct 23;60(2):378-89
– reference: 22470333 - Front Hum Neurosci. 2012 Mar 26;6:70
– reference: 19640484 - Neuron. 2009 Jul 30;63(2):267-76
– reference: 22885059 - Curr Biol. 2012 Sep 11;22(17):1622-7
– reference: 23594744 - Nature. 2013 May 2;497(7447):74-9
– reference: 1594723 - Psychol Rev. 1992 Apr;99(2):195-231
– reference: 2794870 - J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1989 Oct;15(4):338-46
– reference: 21471389 - J Neurosci. 2011 Apr 6;31(14):5526-39
– reference: 17126370 - Neuropsychologia. 2007 Apr 8;45(7):1363-77
– reference: 10818159 - J Neurosci. 2000 Jun 1;20(11):4233-9
– reference: 12676056 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2003 Feb 15;15(2):185-93
– reference: 23066083 - Science. 2012 Oct 12;338(6104):270-3
– reference: 22959354 - Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2012 Dec;22(6):1075-81
– reference: 18823241 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2009 Oct;21(10):1934-45
– reference: 17417939 - Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:123-52
– reference: 17229836 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007 Jan 30;104(5):1726-31
– reference: 9496622 - Annu Rev Psychol. 1998;49:87-115
– reference: 22884326 - Neuron. 2012 Aug 9;75(3):418-24
– reference: 8538790 - Nature. 1996 Jan 18;379(6562):255-7
– reference: 22487032 - Eur J Neurosci. 2012 Apr;35(7):1011-23
– reference: 24267657 - Neuron. 2013 Nov 20;80(4):1090-100
– reference: 23230992 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014 Feb;143(1):182-94
– reference: 7414331 - Science. 1980 Oct 10;210(4466):207-10
– reference: 20624589 - Neuron. 2010 Jul 15;67(1):25-32
– reference: 8774460 - J Neurosci. 1996 Mar 1;16(5):1936-47
– reference: 16868087 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006 Aug 1;103(31):11778-83
– reference: 17989284 - J Neurosci. 2007 Nov 7;27(45):12176-89
SSID ssj0011598
Score 2.3863056
Snippet •We investigated the parallels between relational memory and decision strategies.•Model-based reinforcement learning correlated positively with acquired...
It has recently become widely appreciated that value-based decision making is supported by multiple computational strategies. In particular, animal and human...
* We investigated the parallels between relational memory and decision strategies. * Model-based reinforcement learning correlated positively with acquired...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 4
SubjectTerms Animals
Brain - physiology
Corpus Striatum - physiology
Decision making
Decision Making - physiology
Hippocampus - physiology
Humans
Learning - physiology
Memory
Memory - physiology
Models, Neurological
Models, Psychological
Monte Carlo Method
Neurobiology
Reinforcement (Psychology)
Reinforcement learning
Relational memory
Reward
Spatial Memory - physiology
Title Multiple memory systems as substrates for multiple decision systems
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.014
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24846190
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1645154181
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1645232895
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1664210690
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4233017
Volume 117
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LS-wwFD6IlwtuxOtzfBFBXAh1-kj6WI7DlbledKOCuAlt0uKIdsSOCzf-ds9Jk-IodxYXumibE2hykpMvzZcvAIcV10pXVeZFeZl5nPPMK5JQeDmP_DwOVBSaQ_suLuPRDT-_FbcLMHR7YYhWaWN_G9NNtLZv-rY2-8_jcf-KqIQ4sSPFObNeRDvYeUKt_OS9o3kg4MnSjneI1m5l03C86kfajB5wo3Ya8H-NTd-x51cK5acx6WwFli2YZIP2e3_BQlmvwtqgxon00xs7Yobeaf6br8LPU3e31AU9en03MXdrMLyw3EL2ROzbN9aKPDcsb1iD4cXI2DYMQS5zLESm7Qk9znYdbs5-Xw9Hnj1iwVMiDaceAiiE13EWKYRVQa4rv4p1prMkVLFSBc6c87Qowyqlru7nBY1lCAGDQOE0S4g02oDFelKXW8BigZkRfghfh1wpXSg_0lGpUj8tKsHDHviucqWy-uN0DMajdESzB4n-kOQP6eMV8B4cd1meW_GNecbceUzOtCCJg8O8bLvOu9J230ZS4bBqEP304KBLxo5Hqyl5XU5eWxuEo2km5tnQPmISg-7BZttguoKEHKFfQCnJTFPqDEj4ezalHt8bAXD0FcblZPv_SrwDS_gk2v9Iu7A4fXkt9xBZTYt903X24cfgz9_R5QdZNCHo
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3NT9swFH-CogkuE59bGQwjTTtMipoPO02OpQKVj_YykNAuVmInolNJ0VIO_Pe859gRZVoPk3qI4mep9rPf-zn--WeAbyXXSpdl6kVZkXqc89TL-6HwMh75WRyoKDSX9o0n8eiOX92L-zUYurMwRKu0sb-J6SZa2zc925u9p-m095OohLiwI8U5s1-0DhukTiU6sDG4vB5N2s0EzNhJSz3ECm5z09C8qhmdRw-4ETwN-L_S09_w8z2L8k1autiGjxZPskHzl3dgrah2YW9Q4Vr68YV9Z4bhaT6d78KHM_e01cY9ev1rbp72YDi29EL2SATcF9boPNcsq1mNEcYo2dYMcS5zRESm7SU9znYf7i7Ob4cjz96y4CmRhAsPMRQi7DiNFCKrINOlX8Y61Wk_VLFSOS6esyQvwjKh2e5nOaUzRIFBoHClJUQSHUCnmlfFZ2CxwMqIQISvQ66UzpUf6ahQiZ_kpeBhF3zXuVJZCXK6CWMmHdfst0R_SPKH9PEX8C78aKs8Nfobq4y585hcGkQS88OqakfOu9LO4FpS47BrEAB14bQtxrlHGypZVcyfGxtEpEkqVtnQUWLSg-7Cp2bAtA0JOaK_gEr6S0OpNSDt7-WSavpgNMDRVxia-4f_1-IT2Bzdjm_kzeXk-gtsYYloPisdQWfx57k4RqC1yL_aifQKNXUkmQ
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Multiple+memory+systems+as+substrates+for+multiple+decision+systems&rft.jtitle=Neurobiology+of+learning+and+memory&rft.au=Doll%2C+Bradley+B&rft.au=Shohamy%2C+Daphna&rft.au=Daw%2C+Nathaniel+D&rft.date=2015-01-01&rft.pub=Elsevier+BV&rft.issn=1074-7427&rft.eissn=1095-9564&rft.volume=117&rft.spage=4&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.nlm.2014.04.014&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT&rft.externalDocID=3556813771
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1074-7427&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1074-7427&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1074-7427&client=summon