MAINTAINED NODAL-DISTANCE EFFECTS IN EQUIVALENCE CLASSES
Twelve subjects were trained to select one of two stimuli from a pair (the B pair) when presented with one of two stimuli from another pair (the A pair), thus establishing two AB relations, A1‐B1 and A2‐B2. In a similar fashion, additional stimuli were used to establish BC, CD, and DE relations. Tri...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior Vol. 64; no. 2; pp. 129 - 145 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Oxford, UK
Blackwell Publishing Ltd
01.09.1995
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0022-5002 1938-3711 |
DOI | 10.1901/jeab.1995.64-129 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Twelve subjects were trained to select one of two stimuli from a pair (the B pair) when presented with one of two stimuli from another pair (the A pair), thus establishing two AB relations, A1‐B1 and A2‐B2. In a similar fashion, additional stimuli were used to establish BC, CD, and DE relations. Trials used to train all relations occurred in each session. Once performances were established, probe trials were introduced that tested for the emergence of untrained relations (e.g., B1‐D1 or A1‐E1). These emergent relations were categorized according to nodal distance (i.e., the number of stimuli across which transitivity would have to hold in order for the relation to emerge). For example, a test for A2‐C2 crosses one node (B2), whereas a test for A1‐E1 crosses three nodes (B1, C1, and D1). Only 2 of the subjects formed equivalence classes. The evocation of class‐appropriate responding by each emergent‐relation probe was an inverse function of nodal distance for all 12 subjects. In addition, performance on the originally trained relations was disrupted by the introduction of probes. The 2 subjects who exhibited equivalence classes were then trained to make different numbers of key presses in the presence of each of the four A and E stimuli. In a response‐transfer test, the B, C, and D stimuli evoked the responses trained to the A and E stimuli in the same equivalence class. Likelihood of class‐appropriate responses was an inverse function of nodal distance, and this pattern persisted across testing. Reaction times in the transfer test were an inverted U‐shaped function of nodal distance. Because training of the baseline relations occurred concurrently and the B, C, and D stimuli were presented an equal number of times before the transfer test, the test performances illustrate effects of nodal distance that were not confounded by order or amount of experience with the stimuli. The results imply that ordered, sequential exposure to individual stimulus relations may facilitate the development of equivalence classes and that the relatedness of stimuli within an equivalence class is a relatively permanent inverse function of nodal distance. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Twelve subjects were trained to select one of two stimuli from a pair (the B pair) when presented with one of two stimuli from another pair (the A pair), thus establishing two AB relations, A1-B1 and A2-B2. In a similar fashion, additional stimuli were used to establish BC, CD, and DE relations. Trials used to train all relations occurred in each session. Once performances were established, probe trials were introduced that tested for the emergence of untrained relations (e.g., B1-D1 or A1-E1). These emergent relations were categorized according to nodal distance (i.e., the number of stimuli across which transitivity would have to hold in order for the relation to emerge). For example, a test for A2-C2 crosses one node (B2), whereas a test for A1-E1 crosses three nodes (B1, C1, and D1). Only 2 of the subjects formed equivalence classes. The evocation of class-appropriate responding by each emergent-relation probe was an inverse function of nodal distance for all 12 subjects. In addition, performance on the originally trained relations was disrupted by the introduction of probes. The 2 subjects who exhibited equivalence classes were then trained to make different numbers of key presses in the presence of each of the four A and E stimuli. In a response-transfer test, the B, C, and D stimuli evoked the responses trained to the A and E stimuli in the same equivalence class. Likelihood of class-appropriate responses was an inverse function of nodal distance, and this pattern persisted across testing. Reaction times in the transfer test were an inverted U-shaped function of nodal distance. Because training of the baseline relations occurred concurrently and the B, C, and D stimuli were presented an equal number of times before the transfer test, the test performances illustrate effects of nodal distance that were not confounded by order or amount of experience with the stimuli. The results imply that ordered, sequential exposure to individual stimulus relations may facilitate the development of equivalence classes and that the relatedness of stimuli within an equivalence class is a relatively permanent inverse function of nodal distance. Twelve subjects were trained to select one of two stimuli from a pair (the B pair) when presented with one of two stimuli from another pair (the A pair), thus establishing two AB relations, A1-B1 and A2-B2. In a similar fashion, additional stimuli were used to establish BC, CD, and DE relations. Trials used to train all relations occurred in each session. Once performances were established, probe trials were introduced that tested for the emergence of untrained relations (e.g., B1-D1 or A1-E1). These emergent relations were categorized according to nodal distance (i.e., the number of stimuli across which transitivity would have to hold in order for the relation to emerge). For example, a test for A2-C2 crosses one node (B2), whereas a test for A1-E1 crosses three nodes (B1, C1, and D1). Only 2 of the subjects formed equivalence classes. The evocation of class-appropriate responding by each emergent-relation probe was an inverse function of nodal distance for all 12 subjects. In addition, performance on the originally trained relations was disrupted by the introduction of probes. The 2 subjects who exhibited equivalence classes were then trained to make different numbers of key presses in the presence of each of the four A and E stimuli. In a response-transfer test, the B, C, and D stimuli evoked the responses trained to the A and E stimuli in the same equivalence class. Likelihood of class-appropriate responses was an inverse function of nodal distance, and this pattern persisted across testing. Reaction times in the transfer test were an inverted U-shaped function of nodal distance. Because training of the baseline relations occurred concurrently and the B, C, and D stimuli were presented an equal number of times before the transfer test, the test performances illustrate effects of nodal distance that were not confounded by order or amount of experience with the stimuli. The results imply that ordered, sequential exposure to individual stimulus relations may facilitate the development of equivalence classes and that the relatedness of stimuli within an equivalence class is a relatively permanent inverse function of nodal distance.Twelve subjects were trained to select one of two stimuli from a pair (the B pair) when presented with one of two stimuli from another pair (the A pair), thus establishing two AB relations, A1-B1 and A2-B2. In a similar fashion, additional stimuli were used to establish BC, CD, and DE relations. Trials used to train all relations occurred in each session. Once performances were established, probe trials were introduced that tested for the emergence of untrained relations (e.g., B1-D1 or A1-E1). These emergent relations were categorized according to nodal distance (i.e., the number of stimuli across which transitivity would have to hold in order for the relation to emerge). For example, a test for A2-C2 crosses one node (B2), whereas a test for A1-E1 crosses three nodes (B1, C1, and D1). Only 2 of the subjects formed equivalence classes. The evocation of class-appropriate responding by each emergent-relation probe was an inverse function of nodal distance for all 12 subjects. In addition, performance on the originally trained relations was disrupted by the introduction of probes. The 2 subjects who exhibited equivalence classes were then trained to make different numbers of key presses in the presence of each of the four A and E stimuli. In a response-transfer test, the B, C, and D stimuli evoked the responses trained to the A and E stimuli in the same equivalence class. Likelihood of class-appropriate responses was an inverse function of nodal distance, and this pattern persisted across testing. Reaction times in the transfer test were an inverted U-shaped function of nodal distance. Because training of the baseline relations occurred concurrently and the B, C, and D stimuli were presented an equal number of times before the transfer test, the test performances illustrate effects of nodal distance that were not confounded by order or amount of experience with the stimuli. The results imply that ordered, sequential exposure to individual stimulus relations may facilitate the development of equivalence classes and that the relatedness of stimuli within an equivalence class is a relatively permanent inverse function of nodal distance. |
Author | Buffington, Dawn M. Fields, Lanny Adams, Barbara J. Landon-Jimenez, Donna V. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Lanny surname: Fields fullname: Fields, Lanny organization: THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND, QUEENS COLLEGE, AND THE GRADUATE SCHOOL AND UNIVERSITY CENTER OF THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK – sequence: 2 givenname: Donna V. surname: Landon-Jimenez fullname: Landon-Jimenez, Donna V. organization: THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND, QUEENS COLLEGE, AND THE GRADUATE SCHOOL AND UNIVERSITY CENTER OF THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK – sequence: 3 givenname: Dawn M. surname: Buffington fullname: Buffington, Dawn M. organization: THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND, QUEENS COLLEGE, AND THE GRADUATE SCHOOL AND UNIVERSITY CENTER OF THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK – sequence: 4 givenname: Barbara J. surname: Adams fullname: Adams, Barbara J. organization: THE COLLEGE OF STATEN ISLAND, QUEENS COLLEGE, AND THE GRADUATE SCHOOL AND UNIVERSITY CENTER OF THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16812764$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkcFv0zAUxi00xLrBnROquHDK8LNjO74ghTTdgkomSEa5PTmpCylpMpIUtv8eRy0VTEIcbD_Z3-_zs78zctK0jSXkOdAL0BReb6wpXKXFhfQ9YPoRmYDmgccVwAmZUMqYJ9x8Ss76fuMKLRV7Qk5BBsCU9CckeB8mae5GPJum17Nw4c2SLA_TKJ7G83kc5dk0Safxh5vkU7iIx-1oEWZZnD0lj9em7u2zw3pObuZxHl15i-vLJHI-pVBKe1zqgFkhV1z7wH0jBUjuF6yQsNalEFBIVvjgC8NKVapCr6gRUlHBOUhdUH5O3ux9b3fF1q5K2wydqfG2q7amu8fWVPj3SVN9xS_tDwQuKFDpDF4dDLr2-872A26rvrR1bRrb7npUnPsBY0HglC8fKDftrmvc65CBH3AuYbR78Wc_x0Z-f6kTyL2g7Nq-7-way2owQ9WO7VU1AsUxOxyzwzE7lD667BxIH4BH738jh7t-VrW9_68e38XhW6ZhBL09WPWDvTuCpvuGUnElcJleIss_66UIlviR_wKGzrSa |
CODEN | JEABAU |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_466 crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_664 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40614_017_0134_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lmot_2011_11_001 crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2011_11434359 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2020_104179 crossref_primary_10_1006_jecp_2000_2563 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395446 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_015_0120_9 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395526 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_020_00413_2 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_023_00581_x crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395483 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395682 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2002_78_291 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2006_107_04 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_018_0271_6 crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2011_11434361 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395720 crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_617 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_016_0185_0 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_014_0049_4 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2015_05_012 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395365 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2012_97_163 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jcbs_2020_01_013 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13420_011_0020_z crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_713 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_015_0152_1 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2002_78_527 crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2018_1553379 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395358 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_019_00365_2 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_024_00618_9 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2021_104343 crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2015_1084713 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_1997_68_67 crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2015_1066156 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2012_98_243 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_015_0118_3 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_019_00329_6 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395630 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2006_125_04 crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2011_11434397 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_014_0097_9 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40614_022_00334_1 crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_4230 crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_91 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_020_00377_3 crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_647 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_1997_67_367 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2005_45_03 crossref_primary_10_1002_jeab_726 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395226 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395468 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_015_0129_0 crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2022_2164828 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395505 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_00954 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2008_90_135 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395785 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2001_76_265 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_020_00384_4 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395388 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395387 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2002_78_271 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2007_10_06 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395336 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40614_023_00387_w crossref_primary_10_1080_15021149_2023_2269340 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395614 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_024_00616_x crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2006_58_04 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_018_0326_8 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_023_00552_2 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_023_00554_0 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_019_00345_6 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395691 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13420_013_0128_4 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2005_31_04 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2008_89_359 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395651 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40732_022_00519_9 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_1999_72_117 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395213 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_2011_95_343 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01848 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395374 crossref_primary_10_1007_BF03395252 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/0278-7393.12.3.336 10.1901/jeab.1986.46-243 10.1007/BF03395899 10.1901/jeab.1992.58-173 10.1901/jeab.1993.59-103 10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80069-1 10.1901/jeab.1989.52-261 10.1901/jeab.1990.53-345 10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-125 10.1080/14640749208401013 10.1901/jeab.1985.43-21 10.1016/0749-596X(88)90025-3 10.1901/jeab.1992.57-227 10.1901/jeab.1980.33-285 10.1901/jeab.1988.49-95 10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.414 10.1901/jaba.1994.27-673 10.1037/h0021799 10.1016/0093-934X(87)90060-5 10.1007/BF03395907 10.1037/h0048854 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 10.1037/10011-000 10.1016/0270-4684(86)90005-4 10.1016/0376-6357(91)90077-D 10.1016/S0270-3092(84)80001-6 10.1037/0278-7393.15.2.211 10.1901/jeab.1984.41-251 10.1037/0278-7393.14.3.398 10.1007/BF03395908 10.1037/0097-7403.12.3.203 10.1901/jeab.1987.48-317 10.1016/0022-0965(76)90064-3 10.1901/jeab.1984.42-143 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 1995 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Copyright Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Incorporated Sep 1995 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 1995 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – notice: Copyright Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Incorporated Sep 1995 |
DBID | BSCLL AAYXX CITATION NPM 7QG 7TK K9. 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1901/jeab.1995.64-129 |
DatabaseName | Istex CrossRef PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts Animal Behavior Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic PubMed ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1938-3711 |
EndPage | 145 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC1350106 6801761 16812764 10_1901_jeab_1995_64_129 JEAB2919 ark_67375_WNG_2TX9W58W_R |
Genre | article Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --Z -DZ -ET -~X .GJ .Y3 0-V 05W 0R~ 1OB 1OC 1OL 29L 2FS 2WC 31~ 33P 3EH 3V. 50Y 52M 52U 53G 5GY 5RE 7X7 8-1 88A 88E 8AO 8FE 8FH 8FI 8FJ 8G5 8R4 8R5 9M8 A00 AABNI AAESR AAFWJ AAHHS AAONW AAOUF AASGY AAXRX AAZKR ABCQX ABCUV ABIVO ABJNI ABPPZ ABRJW ABSOO ABTAH ABUWG ACAHQ ACBKW ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACKIV ACNCT ACPOU ACPRK ACXQS ADBBV ADEMA ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADMHG ADXAS ADZMN AEEZP AEGXH AEIGN AEIMD AENEX AEQDE AEUYR AFBPY AFFDN AFFNX AFFPM AFGKR AFKFF AFKRA AFPWT AHBTC AHMBA AI. AIAGR AIFKG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ALUQN AMBMR AMYDB ARALO ASPBG ASTYK AVWKF AYCSE AZBYB AZFZN AZQEC AZVAB B-7 BAWUL BBNVY BDRZF BENPR BFHJK BHPHI BMXJE BNVMJ BOGZA BPHCQ BQESF BRXPI BSCLL BVXVI C1A CCPQU CJNVE CS3 DCZOG DIK DPXWK DRFUL DRSSH DU5 DWQXO E3Z EBD EBS EJD EMOBN F5P FA8 FEDTE FYUFA G-S GNUQQ GODZA GUQSH GX1 HAOEW HCIFZ HF~ HGLYW HMCUK HVGLF HYE HZ~ H~9 KOO L7B LATKE LEEKS LITHE LK8 LOXES LUTES LYRES M0L M0P M1P M2M M2O M2R M7P MEWTI MRFUL MRSSH MSFUL MSSSH MVM MXFUL MXSSH MY~ NHB O66 O9- OHT OK1 OVD P2P P2W PADUT PALCI PQEDU PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ Q2X R.K RIWAO RJQFR ROL RPM SAMSI SUPJJ SV3 TEORI TN5 TR2 TWZ UKHRP UPT UQL VH1 VQA WBKPD WGLLI WH7 WHG WIH WII WOHZO WOQ WSUWO WXSBR WYJ XKC XOL XZL YQT YYQ YZZ ZCA ZCG ZGI ZUP ZXP ZY4 ZZTAW AAHQN AAMNL AANHP AAYCA ACRPL ACYXJ ADNMO AFWVQ AFYRF ALVPJ AAYXX ADXHL AETEA AEYWJ AGHNM AGQPQ CITATION PHGZM PHGZT NPM VXZ YIN 7QG 7TK K9. 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c5779-36982e56d394134a651634b2b61f9c551b62b4145a2c7c7b9d0a5670533169b03 |
ISSN | 0022-5002 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:22:50 EDT 2025 Thu Sep 04 16:20:53 EDT 2025 Sat Aug 23 10:10:53 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:35:32 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:54 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 04:06:25 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:24:07 EST 2025 Wed Oct 30 09:56:54 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 2 |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c5779-36982e56d394134a651634b2b61f9c551b62b4145a2c7c7b9d0a5670533169b03 |
Notes | ArticleID:JEAB2919 ark:/67375/WNG-2TX9W58W-R istex:32DFE020B335F17F4926E2E787A2F68A127858E2 ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 content type line 14 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdfdirect/10.1901/jeab.1995.64-129 |
PMID | 16812764 |
PQID | 214833616 |
PQPubID | 29953 |
PageCount | 17 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1350106 proquest_miscellaneous_733482288 proquest_journals_214833616 pubmed_primary_16812764 crossref_citationtrail_10_1901_jeab_1995_64_129 crossref_primary_10_1901_jeab_1995_64_129 wiley_primary_10_1901_jeab_1995_64_129_JEAB2919 istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_2TX9W58W_R |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 1900 |
PublicationDate | September 1995 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 1995-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 09 year: 1995 text: September 1995 |
PublicationDecade | 1990 |
PublicationPlace | Oxford, UK |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Oxford, UK – name: United States – name: Malden |
PublicationTitle | Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Exp Anal Behav |
PublicationYear | 1995 |
Publisher | Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
Publisher_xml | – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
References | Kennedy, C. L. (1991). Equivalence class formation influenced by the number of nodes separating stimuli. Behavioral Processes, 24, 219-245. Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T., & Newman, S. (1993). Are stimuli in equivalence classes equally related to each other The Psychological Record, 45, 85-105. Saunders, R. L., & Green, G. (1992). The nonequivalence of behavioral and mathematical equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 227-241. Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Newman, S., & Verhave, T. (1992). Interactions of emergent relations during the formation of equivalence classes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45B, 125-138. Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T., & Newman, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345-358. Fields, L., & Verhave, T. (1987). The structure of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 317-332. Balota, D. A., & Lorch, R. F. (1986). Depth of automatic spreading activation: Mediated priming effects in pronunciation but not in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 336-345. McNamara, T. P., & Healy, A. F. (1988). Semantic, phonological, and mediated priming in reading and lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 14, 398-409. Dube, W. V., Green, G., & Serna, R. W. (1993). Auditory successive conditional discrimination and auditory stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 103-114. Lazar, R. M., Davis-Lang, D., & Sanchez, L. (1984). The formation of visual stimulus equivalences in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 251-266. Slamecka, N. J. (1964). An inquiry into the doctrine of remote associations. Psychological Review, 71, 61-76. Slamecka, N. J. (1965). In defense of a new approach to old phenomena. Psychological Review, 72, 242-246. Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22. Adams, B. J., Fields, L., & Verhave, T. (1993). The effects of test order on equivalence class formation. The Psychological Record, 43, 133-152. Chiavello, C., Senehi, J., & Nuding, S. (1987). Semantic priming with abstract and concrete words: Differential asymmetry may be postlexical. Brain and Language, 31, 43-60. Fields, L., Adams, B. J., & Verhave, T. (1993). The effects of the structure of equivalence classes on test performances. The Psychological Record, 43, 697-713. McNamara, T. P., Hardy, J. K., & Hirtle, S. C. (1989). Subjective hierarchies in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 15, 211-227. Dickins, D. W., Bentall, R. P., & Smith, A. B. (1993). The role of individual stimulus names in the emergence of equivalence relations: The effects of interpolated paired-associates training of discordant associations between names. The Psychological Record, 43, 713-724. Saunders, R. L., Wachter, J. A., & Spradlin, J. E. (1988). Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member stimulus class via conditional discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 95-115. Kennedy, C. L., Itkonen, T., & Lindquist, K. (1994). Nodality effects during equivalence class formation: An extension to sight-word reading and concept development. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 673-683. Sidman, M., Wynne, C. K., Maguire, R. W., & Barnes, T. (1989). Functional classes and equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 261-274. Terrace, H. (1986). A nonverbal organism's knowledge of ordinal position in a serial learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 203-214. Sidman, M. (1980). A note on the measurement of conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 285-290. Sidman, M., Kirk, B., & Willson-Morris, M. (1985). Six-member stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 21-42. Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 125-144. Slamecka, N. J. (1985). Ebbinghaus: Some associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 414-435. Bentall, R. P., Dickins, D. W., & Fox, S. R. A. (1993). Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46B, 187-214. McNamara, T. P., & Altarriba, J. (1988). Depth of spreading activation revisited: Semantic mediated priming occurs in lexical decisions. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 545-559. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic memory. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428. McDonagh, E. C., McIlvane, W. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1984). Teaching coin equivalences via matcing to sample. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 5, 177-197. Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240-248. Sidman, M. (1992). Adventitious control by the location of comparison stimuli in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 173-182. Spradlin, J. E., & Saunders, R. L. (1986). The development of stimulus classes using match-to-sample procedures: Sample classification versus comparison classification. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 41-58. Dixon, M., & Spradlin, J. E. (1976). Establishing stimulus equivalences among retarded adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 144-164. Spradlin, J. E., & Dixon, M. H. (1976). Establishing conditional discriminations without direct training: Stimulus classes and labels. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 80, 555-561. Fields, L., Verhave, T., & Fath, S. J. (1984). Stimulus equivalence and transitive associations: A methodological analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 143-157. Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). On memory: A contribution to experimental psychology: 1885 (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussinius, Trans.). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University. Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243-257. Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative, Inc. 1976; 21 1990; 53 1984; 41 1993; 45 1984; 42 1982; 37 1987; 31 1986; 12 1993; 46B 1988; 14 1993; 43 1965; 72 1976; 80 1994; 27 1992; 57 1994 1992; 58 1985; 43 1913 1993; 59 1969; 8 1989; 52 1990 1991; 24 1986; 46 1988; 27 1988; 49 1980; 33 1984; 5 1986; 6 1988; 40 1975; 82 1989; 15 1985; 11 1992; 45B 1987; 48 1964; 71 e_1_2_1_20_1 e_1_2_1_41_1 e_1_2_1_40_1 e_1_2_1_23_1 e_1_2_1_24_1 e_1_2_1_21_1 e_1_2_1_22_1 Spradlin J. E. (e_1_2_1_38_1) 1976; 80 e_1_2_1_27_1 e_1_2_1_28_1 e_1_2_1_25_1 e_1_2_1_26_1 e_1_2_1_7_1 e_1_2_1_8_1 e_1_2_1_30_1 e_1_2_1_5_1 Fields L. (e_1_2_1_16_1) 1993; 45 e_1_2_1_6_1 e_1_2_1_3_1 e_1_2_1_12_1 e_1_2_1_35_1 e_1_2_1_13_1 e_1_2_1_34_1 e_1_2_1_10_1 Sidman M. (e_1_2_1_31_1) 1994 e_1_2_1_33_1 e_1_2_1_11_1 e_1_2_1_32_1 e_1_2_1_39_1 e_1_2_1_17_1 Sidman M. (e_1_2_1_29_1) 1990 e_1_2_1_14_1 e_1_2_1_37_1 e_1_2_1_15_1 e_1_2_1_36_1 Adams B. J. (e_1_2_1_2_1) 1993; 43 Bentall R. P. (e_1_2_1_4_1) 1993; 46 e_1_2_1_9_1 e_1_2_1_18_1 e_1_2_1_19_1 |
References_xml | – reference: Bentall, R. P., Dickins, D. W., & Fox, S. R. A. (1993). Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46B, 187-214. – reference: Dickins, D. W., Bentall, R. P., & Smith, A. B. (1993). The role of individual stimulus names in the emergence of equivalence relations: The effects of interpolated paired-associates training of discordant associations between names. The Psychological Record, 43, 713-724. – reference: Kennedy, C. L., Itkonen, T., & Lindquist, K. (1994). Nodality effects during equivalence class formation: An extension to sight-word reading and concept development. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 673-683. – reference: McNamara, T. P., & Healy, A. F. (1988). Semantic, phonological, and mediated priming in reading and lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 14, 398-409. – reference: Spradlin, J. E., & Saunders, R. L. (1986). The development of stimulus classes using match-to-sample procedures: Sample classification versus comparison classification. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 41-58. – reference: Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22. – reference: Sidman, M. (1980). A note on the measurement of conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 285-290. – reference: Fields, L., Adams, B. J., & Verhave, T. (1993). The effects of the structure of equivalence classes on test performances. The Psychological Record, 43, 697-713. – reference: Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243-257. – reference: McDonagh, E. C., McIlvane, W. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1984). Teaching coin equivalences via matcing to sample. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 5, 177-197. – reference: Saunders, R. L., & Green, G. (1992). The nonequivalence of behavioral and mathematical equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 227-241. – reference: Terrace, H. (1986). A nonverbal organism's knowledge of ordinal position in a serial learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 203-214. – reference: Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 125-144. – reference: Dube, W. V., Green, G., & Serna, R. W. (1993). Auditory successive conditional discrimination and auditory stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 103-114. – reference: Sidman, M. (1992). Adventitious control by the location of comparison stimuli in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 173-182. – reference: Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative, Inc. – reference: Slamecka, N. J. (1964). An inquiry into the doctrine of remote associations. Psychological Review, 71, 61-76. – reference: Spradlin, J. E., & Dixon, M. H. (1976). Establishing conditional discriminations without direct training: Stimulus classes and labels. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 80, 555-561. – reference: Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Newman, S., & Verhave, T. (1992). Interactions of emergent relations during the formation of equivalence classes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45B, 125-138. – reference: Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240-248. – reference: Slamecka, N. J. (1985). Ebbinghaus: Some associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 414-435. – reference: Lazar, R. M., Davis-Lang, D., & Sanchez, L. (1984). The formation of visual stimulus equivalences in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 251-266. – reference: Fields, L., Verhave, T., & Fath, S. J. (1984). Stimulus equivalence and transitive associations: A methodological analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 143-157. – reference: Sidman, M., Wynne, C. K., Maguire, R. W., & Barnes, T. (1989). Functional classes and equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 261-274. – reference: Chiavello, C., Senehi, J., & Nuding, S. (1987). Semantic priming with abstract and concrete words: Differential asymmetry may be postlexical. Brain and Language, 31, 43-60. – reference: McNamara, T. P., & Altarriba, J. (1988). Depth of spreading activation revisited: Semantic mediated priming occurs in lexical decisions. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 545-559. – reference: Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T., & Newman, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345-358. – reference: Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T., & Newman, S. (1993). Are stimuli in equivalence classes equally related to each other The Psychological Record, 45, 85-105. – reference: Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). On memory: A contribution to experimental psychology: 1885 (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussinius, Trans.). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University. – reference: McNamara, T. P., Hardy, J. K., & Hirtle, S. C. (1989). Subjective hierarchies in spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 15, 211-227. – reference: Sidman, M., Kirk, B., & Willson-Morris, M. (1985). Six-member stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 21-42. – reference: Saunders, R. L., Wachter, J. A., & Spradlin, J. E. (1988). Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member stimulus class via conditional discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 95-115. – reference: Slamecka, N. J. (1965). In defense of a new approach to old phenomena. Psychological Review, 72, 242-246. – reference: Balota, D. A., & Lorch, R. F. (1986). Depth of automatic spreading activation: Mediated priming effects in pronunciation but not in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 336-345. – reference: Fields, L., & Verhave, T. (1987). The structure of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 317-332. – reference: Kennedy, C. L. (1991). Equivalence class formation influenced by the number of nodes separating stimuli. Behavioral Processes, 24, 219-245. – reference: Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic memory. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428. – reference: Adams, B. J., Fields, L., & Verhave, T. (1993). The effects of test order on equivalence class formation. The Psychological Record, 43, 133-152. – reference: Dixon, M., & Spradlin, J. E. (1976). Establishing stimulus equivalences among retarded adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 144-164. – volume: 48 start-page: 317 year: 1987 end-page: 332 article-title: The structure of equivalence classes publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 52 start-page: 261 year: 1989 end-page: 274 article-title: Functional classes and equivalence relations publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 57 start-page: 227 year: 1992 end-page: 241 article-title: The nonequivalence of behavioral and mathematical equivalence publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 27 start-page: 545 year: 1988 end-page: 559 article-title: Depth of spreading activation revisited: Semantic mediated priming occurs in lexical decisions publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 6 start-page: 41 year: 1986 end-page: 58 article-title: The development of stimulus classes using match‐to‐sample procedures: Sample classification versus comparison classification publication-title: Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities – volume: 27 start-page: 673 year: 1994 end-page: 683 article-title: Nodality effects during equivalence class formation: An extension to sight‐word reading and concept development publication-title: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis – volume: 43 start-page: 133 year: 1993 end-page: 152 article-title: The effects of test order on equivalence class formation publication-title: The Psychological Record – volume: 43 start-page: 21 year: 1985 end-page: 42 article-title: Six‐member stimulus classes generated by conditional‐discrimination procedures publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 12 start-page: 336 year: 1986 end-page: 345 article-title: Depth of automatic spreading activation: Mediated priming effects in pronunciation but not in lexical decision publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 80 start-page: 555 year: 1976 end-page: 561 article-title: Establishing conditional discriminations without direct training: Stimulus classes and labels publication-title: American Journal of Mental Deficiency – volume: 5 start-page: 177 year: 1984 end-page: 197 article-title: Teaching coin equivalences via matcing to sample publication-title: Applied Research in Mental Retardation – volume: 72 start-page: 242 year: 1965 end-page: 246 article-title: In defense of a new approach to old phenomena publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 12 start-page: 203 year: 1986 end-page: 214 article-title: A nonverbal organism's knowledge of ordinal position in a serial learning task publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes – volume: 14 start-page: 398 year: 1988 end-page: 409 article-title: Semantic, phonological, and mediated priming in reading and lexical decisions publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition – volume: 43 start-page: 713 year: 1993 end-page: 724 article-title: The role of individual stimulus names in the emergence of equivalence relations: The effects of interpolated paired‐associates training of discordant associations between names publication-title: The Psychological Record – volume: 41 start-page: 251 year: 1984 end-page: 266 article-title: The formation of visual stimulus equivalences in children publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 37 start-page: 5 year: 1982 end-page: 22 article-title: Conditional discrimination vs matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 42 start-page: 143 year: 1984 end-page: 157 article-title: Stimulus equivalence and transitive associations: A methodological analysis publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – year: 1994 – volume: 45 start-page: 85 year: 1993 end-page: 105 article-title: Are stimuli in equivalence classes equally related to each other publication-title: The Psychological Record – volume: 71 start-page: 61 year: 1964 end-page: 76 article-title: An inquiry into the doctrine of remote associations publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 53 start-page: 345 year: 1990 end-page: 358 article-title: The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 24 start-page: 219 year: 1991 end-page: 245 article-title: Equivalence class formation influenced by the number of nodes separating stimuli publication-title: Behavioral Processes – volume: 59 start-page: 103 year: 1993 end-page: 114 article-title: Auditory successive conditional discrimination and auditory stimulus equivalence classes publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – year: 1913 – volume: 58 start-page: 173 year: 1992 end-page: 182 article-title: Adventitious control by the location of comparison stimuli in conditional discriminations publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 31 start-page: 43 year: 1987 end-page: 60 article-title: Semantic priming with abstract and concrete words: Differential asymmetry may be postlexical publication-title: Brain and Language – volume: 11 start-page: 414 year: 1985 end-page: 435 article-title: Ebbinghaus: Some associations publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 46 start-page: 243 year: 1986 end-page: 257 article-title: Equivalence class formation in language‐able and language‐disabled children publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 49 start-page: 95 year: 1988 end-page: 115 article-title: Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight‐member stimulus class via conditional discrimination procedures publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – start-page: 93 year: 1990 end-page: 114 – volume: 82 start-page: 407 year: 1975 end-page: 428 article-title: A spreading activation theory of semantic memory publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 8 start-page: 240 year: 1969 end-page: 248 article-title: Retrieval time from semantic memory publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior – volume: 40 start-page: 125 year: 1988 end-page: 144 article-title: Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 21 start-page: 144 year: 1976 end-page: 164 article-title: Establishing stimulus equivalences among retarded adolescents publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology – volume: 43 start-page: 697 year: 1993 end-page: 713 article-title: The effects of the structure of equivalence classes on test performances publication-title: The Psychological Record – volume: 46B start-page: 187 year: 1993 end-page: 214 article-title: Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 15 start-page: 211 year: 1989 end-page: 227 article-title: Subjective hierarchies in spatial memory publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition – volume: 33 start-page: 285 year: 1980 end-page: 290 article-title: A note on the measurement of conditional discriminations publication-title: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior – volume: 45B start-page: 125 year: 1992 end-page: 138 article-title: Interactions of emergent relations during the formation of equivalence classes publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – ident: e_1_2_1_3_1 doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.12.3.336 – ident: e_1_2_1_8_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1986.46-243 – volume: 43 start-page: 133 year: 1993 ident: e_1_2_1_2_1 article-title: The effects of test order on equivalence class formation publication-title: The Psychological Record doi: 10.1007/BF03395899 – ident: e_1_2_1_30_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1992.58-173 – ident: e_1_2_1_11_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1993.59-103 – ident: e_1_2_1_7_1 doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(69)80069-1 – ident: e_1_2_1_34_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1989.52-261 – ident: e_1_2_1_15_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.53-345 – ident: e_1_2_1_33_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5 – ident: e_1_2_1_41_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-125 – start-page: 93 volume-title: Behavior analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies year: 1990 ident: e_1_2_1_29_1 – ident: e_1_2_1_13_1 doi: 10.1080/14640749208401013 – ident: e_1_2_1_32_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1985.43-21 – ident: e_1_2_1_23_1 doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(88)90025-3 – ident: e_1_2_1_26_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1992.57-227 – ident: e_1_2_1_28_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1980.33-285 – volume-title: Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story year: 1994 ident: e_1_2_1_31_1 – ident: e_1_2_1_27_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.49-95 – ident: e_1_2_1_37_1 doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.414 – ident: e_1_2_1_20_1 doi: 10.1901/jaba.1994.27-673 – ident: e_1_2_1_36_1 doi: 10.1037/h0021799 – ident: e_1_2_1_5_1 doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(87)90060-5 – ident: e_1_2_1_14_1 doi: 10.1007/BF03395907 – ident: e_1_2_1_35_1 doi: 10.1037/h0048854 – ident: e_1_2_1_6_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 – ident: e_1_2_1_12_1 doi: 10.1037/10011-000 – ident: e_1_2_1_39_1 doi: 10.1016/0270-4684(86)90005-4 – ident: e_1_2_1_19_1 doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(91)90077-D – volume: 80 start-page: 555 year: 1976 ident: e_1_2_1_38_1 article-title: Establishing conditional discriminations without direct training: Stimulus classes and labels publication-title: American Journal of Mental Deficiency – volume: 45 start-page: 85 year: 1993 ident: e_1_2_1_16_1 article-title: Are stimuli in equivalence classes equally related to each other publication-title: The Psychological Record – ident: e_1_2_1_22_1 doi: 10.1016/S0270-3092(84)80001-6 – ident: e_1_2_1_24_1 doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.15.2.211 – ident: e_1_2_1_21_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1984.41-251 – ident: e_1_2_1_25_1 doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.14.3.398 – volume: 46 start-page: 187 year: 1993 ident: e_1_2_1_4_1 article-title: Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – ident: e_1_2_1_9_1 doi: 10.1007/BF03395908 – ident: e_1_2_1_40_1 doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.12.3.203 – ident: e_1_2_1_17_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1987.48-317 – ident: e_1_2_1_10_1 doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(76)90064-3 – ident: e_1_2_1_18_1 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1984.42-143 |
SSID | ssj0029672 |
Score | 1.6931915 |
Snippet | Twelve subjects were trained to select one of two stimuli from a pair (the B pair) when presented with one of two stimuli from another pair (the A pair), thus... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref wiley istex |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 129 |
SubjectTerms | Behavior College students equivalence class functional class keyboard response nodal-distance effect Psychology reaction time relational-control kernel Stimuli stimulus substitutability transfer test |
Title | MAINTAINED NODAL-DISTANCE EFFECTS IN EQUIVALENCE CLASSES |
URI | https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-2TX9W58W-R/fulltext.pdf https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1901%2Fjeab.1995.64-129 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16812764 https://www.proquest.com/docview/214833616 https://www.proquest.com/docview/733482288 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC1350106 |
Volume | 64 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnR3JbhMx1IraSy-InVBAc0BIHKbNeLyMjylpSVOIEKRKbpZnExGVA00ili_hc3lvxrOERGXJYZTYM-PYb_HbTcjzPInyNAHtROVS-Syiqa8kpz7sLFnG0gQ-RZTvWAwv2WjGZ53Oz1bU0noVHyU_duaV_A9UoQ3gilmy_wDZ-qXQAN8BvnAFCMP1r2D8FtR6VO3Rhb9IzZWfojSIpFqFaWBBkC_rOQxaUHCCsrKLGtyWSIv4jnbFf9OqWFLl89cAx9C3ZZlabW1tmIcf6cL6I3xFaZweLKw1TSztyTrPm6j9gflqG4NsP3WWH-cFcT6r1GXp8TrqaqfxsTSmNWFKVRYB7_VKTpyVzFcB8w2lY76OO5c1zh0W0harDUpLydYWoMqjBzITYyYmPxJYl1E1213l4h_2P-h3gzP95nx8sdlb7O4Ctm6JevU-BQUEDwUZnF_UqrwSsq5Dj7NwDnCF9ax-G3lD4NlH2v22S5vZDsptK0uFtDO5TW45pPD6Jc7dIZ3M3iUH9W75_R6JGuTzNpHPc8jnza3XQj7PId99cnl2Onk19N0xHH7CJRBwKFREMy7SUIHEw4zgIMOzmMYiyFUCEncsaMwCxg1NZCJjlfYMFxKTvAOh4l74gOzZhc0eES8H7ThSIPOmQjHDc5NDSxByYQTLQffvkuNqrXTiatTjUSlXGnVVWF2Nq6txdbVgGla3S17WT3wu67PccO-LYvnrG831J4xrlFxPx681nczUlEdT_b5LDiv4aEfuS00DFoWhCESXeHUv8GJ0sBmbLdZLLTGtndII5vGwBGbzn7DOnxSsS-QGmOsbsMz7Zo-dfyzKvQfo--_BsMcFQvxxmnp02j-hKlCPb5zGITloKPcJ2Vtdr7OnIGCv4mcFrv8CkhDPxw |
linkProvider | Flying Publisher |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Maintained+nodal-distance+effects+in+equivalence+classes&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+the+experimental+analysis+of+behavior&rft.au=Fields%2C+Lanny&rft.au=Landon-Jimenez%2C+Donna+V&rft.au=Buffington%2C+Dawn+M&rft.au=Adams%2C+Barbara+J&rft.date=1995-09-01&rft.pub=Blackwell+Publishing+Ltd&rft.issn=0022-5002&rft.eissn=1938-3711&rft.volume=64&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=129&rft_id=info:doi/10.1901%2Fjeab.1995.64-129&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK&rft.externalDocID=6801761 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0022-5002&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0022-5002&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0022-5002&client=summon |