Responses to song playback vary with the vocal performance of both signal senders and receivers
Mating signals of many animal species are difficult to produce and thus should indicate signaler quality. Growing evidence suggests that receivers modulate their behaviour in response to signals with varying performance levels, although little is known about if and how responses are affected by rece...
Saved in:
Published in | Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences Vol. 280; no. 1768; p. 20131401 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
The Royal Society
07.10.2013
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Mating signals of many animal species are difficult to produce and thus should indicate signaler quality. Growing evidence suggests that receivers modulate their behaviour in response to signals with varying performance levels, although little is known about if and how responses are affected by receiver attributes. To explore this topic we conducted two experiments with swamp sparrows, Melospiza georgiana, in which we challenged territorial males with playback of songs with trill rates that were natural, digitally reduced, or digitally elevated (control-, low- and high-performance stimuli, respectively). In our first experiment, we found that males responded more aggressively to control songs than to low-performance stimuli, that low-performance stimuli with the most severe trill-rate reductions elicited the weakest aggressive responses, and that the subjects' own trill rates predicted aggressive responses. In our second experiment, we found that male responses to high-performance stimuli varied significantly, in ways predicted by two factors: the degree to which we had elevated stimulus performance levels of high-performance stimuli, and subjects' own vocal performance levels. Specifically, males were less aggressive towards stimuli for which we had elevated performance levels to higher degrees, and subject males with higher vocal performances themselves responded more aggressively. These findings together offer a novel illustration of how responses to aggressive signals may rely not just on signal attributes, but also on attributes of responding animals themselves. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Mating signals of many animal species are difficult to produce and thus should indicate signaler quality. Growing evidence suggests that receivers modulate their behaviour in response to signals with varying performance levels, although little is known about if and how responses are affected by receiver attributes. To explore this topic we conducted two experiments with swamp sparrows,
Melospiza georgiana
, in which we challenged territorial males with playback of songs with trill rates that were natural, digitally reduced, or digitally elevated (control-, low- and high-performance stimuli, respectively). In our first experiment, we found that males responded more aggressively to control songs than to low-performance stimuli, that low-performance stimuli with the most severe trill-rate reductions elicited the weakest aggressive responses, and that the subjects' own trill rates predicted aggressive responses. In our second experiment, we found that male responses to high-performance stimuli varied significantly, in ways predicted by two factors: the degree to which we had elevated stimulus performance levels of high-performance stimuli, and subjects' own vocal performance levels. Specifically, males were less aggressive towards stimuli for which we had elevated performance levels to higher degrees, and subject males with higher vocal performances themselves responded more aggressively. These findings together offer a novel illustration of how responses to aggressive signals may rely not just on signal attributes, but also on attributes of responding animals themselves. Mating signals of many animal species are difficult to produce and thus should indicate signaler quality. Growing evidence suggests that receivers modulate their behaviour in response to signals with varying performance levels, although little is known about if and how responses are affected by receiver attributes. To explore this topic we conducted two experiments with swamp sparrows, Melospiza georgiana, in which we challenged territorial males with playback of songs with trill rates that were natural, digitally reduced, or digitally elevated (control-, low- and high-performance stimuli, respectively). In our first experiment, we found that males responded more aggressively to control songs than to low-performance stimuli, that low-performance stimuli with the most severe trill-rate reductions elicited the weakest aggressive responses, and that the subjects' own trill rates predicted aggressive responses. In our second experiment, we found that male responses to high-performance stimuli varied significantly, in ways predicted by two factors: the degree to which we had elevated stimulus performance levels of high-performance stimuli, and subjects' own vocal performance levels. Specifically, males were less aggressive towards stimuli for which we had elevated performance levels to higher degrees, and subject males with higher vocal performances themselves responded more aggressively. These findings together offer a novel illustration of how responses to aggressive signals may rely not just on signal attributes, but also on attributes of responding animals themselves.Mating signals of many animal species are difficult to produce and thus should indicate signaler quality. Growing evidence suggests that receivers modulate their behaviour in response to signals with varying performance levels, although little is known about if and how responses are affected by receiver attributes. To explore this topic we conducted two experiments with swamp sparrows, Melospiza georgiana, in which we challenged territorial males with playback of songs with trill rates that were natural, digitally reduced, or digitally elevated (control-, low- and high-performance stimuli, respectively). In our first experiment, we found that males responded more aggressively to control songs than to low-performance stimuli, that low-performance stimuli with the most severe trill-rate reductions elicited the weakest aggressive responses, and that the subjects' own trill rates predicted aggressive responses. In our second experiment, we found that male responses to high-performance stimuli varied significantly, in ways predicted by two factors: the degree to which we had elevated stimulus performance levels of high-performance stimuli, and subjects' own vocal performance levels. Specifically, males were less aggressive towards stimuli for which we had elevated performance levels to higher degrees, and subject males with higher vocal performances themselves responded more aggressively. These findings together offer a novel illustration of how responses to aggressive signals may rely not just on signal attributes, but also on attributes of responding animals themselves. Mating signals of many animal species are difficult to produce and thus should indicate signaler quality. Growing evidence suggests that receivers modulate their behaviour in response to signals with varying performance levels, although little is known about if and how responses are affected by receiver attributes. To explore this topic we conducted two experiments with swamp sparrows, Melospiza georgiana, in which we challenged territorial males with playback of songs with trill rates that were natural, digitally reduced, or digitally elevated (control-, low- and high-performance stimuli, respectively). In our first experiment, we found that males responded more aggressively to control songs than to low-performance stimuli, that low-performance stimuli with the most severe trill-rate reductions elicited the weakest aggressive responses, and that the subjects' own trill rates predicted aggressive responses. In our second experiment, we found that male responses to high-performance stimuli varied significantly, in ways predicted by two factors: the degree to which we had elevated stimulus performance levels of high-performance stimuli, and subjects' own vocal performance levels. Specifically, males were less aggressive towards stimuli for which we had elevated performance levels to higher degrees, and subject males with higher vocal performances themselves responded more aggressively. These findings together offer a novel illustration of how responses to aggressive signals may rely not just on signal attributes, but also on attributes of responding animals themselves. |
Author | Podos, Jeffrey Lahti, David C. Moseley, Dana L. |
AuthorAffiliation | 1 Department of Biology and Program in Organismic and Evolutionary Biology , University of Massachusetts Amherst , 611 N. Pleasant St., Amherst, MA 01003 , USA 2 Department of Biology, Queens College , City University of New York , 65-30 Kissena Boulevard, Flushing, NY 11367 , USA |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 2 Department of Biology, Queens College , City University of New York , 65-30 Kissena Boulevard, Flushing, NY 11367 , USA – name: 1 Department of Biology and Program in Organismic and Evolutionary Biology , University of Massachusetts Amherst , 611 N. Pleasant St., Amherst, MA 01003 , USA |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Dana L. surname: Moseley fullname: Moseley, Dana L. email: dmoseley@bio.umass.edu organization: Department of Biology and Program in Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, University of Massachusetts Amherst, 611 N. Pleasant St., Amherst, MA 01003, USA – sequence: 2 givenname: David C. surname: Lahti fullname: Lahti, David C. organization: Department of Biology, Queens College, City University of New York, 65-30 Kissena Boulevard, Flushing, NY 11367, USA – sequence: 3 givenname: Jeffrey surname: Podos fullname: Podos, Jeffrey organization: Department of Biology and Program in Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, University of Massachusetts Amherst, 611 N. Pleasant St., Amherst, MA 01003, USA |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23945685$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkc9vFCEcxYmpsdvq1aPh6GVWYGCYuZiYpl1Nmpj480gY5ssu7SyMwK7ufy-TbRs1aTwR8n3vfR98ztCJDx4QeknJkpKufRPT1C8ZofWSckKfoAXlklas4-IELUjXsKrlgp2is5RuCCGdaMUzdMrqImhasUDqE6Qp-AQJ54BT8Gs8jfrQa3OL9zoe8E-XNzhvAO-D0SOeINoQt9obwMHiPpRpcmtfRgn8ADFh7QccwYDbl9tz9NTqMcGLu_Mcfb26_HLxvrr-uPpw8e66MkKKXJkeKBjRge2gEdK2dctI33NmNeWD7GGwwlqg0mrS2lZa2QnZ8IbzYZBNw-tz9PaYO-36LQwGfI56VFN02_IKFbRTf0-826h12KtaCtnJugS8vguI4ccOUlZblwyMo_YQdklRIWhZyCT5v5SzhhAmalqkr_6s9dDnHkAR8KPAxJBSBKuMyzq7MLd0o6JEzZzVzFnNnNXMudiW_9jukx811EdDDIfCIRgH-aBuwi4WdOlxV3V0uZTh18MOHW9VI8vPqW8tV1ffV3JVd5_Vqv4NbobNTw |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1002_ecs2_1259 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2016_12_026 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2021_1156 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2016_06_015 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_aru135 crossref_primary_10_1134_S1062359022100168 crossref_primary_10_35885_1684_7318_2021_2_191_229 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2015_01_038 crossref_primary_10_1080_03949370_2021_1949752 crossref_primary_10_3389_fevo_2020_587120 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10336_022_02021_z crossref_primary_10_1126_science_adj4478 crossref_primary_10_1002_ece3_3760 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2018_03_007 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2018_1356 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_018_25834_6 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_araa135 crossref_primary_10_1093_icb_icx080 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_160341 crossref_primary_10_1186_s40657_015_0015_0 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_arz142 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_80220_5 crossref_primary_10_1086_674379 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2019_02_011 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2016_03_036 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_arv162 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2021_04_009 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2016_12_010 crossref_primary_10_1111_1365_2435_13010 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_016_2088_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2017_08_012 crossref_primary_10_1642_AUK_17_2_1 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsbl_2013_1083 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_020_2825_2 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_221096 crossref_primary_10_1080_09524622_2019_1674693 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_arw063 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_019_2708_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2021_02_007 |
Cites_doi | 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01930.x 10.1038/35051570 10.1007/s00265-010-0949-5 10.1006/anbe.2001.1847 10.1093/beheco/arg090 10.1126/science.296.5569.873 10.1007/s002650050216 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.07.017 10.1007/s00265-010-1073-2 10.1111/j.2006.0908-8857.03839.x 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb02441.x 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0626 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.027 10.1093/beheco/arn021 10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80143-8 10.1006/anbe.1996.0107 10.1038/332152a0 10.1515/9780691207278 10.1098/rspb.2009.0127 10.1016/S0065-3454(09)40005-6 10.1017/CBO9780511754791 10.1006/anbe.1999.1140 10.1093/beheco/arq022 10.1086/596530 10.1093/beheco/arn135 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80110-8 10.1098/rspb.2002.2192 10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80188-7 10.1016/B978-012473070-0/50005-0 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.035 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.009 10.1038/246015a0 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x 10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80175-5 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.025 10.1242/jeb.071944 10.1007/s00265-006-0161-9 10.1098/rspb.2002.2112 10.1098/rspb.2006.3535 10.1111/j.1557-9263.2012.00377.x |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. 2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. 2013 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. – notice: 2013 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. 2013 |
DBID | BSCLL AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 7QG 5PM |
DOI | 10.1098/rspb.2013.1401 |
DatabaseName | Istex CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic Animal Behavior Abstracts PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic Animal Behavior Abstracts |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic Animal Behavior Abstracts MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Sciences (General) Biology |
DocumentTitleAlternate | Vocal performance, senders and receivers |
EISSN | 1471-2945 1471-2954 |
EndPage | 20131401 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC3757973 23945685 10_1098_rspb_2013_1401 ark_67375_V84_FWG7G39S_G |
Genre | Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GroupedDBID | --- -~X 0R~ 0VX 29P 2WC 36Y 4.4 5RE 85S AACGO AANCE ABBHK ABPLY ABTLG ABXSQ ACIWK ACNCT ACPRK ACQIA ADACV ADBBV ADIYS ADULT AEUPB AEXZC AFRAH AJZGM ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALMYZ AOIJS AQVQM BAWUL BGBPD BSCLL BTFSW CS3 DCCCD DIK DOOOF E3Z EBS EJD F5P FRP GX1 H13 HYE HZ~ IPSME JAAYA JBMMH JENOY JHFFW JKQEH JLS JLXEF JPM JSG JSODD JST K-O KQ8 MRS O9- OK1 OP1 RHF RPM RRY SA0 TR2 V1E W8F ~02 ABPTK ADZLD DNJUQ DWIUU AAYXX ACHIC ACRPL ADNMO ADQXQ AGPVY AGQPQ CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 7QG 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c575t-cbe1ec59ef9e657f83820bb42fa14d7bedf5ffe17fa08f87f795764644dd76643 |
ISSN | 0962-8452 1471-2954 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 14:12:40 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 16:12:32 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 11:15:45 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:05:21 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:03:18 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:06:52 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 17 02:37:25 EST 2024 Wed Oct 30 10:00:29 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1768 |
Keywords | competition performance soft song sexual selection individual differences |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c575t-cbe1ec59ef9e657f83820bb42fa14d7bedf5ffe17fa08f87f795764644dd76643 |
Notes | ark:/67375/V84-FWG7G39S-G ArticleID:rspb20131401 istex:734ECE6A83522954C74E2F167D5F92054ECC964B href:rspb20131401.pdf ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.1401 |
PMID | 23945685 |
PQID | 1426002531 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 1 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmed_primary_23945685 proquest_miscellaneous_1551646270 royalsociety_journals_10_1098_rspb_2013_1401 proquest_miscellaneous_1426002531 crossref_citationtrail_10_1098_rspb_2013_1401 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3757973 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2013_1401 istex_primary_ark_67375_V84_FWG7G39S_G |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2013-10-07 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2013-10-07 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 10 year: 2013 text: 2013-10-07 day: 07 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Proc. R. Soc. B |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Proc. R. Soc. B |
PublicationYear | 2013 |
Publisher | The Royal Society |
Publisher_xml | – name: The Royal Society |
References | Maynard-Smith J (e_1_3_3_5_2) 2003 Searcy W (e_1_3_3_4_2) 2005 e_1_3_3_17_2 e_1_3_3_16_2 e_1_3_3_19_2 e_1_3_3_38_2 e_1_3_3_18_2 e_1_3_3_39_2 e_1_3_3_13_2 e_1_3_3_36_2 e_1_3_3_12_2 e_1_3_3_37_2 e_1_3_3_15_2 e_1_3_3_34_2 e_1_3_3_14_2 e_1_3_3_35_2 e_1_3_3_32_2 e_1_3_3_33_2 e_1_3_3_11_2 e_1_3_3_30_2 e_1_3_3_10_2 e_1_3_3_31_2 e_1_3_3_40_2 e_1_3_3_6_2 e_1_3_3_8_2 e_1_3_3_7_2 e_1_3_3_28_2 e_1_3_3_9_2 e_1_3_3_27_2 e_1_3_3_29_2 e_1_3_3_24_2 e_1_3_3_23_2 e_1_3_3_26_2 e_1_3_3_25_2 e_1_3_3_2_2 e_1_3_3_20_2 e_1_3_3_43_2 e_1_3_3_22_2 e_1_3_3_41_2 e_1_3_3_3_2 e_1_3_3_21_2 e_1_3_3_42_2 |
References_xml | – ident: e_1_3_3_26_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01930.x – ident: e_1_3_3_30_2 doi: 10.1038/35051570 – ident: e_1_3_3_38_2 doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-0949-5 – ident: e_1_3_3_10_2 doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1847 – ident: e_1_3_3_21_2 doi: 10.1093/beheco/arg090 – ident: e_1_3_3_41_2 doi: 10.1126/science.296.5569.873 – ident: e_1_3_3_8_2 doi: 10.1007/s002650050216 – ident: e_1_3_3_12_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.07.017 – ident: e_1_3_3_27_2 doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1073-2 – ident: e_1_3_3_23_2 doi: 10.1111/j.2006.0908-8857.03839.x – ident: e_1_3_3_18_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb02441.x – ident: e_1_3_3_28_2 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0626 – ident: e_1_3_3_33_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.027 – ident: e_1_3_3_25_2 doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn021 – ident: e_1_3_3_29_2 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80143-8 – ident: e_1_3_3_17_2 doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0107 – ident: e_1_3_3_40_2 doi: 10.1038/332152a0 – ident: e_1_3_3_3_2 doi: 10.1515/9780691207278 – ident: e_1_3_3_34_2 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0127 – ident: e_1_3_3_16_2 doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(09)40005-6 – ident: e_1_3_3_9_2 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511754791 – ident: e_1_3_3_31_2 doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1140 – ident: e_1_3_3_22_2 doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq022 – ident: e_1_3_3_37_2 doi: 10.1086/596530 – ident: e_1_3_3_15_2 doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn135 – ident: e_1_3_3_6_2 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80110-8 – ident: e_1_3_3_20_2 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2192 – volume-title: Animal signals year: 2003 ident: e_1_3_3_5_2 – volume-title: The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signaling systems (monographs in behavior and ecology) year: 2005 ident: e_1_3_3_4_2 – ident: e_1_3_3_43_2 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80188-7 – ident: e_1_3_3_14_2 doi: 10.1016/B978-012473070-0/50005-0 – ident: e_1_3_3_35_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.035 – ident: e_1_3_3_39_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.009 – ident: e_1_3_3_2_2 doi: 10.1038/246015a0 – ident: e_1_3_3_36_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01154.x – ident: e_1_3_3_7_2 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80175-5 – ident: e_1_3_3_32_2 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.025 – ident: e_1_3_3_19_2 doi: 10.1242/jeb.071944 – ident: e_1_3_3_11_2 doi: 10.1007/s00265-006-0161-9 – ident: e_1_3_3_13_2 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2112 – ident: e_1_3_3_24_2 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3535 – ident: e_1_3_3_42_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1557-9263.2012.00377.x |
SSID | ssj0009585 |
Score | 2.3087687 |
Snippet | Mating signals of many animal species are difficult to produce and thus should indicate signaler quality. Growing evidence suggests that receivers modulate... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref royalsociety istex |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 20131401 |
SubjectTerms | Acoustic Stimulation Animals Competition Competitive Behavior Individual Differences Mating Preference, Animal Melospiza georgiana Sexual Selection Soft Song Sparrows - physiology Vocalization, Animal |
Title | Responses to song playback vary with the vocal performance of both signal senders and receivers |
URI | https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/V84-FWG7G39S-G/fulltext.pdf https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/full/10.1098/rspb.2013.1401 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23945685 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1426002531 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1551646270 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC3757973 |
Volume | 280 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1bb9MwFLbKJiReEBu3cJOREBeVhObq5BEm1gkxNEEHfbPsxGHVpqZq0onyX_ivnBMnXjpaNHiJqsRKE5_P5-J85xxCnsWM-V7mK9tTvm8HYR7agoWJjb4sFrgMvRQTnA8_RQfHwYdxOO71fnVYS4tKOunPtXkl_yNVOAdyxSzZf5CsuSmcgN8gXziChOF4JRl_1gRXXaWhxK5BszOxlCI97Z8jG05vsgIQztFiYYlikyQAPqIskJ0--Y7-aFl3lNP1mkEHqpqt0XVcj4yhK1tagd55aGifTv9dDZWJUaaNbTU--2GBjZ2WTV676H90DBlInFQTQ7Dv75kLRxAzd_PNulsUria7MQOq0eVH6u5CRqCQA13E1lFaB4O9tL1EV5lslbSn-z21aGS6F88f-n-QYE7DvJxJZO35DoaPF5au_bp_yQAaWqKYnyLPjYX8axzw_W9DNvSTL3x4jWx7EIXUueRjt1PTue74at7B1ASN36w-wYrPs43L98e6gGYNL3eO01bqWev4PaNb5GYTsNC3Gn07pKemu-S6lvNyl-w0xqGkL5sK5q9uE26ASauCIjBpC0yKwKQITAoYojUwaQeYtMgpApNqYNIGmBSASQ0w75Dj_fejvQO76eRhpxAOVHYqlavSMFF5oqKQ5bEPjqeUgZcLN8iYVFke5rlyWS4GcR6znCUQBwfgq2cZi8Bpvku2psVU3SdURKB0AlA9MsiCgUilhIg3SiM3wnlT0iJ2O9c8bcrcY7eVM67pFjFH2XCUDUfZWOSFGT_TBV42jnxei84M2wQXizxtZctBWeMXODFVxaKE22A_CA_s3l_G4KfrIPLYwCL3NB7MP3o-LIsoDi3CVpBiBmCx-NUr08lJXTQeHpMlzLfI6y6meKPNyg3v_OCq7_yQ3LhY-Y_IVjVfqMfgrlfySb1ofgNOze9l |
linkProvider | Geneva Foundation for Medical Education and Research |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Responses+to+song+playback+vary+with+the+vocal+performance+of+both+signal+senders+and+receivers&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+Royal+Society.+B%2C+Biological+sciences&rft.au=Moseley%2C+Dana+L.&rft.au=Lahti%2C+David+C.&rft.au=Podos%2C+Jeffrey&rft.date=2013-10-07&rft.pub=The+Royal+Society&rft.issn=0962-8452&rft.eissn=1471-2945&rft.volume=280&rft.issue=1768&rft_id=info:doi/10.1098%2Frspb.2013.1401&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=ark_67375_V84_FWG7G39S_G |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0962-8452&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0962-8452&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0962-8452&client=summon |