Experience and grammatical agreement: Statistical learning shapes number agreement production

A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inCognition Vol. 114; no. 2; pp. 151 - 164
Main Authors Haskell, Todd R., Thornton, Robert, MacDonald, Maryellen C.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Amsterdam Elsevier B.V 01.02.2010
Elsevier
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers’ past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed.
AbstractList A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed.
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. All rights reserved, Elsevier
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. [Copyright Elsevier B.V.]
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers’ past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed.
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed.A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed.
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as "the key to the cabinets", with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as "the keys to the cabinet", where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. (Contains 3 tables and 2 figures.)
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets , with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet , where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed.
Author Haskell, Todd R.
Thornton, Robert
MacDonald, Maryellen C.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Todd R.
  surname: Haskell
  fullname: Haskell, Todd R.
  email: todd.haskell@wwu.edu
  organization: Western Washington University, Bellingham, WA, United States
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Robert
  surname: Thornton
  fullname: Thornton, Robert
  organization: Pomona College, Claremont, CA, United States
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Maryellen C.
  surname: MacDonald
  fullname: MacDonald, Maryellen C.
  organization: University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
BackLink http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ869941$$DView record in ERIC
http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=22374484$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19942213$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkk2P0zAQhi20iO0u_AMEuQCnlLGd2A4SSKtV-dJKHIAjslxn0nWV2F07XcG_x9mW8nGguUTyPPP6Hc97Rk588EjIUwpzClS8XM9tWHk3uuDnDKCZg5oDlffIjCrJS6m4OiEzAAolMClPyVlKawComFQPyCltmooxymfk2-L7BqNDb7Ewvi1W0QyDGZ01fWFWEXFAP74qPo_5LO2OezTRO78q0rXZYCr8dlhi_E0XmxjarZ28PST3O9MnfLT_n5OvbxdfLt-XV5_efbi8uCptLelYcmTQKGStzKOI1naKgqmxsSDbxgq1BKSdwLqTVtimlaYWhonK8ApbYxTn5-TNTnezXQ7Y2uwiml5vohtM_KGDcfrvinfXehVuNVOUCaiywIu9QAw3W0yjHlyy2PfGY9gmraSsJaimPkrKKn-ipvQ4ybkSnN_d_vy_JJfZJwiRwSd_znkY8Nc-M_BsD5iUd9VF461LB44xnv2p6cbHOy7v3h7Ki49KZKXJ-utd2caQUsROWzclIEyv53pNQU8x1Gt9iKGeYqhB6RzD3C__6T84Pdp5sTeW43LrMOpk79LZuoh21G1wRzV-AuYT_Y4
CODEN CGTNAU
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2019_102916
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2014_07_003
crossref_primary_10_1177_1747021818769567
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0022226712000084
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0272263118000025
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_langsci_2017_12_008
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2022_104370
crossref_primary_10_3390_languages8020147
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11525_014_9243_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2015_09_011
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X17001212
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0142716421000357
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_05_004
crossref_primary_10_1515_phras_2012_0006
crossref_primary_10_1177_0023830913496052
crossref_primary_10_1162_opmi_a_00167
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_104161
crossref_primary_10_1080_01690965_2013_804193
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_00590
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2022_101067
crossref_primary_10_1080_2331186X_2018_1429134
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2021_03_029
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10936_019_09628_z
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11525_015_9259_y
crossref_primary_10_1515_flin_2018_0012
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10936_014_9291_9
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2014_00783
crossref_primary_10_1093_jos_ffy015
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_langsci_2016_05_003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2013_05_001
crossref_primary_10_1080_0163853X_2013_841074
crossref_primary_10_1080_02687038_2016_1140120
crossref_primary_10_1111_josl_12025
crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218221094568
crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2013_812017
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_00489
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2015_1136426
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10670_022_00657_0
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lingua_2014_01_013
crossref_primary_10_1515_cog_2017_0035
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_777225
crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2019_1621178
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2019_1631456
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2022_104322
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2023_104445
crossref_primary_10_16995_glossa_8504
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13079
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V.
2015 INIST-CNRS
Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Copyright_xml – notice: 2009 Elsevier B.V.
– notice: 2015 INIST-CNRS
– notice: Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
8BJ
FQK
JBE
7X8
7T9
7TK
5PM
DOI 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.017
DatabaseName CrossRef
ERIC
ERIC (Ovid)
ERIC
ERIC
ERIC (Legacy Platform)
ERIC( SilverPlatter )
ERIC
ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)
Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)
ERIC
Pascal-Francis
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
MEDLINE - Academic
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
Neurosciences Abstracts
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
ERIC
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
MEDLINE - Academic
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
Neurosciences Abstracts
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
Neurosciences Abstracts
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)

MEDLINE - Academic
ERIC

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: ERI
  name: ERIC
  url: https://eric.ed.gov/
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1873-7838
ERIC EJ869941
EndPage 164
ExternalDocumentID PMC2812604
19942213
22374484
EJ869941
10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017
S0010027709002121
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Journal Article
Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: AG025195
– fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: K01 AG025195
– fundername: NICHD NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 HD047425
– fundername: NICHD NIH HHS
  grantid: HD047425
GroupedDBID ---
--K
--M
--Z
-DZ
-~X
.~1
0R~
186
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29F
3EH
4.4
41~
457
4G.
53G
5GY
5VS
6J9
6PF
7-5
71M
8P~
9JM
9JO
AABNK
AACTN
AADFP
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFJI
AAGJA
AAGUQ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAWTL
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABLJU
ABMAC
ABMMH
ABOYX
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACIUM
ACKIV
ACNCT
ACPRK
ACRLP
ACXNI
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADIYS
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AETEA
AFFNX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AFYLN
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
AKYCK
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
AOMHK
ASPBG
AVARZ
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
CS3
DU5
EBS
EFJIC
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FD6
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
G8K
GBLVA
HF~
HMQ
HMW
HVGLF
HZ~
IHE
J1W
K-O
KOM
LPU
M2V
M3V
M41
MO0
MOBAO
MVM
N9A
NHB
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OHT
OKEIE
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
PRBVW
Q38
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SCC
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SNS
SPCBC
SPS
SSB
SSN
SSO
SSY
SSZ
T5K
TN5
UBW
UPT
UQL
WH7
WUQ
XFK
XIH
XJT
XKC
XOL
XPP
YYP
YZZ
ZA5
ZKB
ZMT
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABDPE
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADMHG
ADVLN
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
SSH
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
EFKBS
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
8BJ
FQK
JBE
7X8
7T9
7TK
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c571t-3e2098e2d70096dcf810a5e9c07d9c68b0e1f6e5f7c6c9d7a56a264a34edaa833
IEDL.DBID AIKHN
ISSN 0010-0277
1873-7838
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 14:10:15 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 01:15:46 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 04:04:12 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 05:38:27 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 12:44:06 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 06:01:11 EDT 2025
Wed Apr 02 07:16:21 EDT 2025
Tue Sep 02 19:23:33 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:29:07 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:03:56 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:18:54 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess false
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 2
Keywords Subject–verb agreement
Language production
Implicit learning
Human
Acquisition process
Subject-verb agreement
Language
Statistical learning
Cognition
Verbal production
Experimental study
Grammatical agreement
Language English
License https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0
CC BY 4.0
Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c571t-3e2098e2d70096dcf810a5e9c07d9c68b0e1f6e5f7c6c9d7a56a264a34edaa833
Notes ObjectType-Article-2
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 23
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
PMID 19942213
PQID 37281066
PQPubID 23473
PageCount 14
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2812604
proquest_miscellaneous_877570895
proquest_miscellaneous_744446511
proquest_miscellaneous_733863304
proquest_miscellaneous_37281066
pubmed_primary_19942213
pascalfrancis_primary_22374484
eric_primary_EJ869941
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2010-02-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2010-02-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 02
  year: 2010
  text: 2010-02-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace Amsterdam
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Amsterdam
– name: Netherlands
PublicationTitle Cognition
PublicationTitleAlternate Cognition
PublicationYear 2010
Publisher Elsevier B.V
Elsevier
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier B.V
– name: Elsevier
References Bock, Cutting (bib6) 1992; 31
Spalek, Schriefers (bib47) 2005; 52
Bock, Miller (bib12) 1991; 23
Hare, Elman, Daugherty (bib29) 1995; 10
Hartsuiker, Schriefers, Bock, Kikstra (bib32) 2003; 31
Miozzo, Caramazza (bib42) 1999; 25
Haskell, MacDonald (bib35) 2003; 48
Bock, Eberhard (bib8) 1993; 8
Barlow (bib2) 1992
Hartsuiker, Westenberg (bib33) 2000; 75
Ferreira, Bock (bib26) 2006; 21
Bock, Nicol, Cutting (bib13) 1999; 40
Branigan, Pickering, Cleland (bib14) 1999; 6
Branigan, Pickering, Cleland (bib15) 2000; 75
Schriefers (bib46) 1993; 19
Eberhard, Cutting, Bock (bib23) 2005; 112
Bock, Griffin (bib9) 2000; 129
Eberhard (bib22) 1997; 36
Dell, Reed, Adams, Meyer (bib21) 2000; 26
Ferreira (bib25) 2003; 48
Bock (bib4) 1986; 18
Mallinson, Blake (bib40) 1981
Alario, Caramazza (bib1) 2002; 82
Levelt, Roelofs, Meyer (bib39) 1999; 22
Haskell, MacDonald (bib36) 2005; 31
Chang (bib16) 2009; 61
Janssen, Caramazza (bib38) 2003; 48
Fayol, Largy, Lemaire (bib24) 1994; 47A
Franck, Vigliocco, Anton-Mendez, Collina, Frauenfelder (bib27) 2008; 23
Becker (bib3) 1979; 5
Mirkovic, J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2003).
Cleland, Pickering (bib18) 2003; 49
Corbett (bib19) 1983
Hartsuiker, Bernolet, Schoonbaert, Speybroeck, Vanderelst (bib30) 2008; 58
Marcus, Santorini, Marcinkiewicz (bib41) 1993; 19
Corbett (bib20) 1986; 24
Vigliocco, Zilli (bib52) 1999; 28
Haskell, T. R., Mansfield, C. D., & Brewer, K. M. (submitted for publication). Language and category learning: The case of subject–verb agreement.
Vigliocco, Butterworth, Garrett (bib49) 1996; 61
Bock, Loebell (bib11) 1990; 35
Poster presented at the 16th CUNY conference on human sentence processing, Boston, MA.
Bock, Levelt (bib10) 1994
Bock, Cutler, Eberhard, Butterfield, Cutting, Humphreys (bib5) 2006; 82
Peterson, Savoy (bib44) 1998; 24
Vigliocco, Butterworth, Semenza (bib50) 1995; 34
Pickering, Branigan (bib45) 1998; 39
Bock, Dell, Garnsey, Kramer, Kubose (bib7) 2007
Hartsuiker, Pickering, Veltkamp (bib31) 2004; 15
Haspelmath (bib37) 2006; 42
Thornton, MacDonald (bib48) 2003; 48
Franck, Vigliocco, Nicol (bib28) 2002; 17
Chang, Dell, Bock, Griffin (bib17) 2000; 29
Vigliocco, Nicol (bib51) 1998; 68
References_xml – volume: 21
  start-page: 1011
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1029
  ident: bib26
  article-title: The functions of structural priming
  publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes
– volume: 75
  start-page: B27
  year: 2000
  end-page: B39
  ident: bib33
  article-title: Word order priming in written and spoken sentence production
  publication-title: Cognition
– year: 1983
  ident: bib19
  article-title: Resolution rules: Agreement in person, number, and gender
  publication-title: Order concord and constituency
– volume: 42
  start-page: 25
  year: 2006
  end-page: 70
  ident: bib37
  article-title: Against markedness (and what to replace it with)
  publication-title: Journal of Linguistics
– volume: 40
  start-page: 330
  year: 1999
  end-page: 346
  ident: bib13
  article-title: The ties that bind: Creating number agreement in speech
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 61
  start-page: 261
  year: 1996
  end-page: 298
  ident: bib49
  article-title: Subject–verb agreement in Spanish and English: Differences in the role of conceptual constraints
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 10
  start-page: 601
  year: 1995
  end-page: 630
  ident: bib29
  article-title: Default generalisation in connectionist networks
  publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes
– volume: 19
  start-page: 841
  year: 1993
  end-page: 850
  ident: bib46
  article-title: Syntactic processes in the production of noun phrases
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
– volume: 75
  start-page: B13
  year: 2000
  end-page: B25
  ident: bib15
  article-title: Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 68
  start-page: B13
  year: 1998
  end-page: B29
  ident: bib51
  article-title: Separating hierarchial relations and word order in language production: Is proximity concord syntactic or linear?
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 19
  start-page: 313
  year: 1993
  end-page: 330
  ident: bib41
  article-title: Building a large annotated corpus of English: The Penn Treebank
  publication-title: Computational Linguistics
– volume: 48
  start-page: 740
  year: 2003
  end-page: 759
  ident: bib48
  article-title: Plausibility and grammatical agreement
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 23
  start-page: 45
  year: 1991
  end-page: 93
  ident: bib12
  article-title: Broken agreement
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 29
  start-page: 217
  year: 2000
  end-page: 229
  ident: bib17
  article-title: Structural priming as implicit learning: A comparison of models of sentence production
  publication-title: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research
– volume: 24
  start-page: 995
  year: 1986
  end-page: 1023
  ident: bib20
  article-title: Agreement: A partial specification, based on Slavonic data
  publication-title: Linguistics
– volume: 39
  start-page: 633
  year: 1998
  end-page: 651
  ident: bib45
  article-title: The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– year: 1981
  ident: bib40
  article-title: Language typology: Cross-linguistic studies in syntax
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1
  year: 1999
  end-page: 75
  ident: bib39
  article-title: A theory of lexical access in speech production
  publication-title: Behavioral and Brain Sciences
– volume: 8
  start-page: 57
  year: 1993
  end-page: 99
  ident: bib8
  article-title: Meaning, sound, and syntax in English number agreement
  publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes
– volume: 18
  start-page: 355
  year: 1986
  end-page: 387
  ident: bib4
  article-title: Syntactic persistence in language production
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 58
  start-page: 214
  year: 2008
  end-page: 238
  ident: bib30
  article-title: Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– reference: Mirkovic, J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2003).
– volume: 15
  start-page: 409
  year: 2004
  end-page: 414
  ident: bib31
  article-title: Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 82
  start-page: 64
  year: 2006
  end-page: 113
  ident: bib5
  article-title: Number agreement in British and American English: Disagreeing to agree collectively
  publication-title: Language
– volume: 52
  start-page: 103
  year: 2005
  end-page: 119
  ident: bib47
  article-title: Dominance affects determiner selection in language production
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 28
  start-page: 623
  year: 1999
  end-page: 648
  ident: bib52
  article-title: Syntactic accuracy in sentence production: The case of gender disagreement in Italian language-impaired and unimpaired speakers
  publication-title: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research
– volume: 82
  start-page: 179
  year: 2002
  end-page: 223
  ident: bib1
  article-title: The production of determiners: Evidence from French
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 48
  start-page: 379
  year: 2003
  end-page: 398
  ident: bib25
  article-title: The persistence of optional complementizer production: Why saying ‘that’ is not saying ‘that’ at all
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– year: 1994
  ident: bib10
  article-title: Language production: Grammatical encoding
  publication-title: Handbook of psycholinguistics
– volume: 17
  start-page: 371
  year: 2002
  end-page: 404
  ident: bib28
  article-title: Subject–verb agreement errors in French and English: The role of syntactic hierarchy
  publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes
– volume: 5
  start-page: 252
  year: 1979
  end-page: 259
  ident: bib3
  article-title: Semantic context and word frequency effects in visual word recognition
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 61
  start-page: 374
  year: 2009
  end-page: 397
  ident: bib16
  article-title: Learning to order words: A connectionist model of heavy NP shift and accessibility effects in Japanese and English
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– reference: Haskell, T. R., Mansfield, C. D., & Brewer, K. M. (submitted for publication). Language and category learning: The case of subject–verb agreement.
– volume: 31
  start-page: 891
  year: 2005
  end-page: 904
  ident: bib36
  article-title: Constituent structure and linear order in language production: Evidence from subject–verb agreement
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
– volume: 48
  start-page: 635
  year: 2003
  end-page: 652
  ident: bib38
  article-title: The selection of closed-class words in noun phrase production: The case of Dutch determiners
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– year: 1992
  ident: bib2
  article-title: A situated theory of agreement
– volume: 23
  start-page: 329
  year: 2008
  end-page: 374
  ident: bib27
  article-title: The interplay of syntax and form in sentence production: A cross-linguistic study of form effects on agreement
  publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes
– volume: 129
  start-page: 177
  year: 2000
  end-page: 192
  ident: bib9
  article-title: The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 6
  start-page: 635
  year: 1999
  end-page: 640
  ident: bib14
  article-title: Syntactic priming in written production: Evidence for rapid decay
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review
– reference: . Poster presented at the 16th CUNY conference on human sentence processing, Boston, MA.
– volume: 31
  start-page: 99
  year: 1992
  end-page: 127
  ident: bib6
  article-title: Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 31
  start-page: 1316
  year: 2003
  end-page: 1326
  ident: bib32
  article-title: Morphophonological influences on the construction of subject–verb agreement
  publication-title: Memory and Cognition
– volume: 48
  start-page: 760
  year: 2003
  end-page: 778
  ident: bib35
  article-title: Conflicting cues and competition in subject–verb agreement
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 36
  start-page: 147
  year: 1997
  end-page: 164
  ident: bib22
  article-title: The marked effect of number on subject–verb agreement
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 49
  start-page: 214
  year: 2003
  end-page: 230
  ident: bib18
  article-title: The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun–phrase structure
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 26
  start-page: 1355
  year: 2000
  end-page: 1367
  ident: bib21
  article-title: Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: A study of the role of experience in language production
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
– volume: 47A
  start-page: 437
  year: 1994
  end-page: 464
  ident: bib24
  article-title: Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subject–verb agreement errors: A study in French written language
  publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 112
  start-page: 531
  year: 2005
  end-page: 559
  ident: bib23
  article-title: Making syntax of sense: Number agreement in sentence production
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– start-page: 21
  year: 2007
  end-page: 42
  ident: bib7
  article-title: Car talk, car listen
  publication-title: Automaticity and control in language processing. Advances in behavioural brain science
– volume: 34
  start-page: 186
  year: 1995
  end-page: 215
  ident: bib50
  article-title: Constructing subject–verb agreement in speech: The role of semantic and morphological factors
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 35
  start-page: 1
  year: 1990
  end-page: 39
  ident: bib11
  article-title: Framing sentences
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 24
  start-page: 539
  year: 1998
  end-page: 557
  ident: bib44
  article-title: Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
– volume: 25
  start-page: 907
  year: 1999
  end-page: 922
  ident: bib42
  article-title: The selection of determiners in noun phrase production
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
SSID ssj0004278
Score 2.2231424
Snippet A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the...
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as "the...
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
eric
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 151
SubjectTerms Agreement
Asymmetry
Biological and medical sciences
Computational Linguistics
Corpus Analysis
Cues
Error
Error Analysis
Error Patterns
Female
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Grammar
Human
Humans
Implicit learning
Language
Language Processing
Language production
Language Research
Language use
Learning
Learning - physiology
Learning Processes
Learning. Memory
Male
Miscellaneous
Nouns
Number
Phrase Structure
Priming
Psycholinguistics
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Sentences
Speech Production
Statistical analysis
Structural Analysis (Linguistics)
Subject–verb agreement
Syntactic Features
Syntactic Structures
Verbal Behavior
Verbs
Vocabulary
Young Adult
Title Experience and grammatical agreement: Statistical learning shapes number agreement production
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.017
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ869941
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19942213
https://www.proquest.com/docview/37281066
https://www.proquest.com/docview/733863304
https://www.proquest.com/docview/744446511
https://www.proquest.com/docview/877570895
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC2812604
Volume 114
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1Lb9QwEB6V9lIJIR4FwmPJgWtaO373VlWtliJ6olIvKLIdhy5C2YjdHrj0t9d2nCxBLT1wTcZKPB6PZ-zP3wB8rG1tWGNJ4agpC0q1KQwlTcG1ZqZhDXaR8ebLOZ9f0LNLdrkFx8NdmACrTL6_9-nRW6cnB0mbB91iEe74BvpQIZCKPOU-BdopieLetHeOPn2en2-uR5aDQ0bxyHIC80oonWWbuCvlPorFy-5cpBIk-nGnV16DTV_64q7Y9G-I5R9r1ulTeJKCzfyo788z2HLtc9gdfd7vF_BtQ3Wc67bOA1Yrkrj6Ztqn4nHz8DAPIWlkdPaPU52J7_nqSndulfc1RTbSedeTyPrO7sHF6cnX43mRKi4Ulgm8LogrkZKurEVIbWrbSIw0c8oiUSvLpUEON9yxRlhuVS0049pHVJpQV2stCXkJ2-2yda_DXXBjDeK2dMhHXFhJbZnTwtRWKmcFyoAPKq5soiMPVTF-VgPu7Ec1jk0olqmqUC8TiwzQ2LDrGTkebnI4jGE1Ma7KrxsPN94Loz5-6-RMcqUozmA2MYNRwAdcwie9NIMPg11Ufs6GgxjduuX1qiKi9IrlPIP8HglBiORhq-kfIpQGsjuM7xeRQjCBpGIZvOrNcaMx34WyxCQDMTHUUSDQjk_ftIurSD_u_90nwfTN_yj1Lez2MIyAC3oH2-tf1-69j-7WZgaP9m_wLM3hWdzPuAUy_VXz
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1LT9wwEB4heihSVfVB2_QBOfQasOM3twqBthQ4gcSlimzHKYuqbNRdDr30t9d2nGxTQTn0moyVeDwez9ifvwH4WNvasMaSwlFTFpRqUxhKmoJrzUzDGuwi483ZOZ9d0pMrdrUBh8NdmACrTL6_9-nRW6cn-0mb-918Hu74BvpQIZCKPOU-BXpE_fQNs3Pv1xrnEWpJ9O4YxQPLCcgrYXQWbWKulHsoli67c4lKgOgnnV56_TV94Yu7ItO_AZZ_rFjHz-BpCjXzT31vnsOGa1_A1ujxfr6Er2ui41y3dR6QWpHC1TfTPhGPW4cHeQhII5-zf5yqTHzLl9e6c8u8ryiyls67nkLWd3YbLo-PLg5nRaq3UFgm8KogrkRKurIWIbGpbSMx0swpi0StLJcGOdxwxxphuVW10IxrH09pQl2ttSTkFWy2i9a9CTfBjTWI29IhH29hJbVlTgtTW6mcFSgDPqi4somMPNTE-F4NqLObahybUCpTVaFaJhYZoLFh1_NxPNzkYBjDamJalV81Hm68HUZ9_NbRieRKUZzBzsQMRgEfbgmf8tIMdge7qPyMDccwunWL22VFROkVy3kG-T0SghDJw0bTP0QoDVR3GN8vIoVgAknFMnjdm-NaY74LZYlJBmJiqKNAIB2fvmnn15F83P-7T4Hp2_9R6i48nl2cnVann8-_vIOtHpAREELvYXP149Z98HHeyuzEefwbEGNVtg
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Experience+and+grammatical+agreement%3A+Statistical+learning+shapes+number+agreement+production&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=HASKELL%2C+Todd+R&rft.au=THORNTON%2C+Robert&rft.au=MACDONALD%2C+Maryellen+C&rft.date=2010-02-01&rft.pub=Elsevier&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.volume=114&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=151&rft.epage=164&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.cognition.2009.08.017&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=22374484
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon