Experience and grammatical agreement: Statistical learning shapes number agreement production
A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is...
Saved in:
Published in | Cognition Vol. 114; no. 2; pp. 151 - 164 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Amsterdam
Elsevier B.V
01.02.2010
Elsevier |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as
the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as
the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers’ past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. |
---|---|
AbstractList | A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. All rights reserved, Elsevier A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. [Copyright Elsevier B.V.] A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers’ past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed.A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets, with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet, where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as "the key to the cabinets", with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as "the keys to the cabinet", where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. (Contains 3 tables and 2 figures.) A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the key to the cabinets , with a singular noun followed by a plural one, than with phrases such as the keys to the cabinet , where a plural noun is followed by a singular. These asymmetries are thought to reflect core language production processes. Previous accounts have attributed error patterns to a syntactic number feature present on plurals but not singulars. An alternative approach is presented in which a process similar to structural priming contributes to the error asymmetry via speakers' past experiences with related agreement constructions. A corpus analysis and two agreement production studies test this account. The results suggest that agreement production is shaped by statistical learning from past language experience. Implications for accounts of agreement are discussed. |
Author | Haskell, Todd R. Thornton, Robert MacDonald, Maryellen C. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Todd R. surname: Haskell fullname: Haskell, Todd R. email: todd.haskell@wwu.edu organization: Western Washington University, Bellingham, WA, United States – sequence: 2 givenname: Robert surname: Thornton fullname: Thornton, Robert organization: Pomona College, Claremont, CA, United States – sequence: 3 givenname: Maryellen C. surname: MacDonald fullname: MacDonald, Maryellen C. organization: University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States |
BackLink | http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ869941$$DView record in ERIC http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=22374484$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19942213$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkk2P0zAQhi20iO0u_AMEuQCnlLGd2A4SSKtV-dJKHIAjslxn0nWV2F07XcG_x9mW8nGguUTyPPP6Hc97Rk588EjIUwpzClS8XM9tWHk3uuDnDKCZg5oDlffIjCrJS6m4OiEzAAolMClPyVlKawComFQPyCltmooxymfk2-L7BqNDb7Ewvi1W0QyDGZ01fWFWEXFAP74qPo_5LO2OezTRO78q0rXZYCr8dlhi_E0XmxjarZ28PST3O9MnfLT_n5OvbxdfLt-XV5_efbi8uCptLelYcmTQKGStzKOI1naKgqmxsSDbxgq1BKSdwLqTVtimlaYWhonK8ApbYxTn5-TNTnezXQ7Y2uwiml5vohtM_KGDcfrvinfXehVuNVOUCaiywIu9QAw3W0yjHlyy2PfGY9gmraSsJaimPkrKKn-ipvQ4ybkSnN_d_vy_JJfZJwiRwSd_znkY8Nc-M_BsD5iUd9VF461LB44xnv2p6cbHOy7v3h7Ki49KZKXJ-utd2caQUsROWzclIEyv53pNQU8x1Gt9iKGeYqhB6RzD3C__6T84Pdp5sTeW43LrMOpk79LZuoh21G1wRzV-AuYT_Y4 |
CODEN | CGTNAU |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2019_102916 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2014_07_003 crossref_primary_10_1177_1747021818769567 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0022226712000084 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0272263118000025 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_langsci_2017_12_008 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2022_104370 crossref_primary_10_3390_languages8020147 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11525_014_9243_y crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2015_09_011 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X17001212 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0142716421000357 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_05_004 crossref_primary_10_1515_phras_2012_0006 crossref_primary_10_1177_0023830913496052 crossref_primary_10_1162_opmi_a_00167 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_104161 crossref_primary_10_1080_01690965_2013_804193 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_00590 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2022_101067 crossref_primary_10_1080_2331186X_2018_1429134 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2021_03_029 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10936_019_09628_z crossref_primary_10_1007_s11525_015_9259_y crossref_primary_10_1515_flin_2018_0012 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10936_014_9291_9 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2014_00783 crossref_primary_10_1093_jos_ffy015 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_langsci_2016_05_003 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2013_05_001 crossref_primary_10_1080_0163853X_2013_841074 crossref_primary_10_1080_02687038_2016_1140120 crossref_primary_10_1111_josl_12025 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218221094568 crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2013_812017 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_00489 crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2015_1136426 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10670_022_00657_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lingua_2014_01_013 crossref_primary_10_1515_cog_2017_0035 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_777225 crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2019_1621178 crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2019_1631456 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2022_104322 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2023_104445 crossref_primary_10_16995_glossa_8504 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13079 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2009 Elsevier B.V. 2015 INIST-CNRS Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2009 Elsevier B.V. – notice: 2015 INIST-CNRS – notice: Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 8BJ FQK JBE 7X8 7T9 7TK 5PM |
DOI | 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.017 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ERIC ERIC (Ovid) ERIC ERIC ERIC (Legacy Platform) ERIC( SilverPlatter ) ERIC ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform) Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) ERIC Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) International Bibliography of the Social Sciences International Bibliography of the Social Sciences MEDLINE - Academic Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) Neurosciences Abstracts PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef ERIC MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) MEDLINE - Academic Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) Neurosciences Abstracts |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE Neurosciences Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) MEDLINE - Academic ERIC |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: ERI name: ERIC url: https://eric.ed.gov/ sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1873-7838 |
ERIC | EJ869941 |
EndPage | 164 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC2812604 19942213 22374484 EJ869941 10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017 S0010027709002121 |
Genre | Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIA NIH HHS grantid: AG025195 – fundername: NIA NIH HHS grantid: K01 AG025195 – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS grantid: R01 HD047425 – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS grantid: HD047425 |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M --Z -DZ -~X .~1 0R~ 186 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29F 3EH 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5VS 6J9 6PF 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFJI AAGJA AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAWTL AAXLA AAXUO ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABLJU ABMAC ABMMH ABOYX ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACIUM ACKIV ACNCT ACPRK ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AETEA AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV AKYCK ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ AOMHK ASPBG AVARZ AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CS3 DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FD6 FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q G8K GBLVA HF~ HMQ HMW HVGLF HZ~ IHE J1W K-O KOM LPU M2V M3V M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A NHB O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OKEIE OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. PRBVW Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSO SSY SSZ T5K TN5 UBW UPT UQL WH7 WUQ XFK XIH XJT XKC XOL XPP YYP YZZ ZA5 ZKB ZMT ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABDPE ACVFH ADCNI ADMHG ADVLN AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO EFKBS ERI PET REK WWN IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 8BJ FQK JBE 7X8 7T9 7TK 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c571t-3e2098e2d70096dcf810a5e9c07d9c68b0e1f6e5f7c6c9d7a56a264a34edaa833 |
IEDL.DBID | AIKHN |
ISSN | 0010-0277 1873-7838 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 14:10:15 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 01:15:46 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 04:04:12 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 05:38:27 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 12:44:06 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:01:11 EDT 2025 Wed Apr 02 07:16:21 EDT 2025 Tue Sep 02 19:23:33 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:29:07 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:03:56 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:18:54 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | false |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 2 |
Keywords | Subject–verb agreement Language production Implicit learning Human Acquisition process Subject-verb agreement Language Statistical learning Cognition Verbal production Experimental study Grammatical agreement |
Language | English |
License | https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0 CC BY 4.0 Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c571t-3e2098e2d70096dcf810a5e9c07d9c68b0e1f6e5f7c6c9d7a56a264a34edaa833 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 |
PMID | 19942213 |
PQID | 37281066 |
PQPubID | 23473 |
PageCount | 14 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2812604 proquest_miscellaneous_877570895 proquest_miscellaneous_744446511 proquest_miscellaneous_733863304 proquest_miscellaneous_37281066 pubmed_primary_19942213 pascalfrancis_primary_22374484 eric_primary_EJ869941 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_cognition_2009_08_017 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2010-02-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2010-02-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2010 text: 2010-02-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Amsterdam |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Amsterdam – name: Netherlands |
PublicationTitle | Cognition |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Cognition |
PublicationYear | 2010 |
Publisher | Elsevier B.V Elsevier |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier B.V – name: Elsevier |
References | Bock, Cutting (bib6) 1992; 31 Spalek, Schriefers (bib47) 2005; 52 Bock, Miller (bib12) 1991; 23 Hare, Elman, Daugherty (bib29) 1995; 10 Hartsuiker, Schriefers, Bock, Kikstra (bib32) 2003; 31 Miozzo, Caramazza (bib42) 1999; 25 Haskell, MacDonald (bib35) 2003; 48 Bock, Eberhard (bib8) 1993; 8 Barlow (bib2) 1992 Hartsuiker, Westenberg (bib33) 2000; 75 Ferreira, Bock (bib26) 2006; 21 Bock, Nicol, Cutting (bib13) 1999; 40 Branigan, Pickering, Cleland (bib14) 1999; 6 Branigan, Pickering, Cleland (bib15) 2000; 75 Schriefers (bib46) 1993; 19 Eberhard, Cutting, Bock (bib23) 2005; 112 Bock, Griffin (bib9) 2000; 129 Eberhard (bib22) 1997; 36 Dell, Reed, Adams, Meyer (bib21) 2000; 26 Ferreira (bib25) 2003; 48 Bock (bib4) 1986; 18 Mallinson, Blake (bib40) 1981 Alario, Caramazza (bib1) 2002; 82 Levelt, Roelofs, Meyer (bib39) 1999; 22 Haskell, MacDonald (bib36) 2005; 31 Chang (bib16) 2009; 61 Janssen, Caramazza (bib38) 2003; 48 Fayol, Largy, Lemaire (bib24) 1994; 47A Franck, Vigliocco, Anton-Mendez, Collina, Frauenfelder (bib27) 2008; 23 Becker (bib3) 1979; 5 Mirkovic, J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2003). Cleland, Pickering (bib18) 2003; 49 Corbett (bib19) 1983 Hartsuiker, Bernolet, Schoonbaert, Speybroeck, Vanderelst (bib30) 2008; 58 Marcus, Santorini, Marcinkiewicz (bib41) 1993; 19 Corbett (bib20) 1986; 24 Vigliocco, Zilli (bib52) 1999; 28 Haskell, T. R., Mansfield, C. D., & Brewer, K. M. (submitted for publication). Language and category learning: The case of subject–verb agreement. Vigliocco, Butterworth, Garrett (bib49) 1996; 61 Bock, Loebell (bib11) 1990; 35 Poster presented at the 16th CUNY conference on human sentence processing, Boston, MA. Bock, Levelt (bib10) 1994 Bock, Cutler, Eberhard, Butterfield, Cutting, Humphreys (bib5) 2006; 82 Peterson, Savoy (bib44) 1998; 24 Vigliocco, Butterworth, Semenza (bib50) 1995; 34 Pickering, Branigan (bib45) 1998; 39 Bock, Dell, Garnsey, Kramer, Kubose (bib7) 2007 Hartsuiker, Pickering, Veltkamp (bib31) 2004; 15 Haspelmath (bib37) 2006; 42 Thornton, MacDonald (bib48) 2003; 48 Franck, Vigliocco, Nicol (bib28) 2002; 17 Chang, Dell, Bock, Griffin (bib17) 2000; 29 Vigliocco, Nicol (bib51) 1998; 68 |
References_xml | – volume: 21 start-page: 1011 year: 2006 end-page: 1029 ident: bib26 article-title: The functions of structural priming publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes – volume: 75 start-page: B27 year: 2000 end-page: B39 ident: bib33 article-title: Word order priming in written and spoken sentence production publication-title: Cognition – year: 1983 ident: bib19 article-title: Resolution rules: Agreement in person, number, and gender publication-title: Order concord and constituency – volume: 42 start-page: 25 year: 2006 end-page: 70 ident: bib37 article-title: Against markedness (and what to replace it with) publication-title: Journal of Linguistics – volume: 40 start-page: 330 year: 1999 end-page: 346 ident: bib13 article-title: The ties that bind: Creating number agreement in speech publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 61 start-page: 261 year: 1996 end-page: 298 ident: bib49 article-title: Subject–verb agreement in Spanish and English: Differences in the role of conceptual constraints publication-title: Cognition – volume: 10 start-page: 601 year: 1995 end-page: 630 ident: bib29 article-title: Default generalisation in connectionist networks publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes – volume: 19 start-page: 841 year: 1993 end-page: 850 ident: bib46 article-title: Syntactic processes in the production of noun phrases publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition – volume: 75 start-page: B13 year: 2000 end-page: B25 ident: bib15 article-title: Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue publication-title: Cognition – volume: 68 start-page: B13 year: 1998 end-page: B29 ident: bib51 article-title: Separating hierarchial relations and word order in language production: Is proximity concord syntactic or linear? publication-title: Cognition – volume: 19 start-page: 313 year: 1993 end-page: 330 ident: bib41 article-title: Building a large annotated corpus of English: The Penn Treebank publication-title: Computational Linguistics – volume: 48 start-page: 740 year: 2003 end-page: 759 ident: bib48 article-title: Plausibility and grammatical agreement publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 23 start-page: 45 year: 1991 end-page: 93 ident: bib12 article-title: Broken agreement publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 29 start-page: 217 year: 2000 end-page: 229 ident: bib17 article-title: Structural priming as implicit learning: A comparison of models of sentence production publication-title: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research – volume: 24 start-page: 995 year: 1986 end-page: 1023 ident: bib20 article-title: Agreement: A partial specification, based on Slavonic data publication-title: Linguistics – volume: 39 start-page: 633 year: 1998 end-page: 651 ident: bib45 article-title: The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – year: 1981 ident: bib40 article-title: Language typology: Cross-linguistic studies in syntax – volume: 22 start-page: 1 year: 1999 end-page: 75 ident: bib39 article-title: A theory of lexical access in speech production publication-title: Behavioral and Brain Sciences – volume: 8 start-page: 57 year: 1993 end-page: 99 ident: bib8 article-title: Meaning, sound, and syntax in English number agreement publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes – volume: 18 start-page: 355 year: 1986 end-page: 387 ident: bib4 article-title: Syntactic persistence in language production publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 58 start-page: 214 year: 2008 end-page: 238 ident: bib30 article-title: Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – reference: Mirkovic, J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2003). – volume: 15 start-page: 409 year: 2004 end-page: 414 ident: bib31 article-title: Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 82 start-page: 64 year: 2006 end-page: 113 ident: bib5 article-title: Number agreement in British and American English: Disagreeing to agree collectively publication-title: Language – volume: 52 start-page: 103 year: 2005 end-page: 119 ident: bib47 article-title: Dominance affects determiner selection in language production publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 28 start-page: 623 year: 1999 end-page: 648 ident: bib52 article-title: Syntactic accuracy in sentence production: The case of gender disagreement in Italian language-impaired and unimpaired speakers publication-title: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research – volume: 82 start-page: 179 year: 2002 end-page: 223 ident: bib1 article-title: The production of determiners: Evidence from French publication-title: Cognition – volume: 48 start-page: 379 year: 2003 end-page: 398 ident: bib25 article-title: The persistence of optional complementizer production: Why saying ‘that’ is not saying ‘that’ at all publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – year: 1994 ident: bib10 article-title: Language production: Grammatical encoding publication-title: Handbook of psycholinguistics – volume: 17 start-page: 371 year: 2002 end-page: 404 ident: bib28 article-title: Subject–verb agreement errors in French and English: The role of syntactic hierarchy publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes – volume: 5 start-page: 252 year: 1979 end-page: 259 ident: bib3 article-title: Semantic context and word frequency effects in visual word recognition publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 61 start-page: 374 year: 2009 end-page: 397 ident: bib16 article-title: Learning to order words: A connectionist model of heavy NP shift and accessibility effects in Japanese and English publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – reference: Haskell, T. R., Mansfield, C. D., & Brewer, K. M. (submitted for publication). Language and category learning: The case of subject–verb agreement. – volume: 31 start-page: 891 year: 2005 end-page: 904 ident: bib36 article-title: Constituent structure and linear order in language production: Evidence from subject–verb agreement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 48 start-page: 635 year: 2003 end-page: 652 ident: bib38 article-title: The selection of closed-class words in noun phrase production: The case of Dutch determiners publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – year: 1992 ident: bib2 article-title: A situated theory of agreement – volume: 23 start-page: 329 year: 2008 end-page: 374 ident: bib27 article-title: The interplay of syntax and form in sentence production: A cross-linguistic study of form effects on agreement publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes – volume: 129 start-page: 177 year: 2000 end-page: 192 ident: bib9 article-title: The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 6 start-page: 635 year: 1999 end-page: 640 ident: bib14 article-title: Syntactic priming in written production: Evidence for rapid decay publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review – reference: . Poster presented at the 16th CUNY conference on human sentence processing, Boston, MA. – volume: 31 start-page: 99 year: 1992 end-page: 127 ident: bib6 article-title: Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 31 start-page: 1316 year: 2003 end-page: 1326 ident: bib32 article-title: Morphophonological influences on the construction of subject–verb agreement publication-title: Memory and Cognition – volume: 48 start-page: 760 year: 2003 end-page: 778 ident: bib35 article-title: Conflicting cues and competition in subject–verb agreement publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 36 start-page: 147 year: 1997 end-page: 164 ident: bib22 article-title: The marked effect of number on subject–verb agreement publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 49 start-page: 214 year: 2003 end-page: 230 ident: bib18 article-title: The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun–phrase structure publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 26 start-page: 1355 year: 2000 end-page: 1367 ident: bib21 article-title: Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: A study of the role of experience in language production publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition – volume: 47A start-page: 437 year: 1994 end-page: 464 ident: bib24 article-title: Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subject–verb agreement errors: A study in French written language publication-title: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 112 start-page: 531 year: 2005 end-page: 559 ident: bib23 article-title: Making syntax of sense: Number agreement in sentence production publication-title: Psychological Review – start-page: 21 year: 2007 end-page: 42 ident: bib7 article-title: Car talk, car listen publication-title: Automaticity and control in language processing. Advances in behavioural brain science – volume: 34 start-page: 186 year: 1995 end-page: 215 ident: bib50 article-title: Constructing subject–verb agreement in speech: The role of semantic and morphological factors publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 35 start-page: 1 year: 1990 end-page: 39 ident: bib11 article-title: Framing sentences publication-title: Cognition – volume: 24 start-page: 539 year: 1998 end-page: 557 ident: bib44 article-title: Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition – volume: 25 start-page: 907 year: 1999 end-page: 922 ident: bib42 article-title: The selection of determiners in noun phrase production publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition |
SSID | ssj0004278 |
Score | 2.2231424 |
Snippet | A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as
the... A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as "the... A robust result in research on the production of grammatical agreement is that speakers are more likely to produce an erroneous verb with phrases such as the... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed pascalfrancis eric crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 151 |
SubjectTerms | Agreement Asymmetry Biological and medical sciences Computational Linguistics Corpus Analysis Cues Error Error Analysis Error Patterns Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Grammar Human Humans Implicit learning Language Language Processing Language production Language Research Language use Learning Learning - physiology Learning Processes Learning. Memory Male Miscellaneous Nouns Number Phrase Structure Priming Psycholinguistics Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Psychomotor Performance - physiology Sentences Speech Production Statistical analysis Structural Analysis (Linguistics) Subject–verb agreement Syntactic Features Syntactic Structures Verbal Behavior Verbs Vocabulary Young Adult |
Title | Experience and grammatical agreement: Statistical learning shapes number agreement production |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.017 http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ869941 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19942213 https://www.proquest.com/docview/37281066 https://www.proquest.com/docview/733863304 https://www.proquest.com/docview/744446511 https://www.proquest.com/docview/877570895 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC2812604 |
Volume | 114 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1Lb9QwEB6V9lIJIR4FwmPJgWtaO373VlWtliJ6olIvKLIdhy5C2YjdHrj0t9d2nCxBLT1wTcZKPB6PZ-zP3wB8rG1tWGNJ4agpC0q1KQwlTcG1ZqZhDXaR8ebLOZ9f0LNLdrkFx8NdmACrTL6_9-nRW6cnB0mbB91iEe74BvpQIZCKPOU-BdopieLetHeOPn2en2-uR5aDQ0bxyHIC80oonWWbuCvlPorFy-5cpBIk-nGnV16DTV_64q7Y9G-I5R9r1ulTeJKCzfyo788z2HLtc9gdfd7vF_BtQ3Wc67bOA1Yrkrj6Ztqn4nHz8DAPIWlkdPaPU52J7_nqSndulfc1RTbSedeTyPrO7sHF6cnX43mRKi4Ulgm8LogrkZKurEVIbWrbSIw0c8oiUSvLpUEON9yxRlhuVS0049pHVJpQV2stCXkJ2-2yda_DXXBjDeK2dMhHXFhJbZnTwtRWKmcFyoAPKq5soiMPVTF-VgPu7Ec1jk0olqmqUC8TiwzQ2LDrGTkebnI4jGE1Ma7KrxsPN94Loz5-6-RMcqUozmA2MYNRwAdcwie9NIMPg11Ufs6GgxjduuX1qiKi9IrlPIP8HglBiORhq-kfIpQGsjuM7xeRQjCBpGIZvOrNcaMx34WyxCQDMTHUUSDQjk_ftIurSD_u_90nwfTN_yj1Lez2MIyAC3oH2-tf1-69j-7WZgaP9m_wLM3hWdzPuAUy_VXz |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1LT9wwEB4heihSVfVB2_QBOfQasOM3twqBthQ4gcSlimzHKYuqbNRdDr30t9d2nGxTQTn0moyVeDwez9ifvwH4WNvasMaSwlFTFpRqUxhKmoJrzUzDGuwi483ZOZ9d0pMrdrUBh8NdmACrTL6_9-nRW6cn-0mb-918Hu74BvpQIZCKPOU-BXpE_fQNs3Pv1xrnEWpJ9O4YxQPLCcgrYXQWbWKulHsoli67c4lKgOgnnV56_TV94Yu7ItO_AZZ_rFjHz-BpCjXzT31vnsOGa1_A1ujxfr6Er2ui41y3dR6QWpHC1TfTPhGPW4cHeQhII5-zf5yqTHzLl9e6c8u8ryiyls67nkLWd3YbLo-PLg5nRaq3UFgm8KogrkRKurIWIbGpbSMx0swpi0StLJcGOdxwxxphuVW10IxrH09pQl2ttSTkFWy2i9a9CTfBjTWI29IhH29hJbVlTgtTW6mcFSgDPqi4somMPNTE-F4NqLObahybUCpTVaFaJhYZoLFh1_NxPNzkYBjDamJalV81Hm68HUZ9_NbRieRKUZzBzsQMRgEfbgmf8tIMdge7qPyMDccwunWL22VFROkVy3kG-T0SghDJw0bTP0QoDVR3GN8vIoVgAknFMnjdm-NaY74LZYlJBmJiqKNAIB2fvmnn15F83P-7T4Hp2_9R6i48nl2cnVann8-_vIOtHpAREELvYXP149Z98HHeyuzEefwbEGNVtg |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Experience+and+grammatical+agreement%3A+Statistical+learning+shapes+number+agreement+production&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=HASKELL%2C+Todd+R&rft.au=THORNTON%2C+Robert&rft.au=MACDONALD%2C+Maryellen+C&rft.date=2010-02-01&rft.pub=Elsevier&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.volume=114&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=151&rft.epage=164&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.cognition.2009.08.017&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=22374484 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |