Testing the Dynamic Field Theory: Working Memory for Locations Becomes More Spatially Precise Over Development

The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Chi...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inChild development Vol. 74; no. 5; pp. 1393 - 1417
Main Authors Schutte, Anne R., Spencer, John P., Schöner, Gregor
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Oxford, UK Blackwell Publishing 01.09.2003
Blackwell Publishers
Blackwell
Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory.
AbstractList The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory.The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory.
The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory.
The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', butnot 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4-and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separationat which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory. (Original abstract)
Author Spencer, John P.
Schutte, Anne R.
Schöner, Gregor
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Anne R.
  surname: Schutte
  fullname: Schutte, Anne R.
– sequence: 2
  givenname: John P.
  surname: Spencer
  fullname: Spencer, John P.
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Gregor
  surname: Schöner
  fullname: Schöner, Gregor
BackLink http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=15163152$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14552405$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkk1vEzEQhleoiH7AmQtCFhLctvX3erlB0hZQ0tISyNFyNrPU6e462JtC_j3ebkilHogvlmee1-PxvIfJXuMaSJKXBB-TuE4Il1mqJOXHGEvCnyQH28hecoAxzlOWU7yfHIawiEcqc_Ys2SdcCMqxOEiaCYTWNj9RewNouG5MbQt0ZqGao8kNOL9-j6bO33bEGOp4RqXzaOQK01rXBPQRCldDQGPnAX1bxqipqjX66qGwAdDlHXg0hDuo3LKGpn2ePC1NFeDFZj9Kvp-dTgaf0tHl-efBh1FaiAzzVHKpZpKxjKsCA5G5mJkZGIyZorTEknPDyJzQnPK5lMawGS0p5MpAmSluCDtK3vX3Lr37tYot6tqGAqrKNOBWQWeESiyl3AnG50ihpNoJMiUYpbQD3zwCF27lm9itJrmKhTHLIvR6A61mNcz10tva-LX-N5gIvN0AJhSmKr1p4oc-cIJIRgSN3EnPFd6F4KF8QLDuLKI7Q-jOEPreIlEhHikK295Ps_XGVv_R8V7321aw3lVGD4anP3rZq162CK3zWxmTuSSq6zPt0za08GebNv5Wy4xlQk8vzvV4fHF9dX31RU_ZXz1346A
CODEN CHDEAW
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neunet_2008_03_007
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2014_01_007
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_newideapsych_2007_07_007
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae350
crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12087
crossref_primary_10_1080_13875868_2020_1731515
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2007_06_081
crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_113_2_273
crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2019_1653297
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01113_6
crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218241238737
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2007_00667_x
crossref_primary_10_1177_0963721414548214
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_physd_2016_01_009
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2010_05_004
crossref_primary_10_1111_mono_12478
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2016_11_002
crossref_primary_10_1080_15248370802678331
crossref_primary_10_1111_mono_12254
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506280701766313
crossref_primary_10_1080_09540091_2010_489742
crossref_primary_10_1080_15250000802004379
crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_112_2_329
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2007_00566_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogsys_2025_101339
crossref_primary_10_3917_devel_007_0029
crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193363
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2012_01148_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2013_07_006
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2014_09_013
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12010
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2012_07_006
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2008_10_017
crossref_primary_10_1037_0096_1523_32_2_473
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_01685
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ecresq_2014_04_014
crossref_primary_10_1177_0963721420959835
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_013_0443_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2012_07_002
crossref_primary_10_1109_TCDS_2017_2676839
crossref_primary_10_3945_ajcn_112_040766
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijdevneu_2009_08_010
crossref_primary_10_1177_1059712311421219
crossref_primary_10_1080_10478400701774147
crossref_primary_10_1080_03640210802222021
crossref_primary_10_1002_mono_12104
crossref_primary_10_1080_13875868_2011_579212
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12401
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11571_009_9096_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2006_03_001
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_8624_2008_01214_x
crossref_primary_10_1162_neco_2008_11_06_392
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2008_01_005
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_8624_2006_00955_x
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00087_2018
crossref_primary_10_1207_s15327647jcd0704_3
crossref_primary_10_1207_s15327647jcd0704_4
crossref_primary_10_3390_dynamics3030027
crossref_primary_10_1024_1421_0185_a000023
crossref_primary_10_1080_15248371003700007
crossref_primary_10_1093_ajcn_nqz018
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_009_0255_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2016_02_009
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2011_01083_x
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_20431
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2008_00754_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2013_12_002
crossref_primary_10_1109_TAMD_2011_2138705
crossref_primary_10_1177_17456916231187324
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2004_05_002
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0259511
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2008_11_006
crossref_primary_10_1080_09540091_2017_1405382
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2008_00700_x
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2012_01156_x
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_0832_6
crossref_primary_10_1109_TAMD_2011_2109714
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2017_10_005
crossref_primary_10_1080_15248370802247988
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_0956_7976_2004_01503007_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychres_2009_09_002
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_1058_3
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2012_690732
crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_87
crossref_primary_10_1016_S0010_0285_03_00099_9
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1750_8606_2011_00194_x
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_008_0210_z
crossref_primary_10_1177_1087054708329970
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_013_0596_9
crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2017_1300155
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2012_08_001
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1751_9004_2011_00393_x
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_3117547
crossref_primary_10_1177_0165025415584808
crossref_primary_10_3390_jintelligence6010008
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1750_8606_2011_00183_x
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12264
Cites_doi 10.1126/science.8316836
10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.686
10.1016/S0166-4115(97)80011-8
10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.16
10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90038-7
10.1007/BF00337259
10.1093/cercor/10.9.910
10.1111/1467-7687.00295
10.1126/science.8465199
10.1080/15248372.2003.9669681
10.1111/1467-8624.00351
10.1111/1467-8624.00095
10.1007/s00221-002-1046-4
10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.545
10.1016/0028-3932(90)90031-I
10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-01227.1993
10.1111/1467-7687.00021
10.1111/1467-7687.00296
10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.203
10.1017/S0140525X01003910
10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00088-9
10.1002/dev.420220307
10.1037/11168-000
10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.192
10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.352
10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220
10.1006/cogp.1994.1014
10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768813
10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.235
10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1062
10.1097/00001756-199801260-00025
10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48900.x
10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00125-9
10.1111/1467-8624.00413
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-08-02928.1988
10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright 2003 The Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
2003 INIST-CNRS
Copyright Blackwell Publishers Inc. Sep/Oct 2003
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright 2003 The Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
– notice: 2003 INIST-CNRS
– notice: Copyright Blackwell Publishers Inc. Sep/Oct 2003
DBID BSCLL
AAYXX
CITATION
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QJ
7TK
8BJ
AHOVV
FQK
JBE
U9A
7X8
DOI 10.1111/1467-8624.00614
DatabaseName Istex
CrossRef
Pascal-Francis
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)
Neurosciences Abstracts
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
Education Research Index
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts (ASSIA)
Neurosciences Abstracts
Career and Technical Education (Alumni Edition)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic

CrossRef
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts (ASSIA)
MEDLINE

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
Sociology & Social History
Social Welfare & Social Work
Psychology
EISSN 1467-8624
EndPage 1417
ExternalDocumentID 419411971
14552405
15163152
10_1111_1467_8624_00614
CDEV614
3696185
ark_67375_WNG_MMNRQRQJ_W
Genre article
Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S
Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
Feature
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIMH NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 MH62480
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-ET
-W8
-~X
..I
.3N
.GA
.GJ
.GO
.Y3
05W
07C
0B8
0R~
10A
1OB
1OC
29B
2AX
2KS
2WC
31~
33P
36B
3EH
4.4
41~
50Y
50Z
51W
51Y
52M
52O
52Q
52R
52S
52T
52U
52V
52W
53G
5GY
5HH
5LA
5RE
5VS
66C
692
6J9
6PF
702
7PT
8-0
8-1
8-3
8-4
8-5
85S
8UM
930
A01
A04
AAAZS
AABNI
AAESR
AAHHS
AAHSB
AAIKC
AAMNW
AAONW
AAOUF
AASGY
AAUTI
AAWTL
AAXRX
AAYJJ
AAYOK
AAZKR
ABBHK
ABCQN
ABCUV
ABDBF
ABDPE
ABEML
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABLJU
ABLWH
ABOPQ
ABPPZ
ABPVW
ABQWH
ABSOO
ABXGK
ABXSQ
ACAHQ
ACBKW
ACBWZ
ACCFJ
ACCZN
ACFBH
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACGOF
ACHQT
ACKOT
ACMXC
ACNCT
ACPOU
ACPRK
ACPVT
ACSCC
ACTDY
ACXQS
ADACV
ADBBV
ADBTR
ADEMA
ADEOM
ADEPB
ADIZJ
ADKYN
ADMGS
ADMHG
ADNFJ
ADULT
ADXAS
ADZJE
ADZMN
ADZOD
AEEZP
AEGXH
AEIGN
AEIMD
AEQDE
AEUPB
AEUQT
AEUYR
AFBPY
AFEBI
AFFNX
AFFPM
AFGKR
AFHKK
AFKFF
AFPWT
AFZJQ
AGNAY
AHBTC
AI.
AIACR
AIAGR
AIDAL
AIFKG
AIURR
AIWBW
AJBDE
AJPNJ
ALAGY
ALEEW
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALUQN
AMBMR
AMYDB
AQSKT
ASPBG
ASTYK
ASUFR
AS~
AVWKF
AZBYB
AZFZN
AZVAB
B-7
BAFTC
BDRZF
BFHJK
BKOMP
BMXJE
BNVMJ
BQESF
BROTX
BRXPI
BSCLL
BY8
C2-
CAG
COF
CS3
D-6
D-7
D-C
D-D
DCZOG
DPXWK
DR2
DRFUL
DRMAN
DRSSH
DU5
E3Z
EAD
EAP
EAS
EAU
EBC
EBD
EBS
EDJ
EJD
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPS
ESI
ESX
F00
F01
F5P
FEDTE
FUBAC
G-S
G.N
G50
GENNL
GODZA
HF~
HGD
HGLYW
HVGLF
HZI
HZ~
H~9
IHE
IPSME
IRD
ITG
ITH
IX1
J0M
JAAYA
JAV
JBMMH
JBZCM
JENOY
JHFFW
JKQEH
JLEZI
JLXEF
JPL
JSODD
JST
K48
KBYEO
KCP
LATKE
LC2
LC4
LEEKS
LH4
LITHE
LOXES
LP6
LP7
LUTES
LW6
LYRES
MEWTI
MK4
MLAFT
MRFUL
MRMAN
MRSSH
MSFUL
MSMAN
MSSSH
MVM
MXFUL
MXMAN
MXSSH
N04
N06
N9A
NEJ
NF~
NHB
O66
O9-
OIG
OK1
OMK
OVD
P-O
P2P
P2W
P2Y
P2Z
P4B
P4C
PQQKQ
Q.N
Q11
QB0
R.K
RIG
ROL
RPD
RX1
RXW
SA0
SAMSI
SKT
SUPJJ
SV3
TAE
TEORI
TN5
UB1
UBW
UBZ
UIG
UKR
V8K
VH1
VQA
W8V
W99
WBKPD
WH7
WHDPE
WIH
WII
WIJ
WOHZO
WQZ
WRC
WSUWO
WXI
WXSBR
X6Y
XA6
XG1
XIH
XKC
XOL
XSW
XZL
YF5
YOJ
YYP
YZZ
ZCA
ZCG
ZGI
ZKG
ZUP
ZZTAW
~A~
~IA
~KM
~WP
AAHQN
AAIPD
AAMMB
AAMNL
AANHP
AAYCA
ABAWQ
ACHJO
ACRPL
ACUHS
ACYXJ
ADNMO
AEFGJ
AEYWJ
AFWVQ
AGQPQ
AGXDD
AIDQK
AIDYY
ALVPJ
AFYRF
YR5
AAYXX
AGHNM
CITATION
ABDSA
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
VXZ
7QJ
7TK
8BJ
AHOVV
FQK
JBE
U9A
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c5704-6468b633748c0e1695babea003822f0644a31d12924d66aa3b2f2e98aef784a13
IEDL.DBID DR2
ISSN 0009-3920
IngestDate Mon Jul 21 09:32:16 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 19:14:13 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 23:49:16 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 09:26:55 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 01:51:22 EST 2025
Wed Mar 05 09:27:44 EST 2025
Tue Jul 01 04:10:58 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:10:21 EDT 2025
Wed Jan 22 16:49:38 EST 2025
Thu Jul 03 21:32:01 EDT 2025
Wed Oct 30 10:02:10 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 5
Keywords Human
Preschool age
Stimulus localization
Theory
Cognition
Neural network
Dynamical system
Experimental study
School age
Cognitive development
Working memory
Child
Spatial memory
Language English
License CC BY 4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c5704-6468b633748c0e1695babea003822f0644a31d12924d66aa3b2f2e98aef784a13
Notes istex:9C6B91106371A70280367AA0CC5CD522EF514437
ArticleID:CDEV614
ark:/67375/WNG-MMNRQRQJ-W
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-2
content type line 23
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
PMID 14552405
PQID 198712037
PQPubID 35183
PageCount 25
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_71260666
proquest_miscellaneous_57065868
proquest_miscellaneous_38532228
proquest_journals_198712037
pubmed_primary_14552405
pascalfrancis_primary_15163152
crossref_primary_10_1111_1467_8624_00614
crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_1467_8624_00614
wiley_primary_10_1111_1467_8624_00614_CDEV614
jstor_primary_3696185
istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_MMNRQRQJ_W
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2003-09-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2003-09-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 09
  year: 2003
  text: 2003-09-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2000
PublicationPlace Oxford, UK
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Oxford, UK
– name: Malden, MA
– name: United States
– name: Ann Arbor
PublicationTitle Child development
PublicationTitleAlternate Child Dev
PublicationYear 2003
Publisher Blackwell Publishing
Blackwell Publishers
Blackwell
Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Publisher_xml – name: Blackwell Publishing
– name: Blackwell Publishers
– name: Blackwell
– name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd
References Suzuki, W. A., Miller, E. K., & Desimone, R. (1997). Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 1062-1081.
Di Pellegrino, G., & Wise, S. P. (1993). Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1227-1243.
McDowell, K., Jeka, J. J., Schöner, G., & Hatfield, B. D. (2002). Behavioral and electrocortical evidence of an interaction between probability and task metrics in movement preparation. Experimental Brain Research, 144, 303-313.
Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Wang, X. J. (2000). Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 910-923.
Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the A-not-B and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 267-317.
Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (2002). Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychological Review, 109, 545-572.
Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003a). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science, 6, 392-412.
Bastian, A., Riehle, A., Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (1998). Prior information preshapes the population representation of movement direction in motor cortex. NeuroReport, 9, 315-319.
Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychological Review, 106, 235-260.
Spencer, J. P., & Schutte, A. R. (in press). Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses? Psychological Science.
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex-Developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195-205.
Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Spinnazzola, J. (1998). Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Developmental Psychology, 34, 203-214.
Amari, S. (1977). Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biological Cybernetics, 27, 77-87.
Hund, A. M., & Spencer, J. P. (2003). Developmental changes in the relative weighting of geometric and experience-dependent location cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 3-38.
Munakata, Y., McClelland, J. L., Johnson, M. H., & Siegler, R. S. (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. Psychological Review, 104, 686-719.
Wilson, F. A. W., Scalaidhe, S. P., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 260, 1955-1958.
Erlhagen, W., Bastian, A., Jancke, D., Riehle, A., & Schöner, G. (1999). The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 94, 53-66.
Munakata, Y., & McClelland, J. L. (2003). Connectionist models of development. Developmental Science, 6, 413-429.
Diamond, A., Cruttenden, L., & Neiderman, D. (1994). AB with multiple wells: 1. Why are multiple wells sometimes easier than two wells? 2. Memory or memory +inhibition? Developmental Psychology, 30, 192-205.
Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development, 70, 1297-1313.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Bartsch, K. (1987). Infant search and object permanence: A meta-analysis of the A-not-B error. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, (3, Serial No. 214).
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220-241.
Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11-28.
Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (in press) Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall. Cognitive Psychology.
Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., & Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development, 13, 185-200.
Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic.
Georgopoulos, A. P., Taira, M., & Lukashin, A. (1993). Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex. Science, 260, 47-52.
Munakata, Y. (1998). Infant perseveration and implications for object permanence theories: A PDP model of the AB task. Developmental Science, 1, 161-184.
Diamond, A., & Doar, B. (1989). The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 271-294.
Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. B. (2001). A dynamic field theory of infant perseverative reaching errors. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 1-86.
Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (2002). Prototypes and particulars: Geometric and experience-dependent spatial categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 16-37.
Tanaka, S. (2000). Roles of intracortical inhibition in the formation of spatially tuned delay-period activity of prefrontal cortical neurons: Computational study. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 24, 483-504.
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review, 98, 352-376.
Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2000, August). A dynamic field model of location memory. Paper presented at the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia.
Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003b). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science.
Georgopoulos, A. P., Kettner, R. E., & Schwartz, A. B. (1988). Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2928-2937.
Spencer, J. P., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2001). Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of 2-year-olds. Child Development, 72, 1327-1346.
Schutte, A. R., & Spencer, J. P. (2002). Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A-not-B error beyond infancy: Three-year-olds' delay- and experience-dependent location memory biases. Child Development, 73, 377-404.
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28, 517-527.
Tipper, S. P. (1992). Selection for action: The role of inhibitory mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 105-109.
Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. H. (1994). The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115-147.
2001; 72
1989; 22
1997; 119
1987; 51
1990; 14
2002; 131
1991; 98
1977; 27
2002; 73
2000; 24
1990; 608
1993; 260
1954
1994; 27
2003b
1999; 106
2001; 24
2003a; 6
2000, August
1993; 13
1997; 104
1990; 28
2000; 10
2003; 6
2000; 129
1988; 8
2002; 144
1997; 78
2003; 4
1998; 1
1999; 94
2002; 109
1999; 70
1979; 163
1992; 1
1994; 30
1998; 34
1998; 9
1998; 13
Wellman H. M. (e_1_2_1_41_1) 1987; 51
Spencer J. P. (e_1_2_1_32_1) 2000
e_1_2_1_42_1
e_1_2_1_20_1
Spencer J. P. (e_1_2_1_31_1)
e_1_2_1_40_1
e_1_2_1_23_1
e_1_2_1_24_1
e_1_2_1_21_1
e_1_2_1_22_1
e_1_2_1_43_1
e_1_2_1_27_1
e_1_2_1_28_1
e_1_2_1_25_1
e_1_2_1_26_1
Spencer J. P. (e_1_2_1_35_1)
e_1_2_1_7_1
Smith L. B. (e_1_2_1_29_1) 1999; 106
e_1_2_1_8_1
e_1_2_1_30_1
e_1_2_1_5_1
e_1_2_1_6_1
Huttenlocher J. (e_1_2_1_14_1) 1991; 98
e_1_2_1_3_1
e_1_2_1_12_1
e_1_2_1_4_1
e_1_2_1_13_1
e_1_2_1_34_1
e_1_2_1_10_1
e_1_2_1_33_1
e_1_2_1_2_1
e_1_2_1_11_1
e_1_2_1_16_1
e_1_2_1_39_1
e_1_2_1_17_1
e_1_2_1_38_1
e_1_2_1_37_1
e_1_2_1_15_1
e_1_2_1_36_1
e_1_2_1_9_1
e_1_2_1_18_1
e_1_2_1_19_1
References_xml – reference: Tipper, S. P. (1992). Selection for action: The role of inhibitory mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 105-109.
– reference: Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., & Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development, 13, 185-200.
– reference: Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220-241.
– reference: Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11-28.
– reference: Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the A-not-B and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 267-317.
– reference: Munakata, Y. (1998). Infant perseveration and implications for object permanence theories: A PDP model of the AB task. Developmental Science, 1, 161-184.
– reference: Di Pellegrino, G., & Wise, S. P. (1993). Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1227-1243.
– reference: Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex-Developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195-205.
– reference: Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Spinnazzola, J. (1998). Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Developmental Psychology, 34, 203-214.
– reference: Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Wang, X. J. (2000). Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 910-923.
– reference: Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (2002). Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychological Review, 109, 545-572.
– reference: Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. H. (1994). The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115-147.
– reference: Erlhagen, W., Bastian, A., Jancke, D., Riehle, A., & Schöner, G. (1999). The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 94, 53-66.
– reference: Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. B. (2001). A dynamic field theory of infant perseverative reaching errors. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 1-86.
– reference: Diamond, A., & Doar, B. (1989). The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 271-294.
– reference: Schutte, A. R., & Spencer, J. P. (2002). Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A-not-B error beyond infancy: Three-year-olds' delay- and experience-dependent location memory biases. Child Development, 73, 377-404.
– reference: Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic.
– reference: Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychological Review, 106, 235-260.
– reference: Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review, 98, 352-376.
– reference: Amari, S. (1977). Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biological Cybernetics, 27, 77-87.
– reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003a). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science, 6, 392-412.
– reference: Tanaka, S. (2000). Roles of intracortical inhibition in the formation of spatially tuned delay-period activity of prefrontal cortical neurons: Computational study. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 24, 483-504.
– reference: Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (2002). Prototypes and particulars: Geometric and experience-dependent spatial categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 16-37.
– reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2000, August). A dynamic field model of location memory. Paper presented at the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia.
– reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003b). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science.
– reference: Bastian, A., Riehle, A., Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (1998). Prior information preshapes the population representation of movement direction in motor cortex. NeuroReport, 9, 315-319.
– reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schutte, A. R. (in press). Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses? Psychological Science.
– reference: Diamond, A., Cruttenden, L., & Neiderman, D. (1994). AB with multiple wells: 1. Why are multiple wells sometimes easier than two wells? 2. Memory or memory +inhibition? Developmental Psychology, 30, 192-205.
– reference: Georgopoulos, A. P., Taira, M., & Lukashin, A. (1993). Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex. Science, 260, 47-52.
– reference: Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development, 70, 1297-1313.
– reference: Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28, 517-527.
– reference: Georgopoulos, A. P., Kettner, R. E., & Schwartz, A. B. (1988). Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2928-2937.
– reference: McDowell, K., Jeka, J. J., Schöner, G., & Hatfield, B. D. (2002). Behavioral and electrocortical evidence of an interaction between probability and task metrics in movement preparation. Experimental Brain Research, 144, 303-313.
– reference: Spencer, J. P., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2001). Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of 2-year-olds. Child Development, 72, 1327-1346.
– reference: Wilson, F. A. W., Scalaidhe, S. P., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 260, 1955-1958.
– reference: Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (in press) Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall. Cognitive Psychology.
– reference: Suzuki, W. A., Miller, E. K., & Desimone, R. (1997). Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 1062-1081.
– reference: Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Bartsch, K. (1987). Infant search and object permanence: A meta-analysis of the A-not-B error. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, (3, Serial No. 214).
– reference: Hund, A. M., & Spencer, J. P. (2003). Developmental changes in the relative weighting of geometric and experience-dependent location cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 3-38.
– reference: Munakata, Y., McClelland, J. L., Johnson, M. H., & Siegler, R. S. (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. Psychological Review, 104, 686-719.
– reference: Munakata, Y., & McClelland, J. L. (2003). Connectionist models of development. Developmental Science, 6, 413-429.
– volume: 6
  start-page: 413
  year: 2003
  end-page: 429
  article-title: Connectionist models of development
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 78
  start-page: 1062
  year: 1997
  end-page: 1081
  article-title: Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex
  publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology
– volume: 27
  start-page: 115
  year: 1994
  end-page: 147
  article-title: The coding of spatial location in young children
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 24
  start-page: 1
  year: 2001
  end-page: 86
  article-title: A dynamic field theory of infant perseverative reaching errors
  publication-title: Behavioral and Brain Sciences
– year: 2000, August
– volume: 9
  start-page: 315
  year: 1998
  end-page: 319
  article-title: Prior information preshapes the population representation of movement direction in motor cortex
  publication-title: NeuroReport
– volume: 30
  start-page: 192
  year: 1994
  end-page: 205
  article-title: AB with multiple wells
  publication-title: Developmental Psychology
– article-title: Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses?
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 98
  start-page: 352
  year: 1991
  end-page: 376
  article-title: Categories and particulars
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 163
  start-page: 195
  year: 1979
  end-page: 205
  article-title: Synaptic density in human frontal cortex—Developmental changes and effects of aging
  publication-title: Brain Research
– volume: 72
  start-page: 1327
  year: 2001
  end-page: 1346
  article-title: Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A‐not‐B error
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 10
  start-page: 910
  year: 2000
  end-page: 923
  article-title: Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model
  publication-title: Cerebral Cortex
– volume: 14
  start-page: 11
  year: 1990
  end-page: 28
  article-title: Maturational constraints on language learning
  publication-title: Cognitive Science
– year: 2003b
  article-title: Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 129
  start-page: 220
  year: 2000
  end-page: 241
  article-title: Why do categories affect stimulus judgement?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– year: 1954
– volume: 94
  start-page: 53
  year: 1999
  end-page: 66
  article-title: The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods
– article-title: Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 260
  start-page: 1955
  year: 1993
  end-page: 1958
  article-title: Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 6
  start-page: 392
  year: 2003a
  end-page: 412
  article-title: Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 51
  year: 1987
  article-title: Infant search and object permanence
  publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
– volume: 34
  start-page: 203
  year: 1998
  end-page: 214
  article-title: Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task
  publication-title: Developmental Psychology
– volume: 13
  start-page: 1227
  year: 1993
  end-page: 1243
  article-title: Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 13
  start-page: 185
  year: 1998
  end-page: 200
  article-title: The development of spatial location coding
  publication-title: Cognitive Development
– volume: 119
  start-page: 271
  year: 1997
  end-page: 310
– volume: 131
  start-page: 16
  year: 2002
  end-page: 37
  article-title: Prototypes and particulars
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 28
  start-page: 517
  year: 1990
  end-page: 527
  article-title: Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 1
  start-page: 105
  year: 1992
  end-page: 109
  article-title: Selection for action
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1297
  year: 1999
  end-page: 1313
  article-title: The A‐not‐B error
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 1
  start-page: 161
  year: 1998
  end-page: 184
  article-title: Infant perseveration and implications for object permanence theories
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 144
  start-page: 303
  year: 2002
  end-page: 313
  article-title: Behavioral and electrocortical evidence of an interaction between probability and task metrics in movement preparation
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
– volume: 106
  start-page: 235
  year: 1999
  end-page: 260
  article-title: Knowing in the context of acting
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 22
  start-page: 271
  year: 1989
  end-page: 294
  article-title: The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task
  publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology
– volume: 73
  start-page: 377
  year: 2002
  end-page: 404
  article-title: Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A‐not‐B error beyond infancy
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 608
  start-page: 267
  year: 1990
  end-page: 317
  article-title: The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the A‐not‐B and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys
  publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
– volume: 24
  start-page: 483
  year: 2000
  end-page: 504
  article-title: Roles of intracortical inhibition in the formation of spatially tuned delay‐period activity of prefrontal cortical neurons
  publication-title: Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry
– volume: 260
  start-page: 47
  year: 1993
  end-page: 52
  article-title: Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 4
  start-page: 3
  year: 2003
  end-page: 38
  article-title: Developmental changes in the relative weighting of geometric and experience‐dependent location cues
  publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development
– volume: 109
  start-page: 545
  year: 2002
  end-page: 572
  article-title: Dynamic field theory of movement preparation
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 104
  start-page: 686
  year: 1997
  end-page: 719
  article-title: Rethinking infant knowledge
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 27
  start-page: 77
  year: 1977
  end-page: 87
  article-title: Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral‐inhibition type neural fields
  publication-title: Biological Cybernetics
– volume: 8
  start-page: 2928
  year: 1988
  end-page: 2937
  article-title: Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three‐dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
– ident: e_1_2_1_42_1
  doi: 10.1126/science.8316836
– volume-title: A dynamic field model of location memory
  year: 2000
  ident: e_1_2_1_32_1
– ident: e_1_2_1_23_1
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.686
– ident: e_1_2_1_27_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(97)80011-8
– ident: e_1_2_1_30_1
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.16
– ident: e_1_2_1_24_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90038-7
– ident: e_1_2_1_2_1
  doi: 10.1007/BF00337259
– ident: e_1_2_1_4_1
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.9.910
– ident: e_1_2_1_34_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00295
– ident: e_1_2_1_12_1
  doi: 10.1126/science.8465199
– ident: e_1_2_1_13_1
  doi: 10.1080/15248372.2003.9669681
– ident: e_1_2_1_36_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00351
– volume: 51
  year: 1987
  ident: e_1_2_1_41_1
  article-title: Infant search and object permanence
  publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
– ident: e_1_2_1_31_1
  article-title: Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– ident: e_1_2_1_19_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00095
– ident: e_1_2_1_20_1
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1046-4
– ident: e_1_2_1_10_1
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.545
– ident: e_1_2_1_18_1
  doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90031-I
– ident: e_1_2_1_25_1
  doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2
– ident: e_1_2_1_8_1
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-01227.1993
– ident: e_1_2_1_21_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00021
– ident: e_1_2_1_22_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00296
– ident: e_1_2_1_43_1
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.203
– ident: e_1_2_1_39_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01003910
– ident: e_1_2_1_35_1
  article-title: Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses?
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– ident: e_1_2_1_38_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00088-9
– ident: e_1_2_1_7_1
  doi: 10.1002/dev.420220307
– ident: e_1_2_1_26_1
  doi: 10.1037/11168-000
– ident: e_1_2_1_6_1
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.192
– ident: e_1_2_1_33_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00295
– volume: 98
  start-page: 352
  year: 1991
  ident: e_1_2_1_14_1
  article-title: Categories and particulars
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.352
– ident: e_1_2_1_15_1
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220
– ident: e_1_2_1_16_1
  doi: 10.1006/cogp.1994.1014
– ident: e_1_2_1_40_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768813
– volume: 106
  start-page: 235
  year: 1999
  ident: e_1_2_1_29_1
  article-title: Knowing in the context of acting
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.235
– ident: e_1_2_1_37_1
  doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1062
– ident: e_1_2_1_3_1
  doi: 10.1097/00001756-199801260-00025
– ident: e_1_2_1_5_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48900.x
– ident: e_1_2_1_9_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00125-9
– ident: e_1_2_1_28_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00413
– ident: e_1_2_1_11_1
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-08-02928.1988
– ident: e_1_2_1_17_1
  doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4
SSID ssj0002693
Score 2.16693
Snippet The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
crossref
wiley
jstor
istex
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 1393
SubjectTerms Adult
Age differences
Age groups
Association Learning
Attention
Biological and medical sciences
Child
Child development
Child, Preschool
Children
Cognition
Development
Developmental psychology
Distance Perception
Dynamic field theory
Empirical Articles
Experimentation
Female
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Humans
Male
Memory
Memory, Short-Term
Modeling
Neural Networks (Computer)
Orientation
Overdevelopment
Problem Solving
Psychology
Psychology, Child
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Psychomotor Performance
Retention (Psychology)
Space
Space Perception
Spatial analysis
Spatial location
Spatial models
Stereotyped Behavior
T tests
Task performance
Toys
Working memory
Young children
Title Testing the Dynamic Field Theory: Working Memory for Locations Becomes More Spatially Precise Over Development
URI https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-MMNRQRQJ-W/fulltext.pdf
https://www.jstor.org/stable/3696185
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2F1467-8624.00614
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14552405
https://www.proquest.com/docview/198712037
https://www.proquest.com/docview/38532228
https://www.proquest.com/docview/57065868
https://www.proquest.com/docview/71260666
Volume 74
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1bb9MwFD5CQ0J74VIGyzaGH9DES6rm3vAG28o0kcKqjfJm2YmNULsENa1E-PWc41y6TgyEeIqUnMSJfY7z2f78HYBXMvL8SKgY41vgAEX7mS2lcmytlHCFVFGY0opuMg7PrvzzL0HLJqS9MLU-RDfhRpFh-msKcCHLG0FuQpy2NxA1y6SyJsYWwaLJWkDKDeMulxoigUEj7kNcnlv3b_yX7lMV_2gpisSXFCVWma5zXfwOjG5iW_NzGj0C2X5WzUmZ9VdL2U9_3lJ8_K_vfgwPG-jK3ta-9gTuqbwHD5Jmcb4H211nWvXgoN73y6ZqrsVCsSPWnigWsx7sdRtl1ldqxZLqKeSXJP2Rf2UITtlJlYvrbykbEdWO1VICb1gzx88SIgpXDJE3-1A0s4_sHQ6pr1XJkgILppzL-PR5xT6RkEep2EeMXnaDLLUDV6PTy-Mzu8kLYadBNPDt0A-HMvRIOCcdKCeMA4leJWiR03U1YixfeE6GQMb1szAUwpOudlU8FEpHQ1843jPYyotc7QITOk5xiKscJ84QSmUiCpSrhUaU50VaZBb0W6_gaSOaTrk75rwdPFFzcGoObprDgtfdDd9rvZC7TY-Mm3V2YjEjml0U8On4PU-S8eRicnHOpxbsGD_sDCkHI-IsCw43_HJdYIAoG4GZBfuto_Kmayo5zTI57sCLLHjZXcU-hRaKRK6KVck9xHA0M3i3RUCr48PwDxZYhhkbW_C8jpD12_lBgDgSX982fv63euLHJ6ef8bj3j_b7sG2Yl4YQeABby8VKvUAEuZSHppP4BeDQYlo
linkProvider Wiley-Blackwell
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMw1V1Lb9NAEB6VVoJeeIQCbku7B6i4OIrfMRIHaBrSNg40SpvelrW9rqoEB8WJwPws_gp_iBm_0lQUxKEHTpHisXe1u7P7ze633wC88B3DdIR00b8FBiiRGaq-LzU1klLowpeOHdCJrtezO6fm0bl1vgI_yrswuT5EteFGnpHN1-TgtCF9xcszH6f7DcTN0syCWHks068YtiVvDlvYxy91vX0w2O-oRWYBNbCchqnapt30bYOkV4KG1GzX8rFego7JdD3CVdoUhhbiUqiboW0LYfh6pEu3KWTkNE2hGfjdO7BGecRJr7_VX0hW6bZbZW9D7NEo5ISIPXStwksr4Rp16reSFEkMTZFgJ0V5do3fwd9lNJ0th-0H8LNsyJwFM6rPZ349-H5NY_L_aumHcL9A5-xt7k6PYEXGNbjrFfyDGqxX60Vag-38ajMbynEkppLtsfKPyXRUg83qLtDiSS7Kkj6GeEDqJvEFQ_zNWmksPl8GrE1sQparJbxmxTEG84gLnTIMLlh3UmywsncSG1EmzJtgwZRWGr8-TtlH0ipJJPuAExS7wgfbgNNbabYnsBpPYvkMmIjcAKN4qWluiGgxFI4l9UhECGQNJxKhAvVyGPKg0IWn9CRjXsaH1P2cup9n3a_Aq-qFL7kkys2me9m4ruzEdERMQsfiw9577nm9_kn_5IgPFdjIBn5lSGkmEUoqsLPkCIsCLQwkEHsqsFV6Bi9m34TTRpqmNwxHgd3qKU6bdBYmYjmZJ9xAmEqbnzdbWEQAaNp_sMAysvBfgae5Sy5qZ1oWQmWsvpo51t_aie-3Ds7wd_Mf7XfhXmfgdXn3sHe8BesZ0TTjP27D6mw6l88RMM_8nWyGYvDpth31F5Bpvb0
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMw1V1Lb9NAEB6VVqp64REKuC3tHqDi4ih-x0gcoGnoy6GN2qa3ZW3vIpTgVHEqMP-Kv8IvYsavNBUFceiBU6R47F3t7Ox-s_PtDMCL0LNsT0gf7Vugg6LsWA9DaehKSmGKUHpuRBHdoOfundkHF87FAvyo7sIU-SHqAzeyjHy9JgO_jNU1I89NnK43EDXLsEte5aHMvqLXlr7Z76CKX5pmd_d0Z08vCwvokeO1bN213XboWpR5JWpJw_WdELslKEpmmgo3aVtYRow7oWnHriuEFZrKlH5bSOW1bWFY-N17sGS7LZ-qRXT6s4xVpuvXxdsQerTKbEJEHrrR4bmNcIl0-q3iRBJBU6SoI1UU1_gd-p0H0_lu2H0AP6txLEgww-bVNGxG32-kmPyvBvoh3C-xOXtbGNMjWJBJA5aDkn3QgJV6t8gasFFcbGYDOVJiItk2q_4YT4YNWKtvAs2eFClZsseQnFJuk-QTQ_TNOlkivnyOWJe4hKzIlfCalUEMFhATOmPoWrCjcXm8yt5JHEOZsmCMDVNRafz6KGPHlKkklewDLk_sGhtsFc7uZNiewGIyTuQzYEL5Efrw0jD8GLFiLDxHmkoohLGWp0SsQbOahTwqs8JTcZIRr7xDUj8n9fNc_Rq8ql-4LBKi3C66nU_rWk5MhsQj9Bw-6L3nQdDrn_RPDvhAg9V83teCVGQSgaQGm3N2MGvQQTcCkacG65Vh8HLtTTkdoxlmy_I02Kqf4qJJkTCRyPFVyi0EqXT0ebuEQ-H_tvsHCWwjd_41eFpY5Kx3tuMgUMbu67ld_W2c-E5n9xx_1_5RfguWjztdfrTfO1yHlZxlmpMfN2BxOrmSzxEtT8PNfH1i8PGu7fQXdAu8bA
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Testing+the+dynamic+field+theory%3A+working+memory+for+locations+becomes+more+spatially+precise+over+development&rft.jtitle=Child+development&rft.au=Schutte%2C+Anne+R&rft.au=Spencer%2C+John+P&rft.au=Sch%C3%B6ner%2C+Gregor&rft.date=2003-09-01&rft.issn=0009-3920&rft.volume=74&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=1393&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2F1467-8624.00614&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0009-3920&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0009-3920&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0009-3920&client=summon