Testing the Dynamic Field Theory: Working Memory for Locations Becomes More Spatially Precise Over Development
The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Chi...
Saved in:
Published in | Child development Vol. 74; no. 5; pp. 1393 - 1417 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Oxford, UK
Blackwell Publishing
01.09.2003
Blackwell Publishers Blackwell Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory. |
---|---|
AbstractList | The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory.The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory. The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', but not 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4- and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separation at which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory. The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions in working memory that become enhanced over development. This was tested by varying the separation between A and B locations in a sandbox. Children searched for an object 6 times at an A location, followed by 3 trials at a B location. Two- and 4-year-olds', butnot 6-year-olds', responses were biased toward A when A and B were 9-in. and 6-in. apart. When A and B were separated by 2 in., however, 4-and 6-year-olds' responses were biased toward A. Thus, the separationat which responses were biased toward A decreased across age groups, supporting the predictions of the theory. (Original abstract) |
Author | Spencer, John P. Schutte, Anne R. Schöner, Gregor |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Anne R. surname: Schutte fullname: Schutte, Anne R. – sequence: 2 givenname: John P. surname: Spencer fullname: Spencer, John P. – sequence: 3 givenname: Gregor surname: Schöner fullname: Schöner, Gregor |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=15163152$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14552405$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkk1vEzEQhleoiH7AmQtCFhLctvX3erlB0hZQ0tISyNFyNrPU6e462JtC_j3ebkilHogvlmee1-PxvIfJXuMaSJKXBB-TuE4Il1mqJOXHGEvCnyQH28hecoAxzlOWU7yfHIawiEcqc_Ys2SdcCMqxOEiaCYTWNj9RewNouG5MbQt0ZqGao8kNOL9-j6bO33bEGOp4RqXzaOQK01rXBPQRCldDQGPnAX1bxqipqjX66qGwAdDlHXg0hDuo3LKGpn2ePC1NFeDFZj9Kvp-dTgaf0tHl-efBh1FaiAzzVHKpZpKxjKsCA5G5mJkZGIyZorTEknPDyJzQnPK5lMawGS0p5MpAmSluCDtK3vX3Lr37tYot6tqGAqrKNOBWQWeESiyl3AnG50ihpNoJMiUYpbQD3zwCF27lm9itJrmKhTHLIvR6A61mNcz10tva-LX-N5gIvN0AJhSmKr1p4oc-cIJIRgSN3EnPFd6F4KF8QLDuLKI7Q-jOEPreIlEhHikK295Ps_XGVv_R8V7321aw3lVGD4anP3rZq162CK3zWxmTuSSq6zPt0za08GebNv5Wy4xlQk8vzvV4fHF9dX31RU_ZXz1346A |
CODEN | CHDEAW |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neunet_2008_03_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2014_01_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_newideapsych_2007_07_007 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae350 crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12087 crossref_primary_10_1080_13875868_2020_1731515 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2007_06_081 crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_113_2_273 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2019_1653297 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01113_6 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218241238737 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2007_00667_x crossref_primary_10_1177_0963721414548214 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_physd_2016_01_009 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2010_05_004 crossref_primary_10_1111_mono_12478 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2016_11_002 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248370802678331 crossref_primary_10_1111_mono_12254 crossref_primary_10_1080_13506280701766313 crossref_primary_10_1080_09540091_2010_489742 crossref_primary_10_1080_15250000802004379 crossref_primary_10_1037_0033_295X_112_2_329 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2007_00566_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogsys_2025_101339 crossref_primary_10_3917_devel_007_0029 crossref_primary_10_3758_BF03193363 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2012_01148_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2013_07_006 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2014_09_013 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2012_07_006 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2008_10_017 crossref_primary_10_1037_0096_1523_32_2_473 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_01685 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ecresq_2014_04_014 crossref_primary_10_1177_0963721420959835 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_013_0443_y crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2012_07_002 crossref_primary_10_1109_TCDS_2017_2676839 crossref_primary_10_3945_ajcn_112_040766 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijdevneu_2009_08_010 crossref_primary_10_1177_1059712311421219 crossref_primary_10_1080_10478400701774147 crossref_primary_10_1080_03640210802222021 crossref_primary_10_1002_mono_12104 crossref_primary_10_1080_13875868_2011_579212 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12401 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11571_009_9096_y crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2006_03_001 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_8624_2008_01214_x crossref_primary_10_1162_neco_2008_11_06_392 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2008_01_005 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_8624_2006_00955_x crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00087_2018 crossref_primary_10_1207_s15327647jcd0704_3 crossref_primary_10_1207_s15327647jcd0704_4 crossref_primary_10_3390_dynamics3030027 crossref_primary_10_1024_1421_0185_a000023 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248371003700007 crossref_primary_10_1093_ajcn_nqz018 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_009_0255_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2016_02_009 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2011_01083_x crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_20431 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2008_00754_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2013_12_002 crossref_primary_10_1109_TAMD_2011_2138705 crossref_primary_10_1177_17456916231187324 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2004_05_002 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0259511 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2008_11_006 crossref_primary_10_1080_09540091_2017_1405382 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2008_00700_x crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2012_01156_x crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_0832_6 crossref_primary_10_1109_TAMD_2011_2109714 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2017_10_005 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248370802247988 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_0956_7976_2004_01503007_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychres_2009_09_002 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_015_1058_3 crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2012_690732 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_87 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0010_0285_03_00099_9 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1750_8606_2011_00194_x crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_008_0210_z crossref_primary_10_1177_1087054708329970 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_013_0596_9 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2017_1300155 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2012_08_001 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1751_9004_2011_00393_x crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_3117547 crossref_primary_10_1177_0165025415584808 crossref_primary_10_3390_jintelligence6010008 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1750_8606_2011_00183_x crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12264 |
Cites_doi | 10.1126/science.8316836 10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.686 10.1016/S0166-4115(97)80011-8 10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.16 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90038-7 10.1007/BF00337259 10.1093/cercor/10.9.910 10.1111/1467-7687.00295 10.1126/science.8465199 10.1080/15248372.2003.9669681 10.1111/1467-8624.00351 10.1111/1467-8624.00095 10.1007/s00221-002-1046-4 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.545 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90031-I 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-01227.1993 10.1111/1467-7687.00021 10.1111/1467-7687.00296 10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.203 10.1017/S0140525X01003910 10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00088-9 10.1002/dev.420220307 10.1037/11168-000 10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.192 10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.352 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220 10.1006/cogp.1994.1014 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768813 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.235 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1062 10.1097/00001756-199801260-00025 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48900.x 10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00125-9 10.1111/1467-8624.00413 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-08-02928.1988 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright 2003 The Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. 2003 INIST-CNRS Copyright Blackwell Publishers Inc. Sep/Oct 2003 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright 2003 The Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. – notice: 2003 INIST-CNRS – notice: Copyright Blackwell Publishers Inc. Sep/Oct 2003 |
DBID | BSCLL AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QJ 7TK 8BJ AHOVV FQK JBE U9A 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1111/1467-8624.00614 |
DatabaseName | Istex CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA) Neurosciences Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Education Research Index International Bibliography of the Social Sciences International Bibliography of the Social Sciences MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts (ASSIA) Neurosciences Abstracts Career and Technical Education (Alumni Edition) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts (ASSIA) MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Sociology & Social History Social Welfare & Social Work Psychology |
EISSN | 1467-8624 |
EndPage | 1417 |
ExternalDocumentID | 419411971 14552405 15163152 10_1111_1467_8624_00614 CDEV614 3696185 ark_67375_WNG_MMNRQRQJ_W |
Genre | article Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Feature |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIMH NIH HHS grantid: R01 MH62480 |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -ET -W8 -~X ..I .3N .GA .GJ .GO .Y3 05W 07C 0B8 0R~ 10A 1OB 1OC 29B 2AX 2KS 2WC 31~ 33P 36B 3EH 4.4 41~ 50Y 50Z 51W 51Y 52M 52O 52Q 52R 52S 52T 52U 52V 52W 53G 5GY 5HH 5LA 5RE 5VS 66C 692 6J9 6PF 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 85S 8UM 930 A01 A04 AAAZS AABNI AAESR AAHHS AAHSB AAIKC AAMNW AAONW AAOUF AASGY AAUTI AAWTL AAXRX AAYJJ AAYOK AAZKR ABBHK ABCQN ABCUV ABDBF ABDPE ABEML ABIVO ABJNI ABLJU ABLWH ABOPQ ABPPZ ABPVW ABQWH ABSOO ABXGK ABXSQ ACAHQ ACBKW ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFO ACGFS ACGOF ACHQT ACKOT ACMXC ACNCT ACPOU ACPRK ACPVT ACSCC ACTDY ACXQS ADACV ADBBV ADBTR ADEMA ADEOM ADEPB ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADMHG ADNFJ ADULT ADXAS ADZJE ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEGXH AEIGN AEIMD AEQDE AEUPB AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFEBI AFFNX AFFPM AFGKR AFHKK AFKFF AFPWT AFZJQ AGNAY AHBTC AI. AIACR AIAGR AIDAL AIFKG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE AJPNJ ALAGY ALEEW ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN AMBMR AMYDB AQSKT ASPBG ASTYK ASUFR AS~ AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB B-7 BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BKOMP BMXJE BNVMJ BQESF BROTX BRXPI BSCLL BY8 C2- CAG COF CS3 D-6 D-7 D-C D-D DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRMAN DRSSH DU5 E3Z EAD EAP EAS EAU EBC EBD EBS EDJ EJD EMB EMK EMOBN EPS ESI ESX F00 F01 F5P FEDTE FUBAC G-S G.N G50 GENNL GODZA HF~ HGD HGLYW HVGLF HZI HZ~ H~9 IHE IPSME IRD ITG ITH IX1 J0M JAAYA JAV JBMMH JBZCM JENOY JHFFW JKQEH JLEZI JLXEF JPL JSODD JST K48 KBYEO KCP LATKE LC2 LC4 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES MEWTI MK4 MLAFT MRFUL MRMAN MRSSH MSFUL MSMAN MSSSH MVM MXFUL MXMAN MXSSH N04 N06 N9A NEJ NF~ NHB O66 O9- OIG OK1 OMK OVD P-O P2P P2W P2Y P2Z P4B P4C PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 R.K RIG ROL RPD RX1 RXW SA0 SAMSI SKT SUPJJ SV3 TAE TEORI TN5 UB1 UBW UBZ UIG UKR V8K VH1 VQA W8V W99 WBKPD WH7 WHDPE WIH WII WIJ WOHZO WQZ WRC WSUWO WXI WXSBR X6Y XA6 XG1 XIH XKC XOL XSW XZL YF5 YOJ YYP YZZ ZCA ZCG ZGI ZKG ZUP ZZTAW ~A~ ~IA ~KM ~WP AAHQN AAIPD AAMMB AAMNL AANHP AAYCA ABAWQ ACHJO ACRPL ACUHS ACYXJ ADNMO AEFGJ AEYWJ AFWVQ AGQPQ AGXDD AIDQK AIDYY ALVPJ AFYRF YR5 AAYXX AGHNM CITATION ABDSA IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM VXZ 7QJ 7TK 8BJ AHOVV FQK JBE U9A 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c5704-6468b633748c0e1695babea003822f0644a31d12924d66aa3b2f2e98aef784a13 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 0009-3920 |
IngestDate | Mon Jul 21 09:32:16 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 19:14:13 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 23:49:16 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 09:26:55 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 01:51:22 EST 2025 Wed Mar 05 09:27:44 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 04:10:58 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:10:21 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:49:38 EST 2025 Thu Jul 03 21:32:01 EDT 2025 Wed Oct 30 10:02:10 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 5 |
Keywords | Human Preschool age Stimulus localization Theory Cognition Neural network Dynamical system Experimental study School age Cognitive development Working memory Child Spatial memory |
Language | English |
License | CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c5704-6468b633748c0e1695babea003822f0644a31d12924d66aa3b2f2e98aef784a13 |
Notes | istex:9C6B91106371A70280367AA0CC5CD522EF514437 ArticleID:CDEV614 ark:/67375/WNG-MMNRQRQJ-W SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 14 ObjectType-Article-2 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 |
PMID | 14552405 |
PQID | 198712037 |
PQPubID | 35183 |
PageCount | 25 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_71260666 proquest_miscellaneous_57065868 proquest_miscellaneous_38532228 proquest_journals_198712037 pubmed_primary_14552405 pascalfrancis_primary_15163152 crossref_primary_10_1111_1467_8624_00614 crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_1467_8624_00614 wiley_primary_10_1111_1467_8624_00614_CDEV614 jstor_primary_3696185 istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_MMNRQRQJ_W |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2003-09-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2003-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 09 year: 2003 text: 2003-09-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Oxford, UK |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Oxford, UK – name: Malden, MA – name: United States – name: Ann Arbor |
PublicationTitle | Child development |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Child Dev |
PublicationYear | 2003 |
Publisher | Blackwell Publishing Blackwell Publishers Blackwell Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
Publisher_xml | – name: Blackwell Publishing – name: Blackwell Publishers – name: Blackwell – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
References | Suzuki, W. A., Miller, E. K., & Desimone, R. (1997). Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 1062-1081. Di Pellegrino, G., & Wise, S. P. (1993). Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1227-1243. McDowell, K., Jeka, J. J., Schöner, G., & Hatfield, B. D. (2002). Behavioral and electrocortical evidence of an interaction between probability and task metrics in movement preparation. Experimental Brain Research, 144, 303-313. Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Wang, X. J. (2000). Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 910-923. Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the A-not-B and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 267-317. Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (2002). Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychological Review, 109, 545-572. Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003a). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science, 6, 392-412. Bastian, A., Riehle, A., Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (1998). Prior information preshapes the population representation of movement direction in motor cortex. NeuroReport, 9, 315-319. Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychological Review, 106, 235-260. Spencer, J. P., & Schutte, A. R. (in press). Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses? Psychological Science. Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex-Developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195-205. Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Spinnazzola, J. (1998). Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Developmental Psychology, 34, 203-214. Amari, S. (1977). Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biological Cybernetics, 27, 77-87. Hund, A. M., & Spencer, J. P. (2003). Developmental changes in the relative weighting of geometric and experience-dependent location cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 3-38. Munakata, Y., McClelland, J. L., Johnson, M. H., & Siegler, R. S. (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. Psychological Review, 104, 686-719. Wilson, F. A. W., Scalaidhe, S. P., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 260, 1955-1958. Erlhagen, W., Bastian, A., Jancke, D., Riehle, A., & Schöner, G. (1999). The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 94, 53-66. Munakata, Y., & McClelland, J. L. (2003). Connectionist models of development. Developmental Science, 6, 413-429. Diamond, A., Cruttenden, L., & Neiderman, D. (1994). AB with multiple wells: 1. Why are multiple wells sometimes easier than two wells? 2. Memory or memory +inhibition? Developmental Psychology, 30, 192-205. Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development, 70, 1297-1313. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Bartsch, K. (1987). Infant search and object permanence: A meta-analysis of the A-not-B error. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, (3, Serial No. 214). Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220-241. Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11-28. Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (in press) Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall. Cognitive Psychology. Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., & Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development, 13, 185-200. Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic. Georgopoulos, A. P., Taira, M., & Lukashin, A. (1993). Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex. Science, 260, 47-52. Munakata, Y. (1998). Infant perseveration and implications for object permanence theories: A PDP model of the AB task. Developmental Science, 1, 161-184. Diamond, A., & Doar, B. (1989). The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 271-294. Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. B. (2001). A dynamic field theory of infant perseverative reaching errors. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 1-86. Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (2002). Prototypes and particulars: Geometric and experience-dependent spatial categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 16-37. Tanaka, S. (2000). Roles of intracortical inhibition in the formation of spatially tuned delay-period activity of prefrontal cortical neurons: Computational study. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 24, 483-504. Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review, 98, 352-376. Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2000, August). A dynamic field model of location memory. Paper presented at the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia. Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003b). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science. Georgopoulos, A. P., Kettner, R. E., & Schwartz, A. B. (1988). Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2928-2937. Spencer, J. P., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2001). Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of 2-year-olds. Child Development, 72, 1327-1346. Schutte, A. R., & Spencer, J. P. (2002). Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A-not-B error beyond infancy: Three-year-olds' delay- and experience-dependent location memory biases. Child Development, 73, 377-404. Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28, 517-527. Tipper, S. P. (1992). Selection for action: The role of inhibitory mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 105-109. Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. H. (1994). The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115-147. 2001; 72 1989; 22 1997; 119 1987; 51 1990; 14 2002; 131 1991; 98 1977; 27 2002; 73 2000; 24 1990; 608 1993; 260 1954 1994; 27 2003b 1999; 106 2001; 24 2003a; 6 2000, August 1993; 13 1997; 104 1990; 28 2000; 10 2003; 6 2000; 129 1988; 8 2002; 144 1997; 78 2003; 4 1998; 1 1999; 94 2002; 109 1999; 70 1979; 163 1992; 1 1994; 30 1998; 34 1998; 9 1998; 13 Wellman H. M. (e_1_2_1_41_1) 1987; 51 Spencer J. P. (e_1_2_1_32_1) 2000 e_1_2_1_42_1 e_1_2_1_20_1 Spencer J. P. (e_1_2_1_31_1) e_1_2_1_40_1 e_1_2_1_23_1 e_1_2_1_24_1 e_1_2_1_21_1 e_1_2_1_22_1 e_1_2_1_43_1 e_1_2_1_27_1 e_1_2_1_28_1 e_1_2_1_25_1 e_1_2_1_26_1 Spencer J. P. (e_1_2_1_35_1) e_1_2_1_7_1 Smith L. B. (e_1_2_1_29_1) 1999; 106 e_1_2_1_8_1 e_1_2_1_30_1 e_1_2_1_5_1 e_1_2_1_6_1 Huttenlocher J. (e_1_2_1_14_1) 1991; 98 e_1_2_1_3_1 e_1_2_1_12_1 e_1_2_1_4_1 e_1_2_1_13_1 e_1_2_1_34_1 e_1_2_1_10_1 e_1_2_1_33_1 e_1_2_1_2_1 e_1_2_1_11_1 e_1_2_1_16_1 e_1_2_1_39_1 e_1_2_1_17_1 e_1_2_1_38_1 e_1_2_1_37_1 e_1_2_1_15_1 e_1_2_1_36_1 e_1_2_1_9_1 e_1_2_1_18_1 e_1_2_1_19_1 |
References_xml | – reference: Tipper, S. P. (1992). Selection for action: The role of inhibitory mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 105-109. – reference: Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B., & Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development, 13, 185-200. – reference: Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220-241. – reference: Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11-28. – reference: Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the A-not-B and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 267-317. – reference: Munakata, Y. (1998). Infant perseveration and implications for object permanence theories: A PDP model of the AB task. Developmental Science, 1, 161-184. – reference: Di Pellegrino, G., & Wise, S. P. (1993). Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1227-1243. – reference: Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex-Developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195-205. – reference: Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S., & Spinnazzola, J. (1998). Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Developmental Psychology, 34, 203-214. – reference: Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Wang, X. J. (2000). Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 910-923. – reference: Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (2002). Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychological Review, 109, 545-572. – reference: Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Sandberg, E. H. (1994). The coding of spatial location in young children. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115-147. – reference: Erlhagen, W., Bastian, A., Jancke, D., Riehle, A., & Schöner, G. (1999). The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 94, 53-66. – reference: Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. B. (2001). A dynamic field theory of infant perseverative reaching errors. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 1-86. – reference: Diamond, A., & Doar, B. (1989). The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 271-294. – reference: Schutte, A. R., & Spencer, J. P. (2002). Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A-not-B error beyond infancy: Three-year-olds' delay- and experience-dependent location memory biases. Child Development, 73, 377-404. – reference: Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic. – reference: Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychological Review, 106, 235-260. – reference: Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: Prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review, 98, 352-376. – reference: Amari, S. (1977). Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biological Cybernetics, 27, 77-87. – reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003a). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science, 6, 392-412. – reference: Tanaka, S. (2000). Roles of intracortical inhibition in the formation of spatially tuned delay-period activity of prefrontal cortical neurons: Computational study. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 24, 483-504. – reference: Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (2002). Prototypes and particulars: Geometric and experience-dependent spatial categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 16-37. – reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2000, August). A dynamic field model of location memory. Paper presented at the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia. – reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2003b). Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development. Developmental Science. – reference: Bastian, A., Riehle, A., Erlhagen, W., & Schöner, G. (1998). Prior information preshapes the population representation of movement direction in motor cortex. NeuroReport, 9, 315-319. – reference: Spencer, J. P., & Schutte, A. R. (in press). Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses? Psychological Science. – reference: Diamond, A., Cruttenden, L., & Neiderman, D. (1994). AB with multiple wells: 1. Why are multiple wells sometimes easier than two wells? 2. Memory or memory +inhibition? Developmental Psychology, 30, 192-205. – reference: Georgopoulos, A. P., Taira, M., & Lukashin, A. (1993). Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex. Science, 260, 47-52. – reference: Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development, 70, 1297-1313. – reference: Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28, 517-527. – reference: Georgopoulos, A. P., Kettner, R. E., & Schwartz, A. B. (1988). Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 2928-2937. – reference: McDowell, K., Jeka, J. J., Schöner, G., & Hatfield, B. D. (2002). Behavioral and electrocortical evidence of an interaction between probability and task metrics in movement preparation. Experimental Brain Research, 144, 303-313. – reference: Spencer, J. P., Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2001). Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of 2-year-olds. Child Development, 72, 1327-1346. – reference: Wilson, F. A. W., Scalaidhe, S. P., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 260, 1955-1958. – reference: Spencer, J. P., & Hund, A. M. (in press) Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall. Cognitive Psychology. – reference: Suzuki, W. A., Miller, E. K., & Desimone, R. (1997). Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 1062-1081. – reference: Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Bartsch, K. (1987). Infant search and object permanence: A meta-analysis of the A-not-B error. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, (3, Serial No. 214). – reference: Hund, A. M., & Spencer, J. P. (2003). Developmental changes in the relative weighting of geometric and experience-dependent location cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 3-38. – reference: Munakata, Y., McClelland, J. L., Johnson, M. H., & Siegler, R. S. (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. Psychological Review, 104, 686-719. – reference: Munakata, Y., & McClelland, J. L. (2003). Connectionist models of development. Developmental Science, 6, 413-429. – volume: 6 start-page: 413 year: 2003 end-page: 429 article-title: Connectionist models of development publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 78 start-page: 1062 year: 1997 end-page: 1081 article-title: Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology – volume: 27 start-page: 115 year: 1994 end-page: 147 article-title: The coding of spatial location in young children publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 24 start-page: 1 year: 2001 end-page: 86 article-title: A dynamic field theory of infant perseverative reaching errors publication-title: Behavioral and Brain Sciences – year: 2000, August – volume: 9 start-page: 315 year: 1998 end-page: 319 article-title: Prior information preshapes the population representation of movement direction in motor cortex publication-title: NeuroReport – volume: 30 start-page: 192 year: 1994 end-page: 205 article-title: AB with multiple wells publication-title: Developmental Psychology – article-title: Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses? publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 98 start-page: 352 year: 1991 end-page: 376 article-title: Categories and particulars publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 163 start-page: 195 year: 1979 end-page: 205 article-title: Synaptic density in human frontal cortex—Developmental changes and effects of aging publication-title: Brain Research – volume: 72 start-page: 1327 year: 2001 end-page: 1346 article-title: Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A‐not‐B error publication-title: Child Development – volume: 10 start-page: 910 year: 2000 end-page: 923 article-title: Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – volume: 14 start-page: 11 year: 1990 end-page: 28 article-title: Maturational constraints on language learning publication-title: Cognitive Science – year: 2003b article-title: Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 129 start-page: 220 year: 2000 end-page: 241 article-title: Why do categories affect stimulus judgement? publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – year: 1954 – volume: 94 start-page: 53 year: 1999 end-page: 66 article-title: The distribution of neuronal population activation (DPA) as a tool to study interaction and integration in cortical representations publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods – article-title: Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 260 start-page: 1955 year: 1993 end-page: 1958 article-title: Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex publication-title: Science – volume: 6 start-page: 392 year: 2003a end-page: 412 article-title: Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 51 year: 1987 article-title: Infant search and object permanence publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development – volume: 34 start-page: 203 year: 1998 end-page: 214 article-title: Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task publication-title: Developmental Psychology – volume: 13 start-page: 1227 year: 1993 end-page: 1243 article-title: Visuospatial versus visuomotor activity in the premotor and prefrontal cortex of a primate publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 13 start-page: 185 year: 1998 end-page: 200 article-title: The development of spatial location coding publication-title: Cognitive Development – volume: 119 start-page: 271 year: 1997 end-page: 310 – volume: 131 start-page: 16 year: 2002 end-page: 37 article-title: Prototypes and particulars publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 28 start-page: 517 year: 1990 end-page: 527 article-title: Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 1 start-page: 105 year: 1992 end-page: 109 article-title: Selection for action publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science – volume: 70 start-page: 1297 year: 1999 end-page: 1313 article-title: The A‐not‐B error publication-title: Child Development – volume: 1 start-page: 161 year: 1998 end-page: 184 article-title: Infant perseveration and implications for object permanence theories publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 144 start-page: 303 year: 2002 end-page: 313 article-title: Behavioral and electrocortical evidence of an interaction between probability and task metrics in movement preparation publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 106 start-page: 235 year: 1999 end-page: 260 article-title: Knowing in the context of acting publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 22 start-page: 271 year: 1989 end-page: 294 article-title: The performance of human infants on a measure of frontal cortex function, the delayed response task publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – volume: 73 start-page: 377 year: 2002 end-page: 404 article-title: Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A‐not‐B error beyond infancy publication-title: Child Development – volume: 608 start-page: 267 year: 1990 end-page: 317 article-title: The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the A‐not‐B and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences – volume: 24 start-page: 483 year: 2000 end-page: 504 article-title: Roles of intracortical inhibition in the formation of spatially tuned delay‐period activity of prefrontal cortical neurons publication-title: Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry – volume: 260 start-page: 47 year: 1993 end-page: 52 article-title: Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex publication-title: Science – volume: 4 start-page: 3 year: 2003 end-page: 38 article-title: Developmental changes in the relative weighting of geometric and experience‐dependent location cues publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development – volume: 109 start-page: 545 year: 2002 end-page: 572 article-title: Dynamic field theory of movement preparation publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 104 start-page: 686 year: 1997 end-page: 719 article-title: Rethinking infant knowledge publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 27 start-page: 77 year: 1977 end-page: 87 article-title: Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral‐inhibition type neural fields publication-title: Biological Cybernetics – volume: 8 start-page: 2928 year: 1988 end-page: 2937 article-title: Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three‐dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – ident: e_1_2_1_42_1 doi: 10.1126/science.8316836 – volume-title: A dynamic field model of location memory year: 2000 ident: e_1_2_1_32_1 – ident: e_1_2_1_23_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.4.686 – ident: e_1_2_1_27_1 doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(97)80011-8 – ident: e_1_2_1_30_1 doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.1.16 – ident: e_1_2_1_24_1 doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90038-7 – ident: e_1_2_1_2_1 doi: 10.1007/BF00337259 – ident: e_1_2_1_4_1 doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.9.910 – ident: e_1_2_1_34_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00295 – ident: e_1_2_1_12_1 doi: 10.1126/science.8465199 – ident: e_1_2_1_13_1 doi: 10.1080/15248372.2003.9669681 – ident: e_1_2_1_36_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00351 – volume: 51 year: 1987 ident: e_1_2_1_41_1 article-title: Infant search and object permanence publication-title: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development – ident: e_1_2_1_31_1 article-title: Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – ident: e_1_2_1_19_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00095 – ident: e_1_2_1_20_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1046-4 – ident: e_1_2_1_10_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.545 – ident: e_1_2_1_18_1 doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90031-I – ident: e_1_2_1_25_1 doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2 – ident: e_1_2_1_8_1 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-01227.1993 – ident: e_1_2_1_21_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00021 – ident: e_1_2_1_22_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00296 – ident: e_1_2_1_43_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.203 – ident: e_1_2_1_39_1 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01003910 – ident: e_1_2_1_35_1 article-title: Do perseverative biases in early development solely reflect prepotent responses? publication-title: Psychological Science – ident: e_1_2_1_38_1 doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00088-9 – ident: e_1_2_1_7_1 doi: 10.1002/dev.420220307 – ident: e_1_2_1_26_1 doi: 10.1037/11168-000 – ident: e_1_2_1_6_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.192 – ident: e_1_2_1_33_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00295 – volume: 98 start-page: 352 year: 1991 ident: e_1_2_1_14_1 article-title: Categories and particulars publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.352 – ident: e_1_2_1_15_1 doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220 – ident: e_1_2_1_16_1 doi: 10.1006/cogp.1994.1014 – ident: e_1_2_1_40_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768813 – volume: 106 start-page: 235 year: 1999 ident: e_1_2_1_29_1 article-title: Knowing in the context of acting publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.235 – ident: e_1_2_1_37_1 doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1062 – ident: e_1_2_1_3_1 doi: 10.1097/00001756-199801260-00025 – ident: e_1_2_1_5_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48900.x – ident: e_1_2_1_9_1 doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00125-9 – ident: e_1_2_1_28_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00413 – ident: e_1_2_1_11_1 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-08-02928.1988 – ident: e_1_2_1_17_1 doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4 |
SSID | ssj0002693 |
Score | 2.16693 |
Snippet | The dynamic field theory predicts that biases toward remembered locations depend on the separation between targets, and the spatial precision of interactions... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref wiley jstor istex |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 1393 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Age differences Age groups Association Learning Attention Biological and medical sciences Child Child development Child, Preschool Children Cognition Development Developmental psychology Distance Perception Dynamic field theory Empirical Articles Experimentation Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Male Memory Memory, Short-Term Modeling Neural Networks (Computer) Orientation Overdevelopment Problem Solving Psychology Psychology, Child Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Psychomotor Performance Retention (Psychology) Space Space Perception Spatial analysis Spatial location Spatial models Stereotyped Behavior T tests Task performance Toys Working memory Young children |
Title | Testing the Dynamic Field Theory: Working Memory for Locations Becomes More Spatially Precise Over Development |
URI | https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-MMNRQRQJ-W/fulltext.pdf https://www.jstor.org/stable/3696185 https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2F1467-8624.00614 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14552405 https://www.proquest.com/docview/198712037 https://www.proquest.com/docview/38532228 https://www.proquest.com/docview/57065868 https://www.proquest.com/docview/71260666 |
Volume | 74 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1bb9MwFD5CQ0J74VIGyzaGH9DES6rm3vAG28o0kcKqjfJm2YmNULsENa1E-PWc41y6TgyEeIqUnMSJfY7z2f78HYBXMvL8SKgY41vgAEX7mS2lcmytlHCFVFGY0opuMg7PrvzzL0HLJqS9MLU-RDfhRpFh-msKcCHLG0FuQpy2NxA1y6SyJsYWwaLJWkDKDeMulxoigUEj7kNcnlv3b_yX7lMV_2gpisSXFCVWma5zXfwOjG5iW_NzGj0C2X5WzUmZ9VdL2U9_3lJ8_K_vfgwPG-jK3ta-9gTuqbwHD5Jmcb4H211nWvXgoN73y6ZqrsVCsSPWnigWsx7sdRtl1ldqxZLqKeSXJP2Rf2UITtlJlYvrbykbEdWO1VICb1gzx88SIgpXDJE3-1A0s4_sHQ6pr1XJkgILppzL-PR5xT6RkEep2EeMXnaDLLUDV6PTy-Mzu8kLYadBNPDt0A-HMvRIOCcdKCeMA4leJWiR03U1YixfeE6GQMb1szAUwpOudlU8FEpHQ1843jPYyotc7QITOk5xiKscJ84QSmUiCpSrhUaU50VaZBb0W6_gaSOaTrk75rwdPFFzcGoObprDgtfdDd9rvZC7TY-Mm3V2YjEjml0U8On4PU-S8eRicnHOpxbsGD_sDCkHI-IsCw43_HJdYIAoG4GZBfuto_Kmayo5zTI57sCLLHjZXcU-hRaKRK6KVck9xHA0M3i3RUCr48PwDxZYhhkbW_C8jpD12_lBgDgSX982fv63euLHJ6ef8bj3j_b7sG2Yl4YQeABby8VKvUAEuZSHppP4BeDQYlo |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMw1V1Lb9NAEB6VVoJeeIQCbku7B6i4OIrfMRIHaBrSNg40SpvelrW9rqoEB8WJwPws_gp_iBm_0lQUxKEHTpHisXe1u7P7ze633wC88B3DdIR00b8FBiiRGaq-LzU1klLowpeOHdCJrtezO6fm0bl1vgI_yrswuT5EteFGnpHN1-TgtCF9xcszH6f7DcTN0syCWHks068YtiVvDlvYxy91vX0w2O-oRWYBNbCchqnapt30bYOkV4KG1GzX8rFego7JdD3CVdoUhhbiUqiboW0LYfh6pEu3KWTkNE2hGfjdO7BGecRJr7_VX0hW6bZbZW9D7NEo5ISIPXStwksr4Rp16reSFEkMTZFgJ0V5do3fwd9lNJ0th-0H8LNsyJwFM6rPZ349-H5NY_L_aumHcL9A5-xt7k6PYEXGNbjrFfyDGqxX60Vag-38ajMbynEkppLtsfKPyXRUg83qLtDiSS7Kkj6GeEDqJvEFQ_zNWmksPl8GrE1sQparJbxmxTEG84gLnTIMLlh3UmywsncSG1EmzJtgwZRWGr8-TtlH0ipJJPuAExS7wgfbgNNbabYnsBpPYvkMmIjcAKN4qWluiGgxFI4l9UhECGQNJxKhAvVyGPKg0IWn9CRjXsaH1P2cup9n3a_Aq-qFL7kkys2me9m4ruzEdERMQsfiw9577nm9_kn_5IgPFdjIBn5lSGkmEUoqsLPkCIsCLQwkEHsqsFV6Bi9m34TTRpqmNwxHgd3qKU6bdBYmYjmZJ9xAmEqbnzdbWEQAaNp_sMAysvBfgae5Sy5qZ1oWQmWsvpo51t_aie-3Ds7wd_Mf7XfhXmfgdXn3sHe8BesZ0TTjP27D6mw6l88RMM_8nWyGYvDpth31F5Bpvb0 |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMw1V1Lb9NAEB6VVqp64REKuC3tHqDi4ih-x0gcoGnoy6GN2qa3ZW3vIpTgVHEqMP-Kv8IvYsavNBUFceiBU6R47F3t7Ox-s_PtDMCL0LNsT0gf7Vugg6LsWA9DaehKSmGKUHpuRBHdoOfundkHF87FAvyo7sIU-SHqAzeyjHy9JgO_jNU1I89NnK43EDXLsEte5aHMvqLXlr7Z76CKX5pmd_d0Z08vCwvokeO1bN213XboWpR5JWpJw_WdELslKEpmmgo3aVtYRow7oWnHriuEFZrKlH5bSOW1bWFY-N17sGS7LZ-qRXT6s4xVpuvXxdsQerTKbEJEHrrR4bmNcIl0-q3iRBJBU6SoI1UU1_gd-p0H0_lu2H0AP6txLEgww-bVNGxG32-kmPyvBvoh3C-xOXtbGNMjWJBJA5aDkn3QgJV6t8gasFFcbGYDOVJiItk2q_4YT4YNWKtvAs2eFClZsseQnFJuk-QTQ_TNOlkivnyOWJe4hKzIlfCalUEMFhATOmPoWrCjcXm8yt5JHEOZsmCMDVNRafz6KGPHlKkklewDLk_sGhtsFc7uZNiewGIyTuQzYEL5Efrw0jD8GLFiLDxHmkoohLGWp0SsQbOahTwqs8JTcZIRr7xDUj8n9fNc_Rq8ql-4LBKi3C66nU_rWk5MhsQj9Bw-6L3nQdDrn_RPDvhAg9V83teCVGQSgaQGm3N2MGvQQTcCkacG65Vh8HLtTTkdoxlmy_I02Kqf4qJJkTCRyPFVyi0EqXT0ebuEQ-H_tvsHCWwjd_41eFpY5Kx3tuMgUMbu67ld_W2c-E5n9xx_1_5RfguWjztdfrTfO1yHlZxlmpMfN2BxOrmSzxEtT8PNfH1i8PGu7fQXdAu8bA |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Testing+the+dynamic+field+theory%3A+working+memory+for+locations+becomes+more+spatially+precise+over+development&rft.jtitle=Child+development&rft.au=Schutte%2C+Anne+R&rft.au=Spencer%2C+John+P&rft.au=Sch%C3%B6ner%2C+Gregor&rft.date=2003-09-01&rft.issn=0009-3920&rft.volume=74&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=1393&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2F1467-8624.00614&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0009-3920&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0009-3920&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0009-3920&client=summon |