Language and task switching in the bilingual brain: Bilinguals are staying, not switching, experts

Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inNeuropsychologia Vol. 66; no. Jan; pp. 193 - 203
Main Authors Weissberger, Gali H., Gollan, Tamar H., Bondi, Mark W., Clark, Lindsay R., Wierenga, Christina E.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published England Elsevier Ltd 01.01.2015
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0028-3932
1873-3514
1873-3514
DOI10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing the possibility that some control mechanisms are unique to language. We investigated this hypothesis by directly comparing the neural correlates of task switching and language switching. Nineteen Spanish–English bilingual university students underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study employing a hybrid (event-related and blocked) design involving both color-shape switching and language switching paradigms. We compared the two switching tasks using within-subject voxel-wise t-tests for each of three trial types (single trials in single blocks, and stay and switch trials in mixed blocks). Comparing trial types to baseline in each task revealed widespread activation for single, stay, and switch trials in both color-shape and language switching. Direct comparisons of each task for each trial type revealed few differences between tasks on single and switch trials, but large task differences during stay trials, with more widespread activation for the non-linguistic than for the language task. Our results confirm previous suggestions of shared mechanisms of switching across domains, but also reveal bilinguals have greater efficiency for sustaining the inhibition of the non-target language than the non-target task when two responses are available. This efficiency of language control might arise from bilinguals' need to control interference from the non-target language specifically when not switching languages, when speaking in single- or mixed-language contexts. •Authors examined the relationship between bilingual language-switching and task-switching.•Single/stay/switch trials were compared across tasks.•Greatest differences in activation were found between language stay and color-shape stay trials.•Findings suggest greater efficiency for maintaining 2 languages, than 2 non-linguistic tasks available for response.•Bilinguals are experts at “staying” in a single language when faced with linguistic alternatives.
AbstractList Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing the possibility that some control mechanisms are unique to language. We investigated this hypothesis by directly comparing the neural correlates of task switching and language switching. Nineteen Spanish-English bilingual university students underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study employing a hybrid (event-related and blocked) design involving both color-shape switching and language switching paradigms. We compared the two switching tasks using within-subject voxel-wise t-tests for each of three trial types (single trials in single blocks, and stay and switch trials in mixed blocks). Comparing trial types to baseline in each task revealed widespread activation for single, stay, and switch trials in both color-shape and language switching. Direct comparisons of each task for each trial type revealed few differences between tasks on single and switch trials, but large task differences during stay trials, with more widespread activation for the non-linguistic than for the language task. Our results confirm previous suggestions of shared mechanisms of switching across domains, but also reveal bilinguals have greater efficiency for sustaining the inhibition of the non-target language than the non-target task when two responses are available. This efficiency of language control might arise from bilinguals' need to control interference from the non-target language specifically when not switching languages, when speaking in single- or mixed-language contexts.
Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing the possibility that some control mechanisms are unique to language. We investigated this hypothesis by directly comparing the neural correlates of task switching and language switching. Nineteen Spanish-English bilingual university students underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study employing a hybrid (event-related and blocked) design involving both color-shape switching and language switching paradigms. We compared the two switching tasks using within-subject voxel-wise t-tests for each of three trial types (single trials in single blocks, and stay and switch trials in mixed blocks). Comparing trial types to baseline in each task revealed widespread activation for single, stay, and switch trials in both color-shape and language switching. Direct comparisons of each task for each trial type revealed few differences between tasks on single and switch trials, but large task differences during stay trials, with more widespread activation for the non-linguistic than for the language task. Our results confirm previous suggestions of shared mechanisms of switching across domains, but also reveal bilinguals have greater efficiency for sustaining the inhibition of the non-target language than the non-target task when two responses are available. This efficiency of language control might arise from bilinguals' need to control interference from the non-target language specifically when not switching languages, when speaking in single- or mixed-language contexts. [Copyright Elsevier Ltd.]
Bilinguals’ ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing the possibility that some control mechanisms are unique to language. We investigated this hypothesis by directly comparing the neural correlates of task switching and language switching. Nineteen Spanish-English bilingual university students underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study employing a hybrid (event-related and blocked) design involving both color-shape switching and language switching paradigms. We compared the two switching tasks using within-subject voxel-wise t-tests for each of three trial types (single trials in single blocks, and stay and switch trials in mixed blocks). Comparing trial types to baseline in each task revealed widespread activation for single, stay, and switch trials in both color-shape and language switching. Direct comparisons of each task for each trial type revealed few differences between tasks on single and switch trials, but large task differences during stay trials, with more widespread activation for the non-linguistic than for the language task. Our results confirm previous suggestions of shared mechanisms of switching across domains, but also reveal bilinguals have greater efficiency for sustaining the inhibition of the non-target language than the non-target task when two responses are available. This efficiency of language control might arise from bilinguals’ need to control interference from the non-target language specifically when not switching languages, when speaking in single- or mixed-language contexts.
Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing the possibility that some control mechanisms are unique to language. We investigated this hypothesis by directly comparing the neural correlates of task switching and language switching. Nineteen Spanish-English bilingual university students underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study employing a hybrid (event-related and blocked) design involving both color-shape switching and language switching paradigms. We compared the two switching tasks using within-subject voxel-wise t-tests for each of three trial types (single trials in single blocks, and stay and switch trials in mixed blocks). Comparing trial types to baseline in each task revealed widespread activation for single, stay, and switch trials in both color-shape and language switching. Direct comparisons of each task for each trial type revealed few differences between tasks on single and switch trials, but large task differences during stay trials, with more widespread activation for the non-linguistic than for the language task. Our results confirm previous suggestions of shared mechanisms of switching across domains, but also reveal bilinguals have greater efficiency for sustaining the inhibition of the non-target language than the non-target task when two responses are available. This efficiency of language control might arise from bilinguals' need to control interference from the non-target language specifically when not switching languages, when speaking in single- or mixed-language contexts.Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing the possibility that some control mechanisms are unique to language. We investigated this hypothesis by directly comparing the neural correlates of task switching and language switching. Nineteen Spanish-English bilingual university students underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study employing a hybrid (event-related and blocked) design involving both color-shape switching and language switching paradigms. We compared the two switching tasks using within-subject voxel-wise t-tests for each of three trial types (single trials in single blocks, and stay and switch trials in mixed blocks). Comparing trial types to baseline in each task revealed widespread activation for single, stay, and switch trials in both color-shape and language switching. Direct comparisons of each task for each trial type revealed few differences between tasks on single and switch trials, but large task differences during stay trials, with more widespread activation for the non-linguistic than for the language task. Our results confirm previous suggestions of shared mechanisms of switching across domains, but also reveal bilinguals have greater efficiency for sustaining the inhibition of the non-target language than the non-target task when two responses are available. This efficiency of language control might arise from bilinguals' need to control interference from the non-target language specifically when not switching languages, when speaking in single- or mixed-language contexts.
Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task switching. However, recent studies also reveal some limitations on the extent control mechanisms are shared across domains, introducing the possibility that some control mechanisms are unique to language. We investigated this hypothesis by directly comparing the neural correlates of task switching and language switching. Nineteen Spanish–English bilingual university students underwent a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study employing a hybrid (event-related and blocked) design involving both color-shape switching and language switching paradigms. We compared the two switching tasks using within-subject voxel-wise t-tests for each of three trial types (single trials in single blocks, and stay and switch trials in mixed blocks). Comparing trial types to baseline in each task revealed widespread activation for single, stay, and switch trials in both color-shape and language switching. Direct comparisons of each task for each trial type revealed few differences between tasks on single and switch trials, but large task differences during stay trials, with more widespread activation for the non-linguistic than for the language task. Our results confirm previous suggestions of shared mechanisms of switching across domains, but also reveal bilinguals have greater efficiency for sustaining the inhibition of the non-target language than the non-target task when two responses are available. This efficiency of language control might arise from bilinguals' need to control interference from the non-target language specifically when not switching languages, when speaking in single- or mixed-language contexts. •Authors examined the relationship between bilingual language-switching and task-switching.•Single/stay/switch trials were compared across tasks.•Greatest differences in activation were found between language stay and color-shape stay trials.•Findings suggest greater efficiency for maintaining 2 languages, than 2 non-linguistic tasks available for response.•Bilinguals are experts at “staying” in a single language when faced with linguistic alternatives.
Author Clark, Lindsay R.
Weissberger, Gali H.
Gollan, Tamar H.
Wierenga, Christina E.
Bondi, Mark W.
AuthorAffiliation 1 San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego, Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, 6363 Alvarado Ct. #103, San Diego, 92120-4913
2 Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., MC: 0603, La Jolla, CA 92093-0603
3 VA San Diego Healthcare System, 3350 La Jolla Village Dr., San Diego, 92161
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: 2 Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., MC: 0603, La Jolla, CA 92093-0603
– name: 1 San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego, Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, 6363 Alvarado Ct. #103, San Diego, 92120-4913
– name: 3 VA San Diego Healthcare System, 3350 La Jolla Village Dr., San Diego, 92161
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Gali H.
  surname: Weissberger
  fullname: Weissberger, Gali H.
  organization: San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego, Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, 6363 Alvarado Ct. #103, San Diego 92120-4913, United States
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Tamar H.
  surname: Gollan
  fullname: Gollan, Tamar H.
  organization: Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., MC: 0603, La Jolla, CA 92093-0603, United States
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Mark W.
  surname: Bondi
  fullname: Bondi, Mark W.
  organization: Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., MC: 0603, La Jolla, CA 92093-0603, United States
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Lindsay R.
  surname: Clark
  fullname: Clark, Lindsay R.
  organization: San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego, Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, 6363 Alvarado Ct. #103, San Diego 92120-4913, United States
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Christina E.
  surname: Wierenga
  fullname: Wierenga, Christina E.
  email: cwierenga@ucsd.edu
  organization: Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., MC: 0603, La Jolla, CA 92093-0603, United States
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25446970$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqNkktv1DAURi1URKeFv4C8QiyawY4fiVkgQXkUaSQ2sLYc5ybjIWMPtlOYf0-i6bTAhllZ9j0-ftzvAp354AGhF5QsKaHy1WbpYYxhl_Z2HYbQO7MsCeVTcUlY9QgtaF2xggnKz9CCkLIumGLlObpIaUMI4aKsn6DzUnAuVUUWqFkZ34-mB2x8i7NJ33H66bJdO99j53FeA27c4GZowE00zr_G744LCZsIOGWzn-ZX2If8sPsKw68dxJyeosfdhMKzu_ESffv44ev1TbH68unz9dtVYYVUueCia6Qsje2kZa1kUAPU3Da14LLplFHQ2Fp1omxoyZRkHDgoKypBlQRFG3aJ3hy8u7HZQmvB52gGvYtua-JeB-P03xXv1roPt5oLJauSTIKXd4IYfoyQst66ZGEYjIcwJk0rVvOKCiX-j0pJOJOipiegfOpsLUg1oc__fMH91Y_tmoD3B8DGkFKETluXTXZhfpAbNCV6Tone6H9ToueUzPUpJQ8_da85nnSy4OYggKmftw6iTtaBt9C6CDbrNrhTVb8BhBHnkw
CODEN NUPSA6
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2016_01_016
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_13879
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_716336
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728921001061
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728924000841
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728923000755
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2024_105518
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728923000834
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_01832
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2021_02_029
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_0553_17_2017
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2018_09_001
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107593
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728925000094
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2018_06_023
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107592
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2022_108204
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2017_03_005
crossref_primary_10_1177_13670069231170142
crossref_primary_10_1162_nol_a_00032
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728922000426
crossref_primary_10_2298_PSI210121029H
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_025_02672_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2019_12_014
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_00395
crossref_primary_10_3390_bs9120120
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2019_107131
crossref_primary_10_1080_13670050_2023_2210247
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2019_05_021
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2016_03_001
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10936_023_09958_z
crossref_primary_10_1017_S030500091900059X
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728916001097
crossref_primary_10_1155_2016_7069274
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_01839
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2016_06_008
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2019_104680
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2017_07_007
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci14040304
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728916000249
crossref_primary_10_1162_nol_a_00128
crossref_primary_10_1177_01427237211024220
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_01171
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2019_06_022
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10548_019_00735_7
crossref_primary_10_1186_s12868_021_00640_5
crossref_primary_10_1177_13670069221086302
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2015_04_002
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1355617715000521
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_amper_2023_100145
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2022_108273
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijpsycho_2020_04_020
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2018_10_001
crossref_primary_10_1017_S136672892100002X
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_910382
crossref_primary_10_1162_nol_a_00059
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00429_022_02608_5
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11682_024_00921_7
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728916000456
crossref_primary_10_1093_geronb_gbaa058
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2020_104754
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2597_15_2016
crossref_primary_10_5334_joc_186
crossref_primary_10_32321_cutad_1553753
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12865
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2020_105038
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0165029
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_49375_9
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2022_108430
crossref_primary_10_1017_S136672891700044X
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2016_1250927
crossref_primary_10_1177_1367006920951873
crossref_primary_10_1111_mbe_12362
crossref_primary_10_1017_S1366728918001013
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_01032
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2019_104665
Cites_doi 10.1073/pnas.92.7.2899
10.1080/20445911.2013.821993
10.1093/cercor/bhm182
10.1097/00001756-200107030-00054
10.1006/nimg.2001.0810
10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00466-5
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033
10.1006/brcg.1999.1088
10.1016/S0911-6044(97)00017-1
10.1006/nimg.2001.0873
10.1006/jmla.2001.2793
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5051-06.2007
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2828-08.2009
10.1016/S0911-6044(97)00018-3
10.1093/neucas/6.1.51
10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00054-X
10.1006/nimg.2002.1319
10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.004
10.3758/BF03204507
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.054
10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00029-4
10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014
10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181825b1f
10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.055
10.1006/brln.1999.2278
10.1037/a0028281
10.1080/01690965.2011.613209
10.1073/pnas.95.3.853
10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
10.1073/pnas.240460497
10.1016/j.bandl.2008.12.005
10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.08.007
10.1093/cercor/bhr287
10.1006/jmla.1998.2602
10.1080/20445911.2014.891597
10.1177/1529100610387084
10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.001
10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.137
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.049
10.1093/cercor/bhp206
10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003
10.1162/0898929053279559
10.1017/S1366728909990526
10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013
10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00399
10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00234
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.002
10.1126/science.1127761
10.1017/S1355617711000580
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2014 Elsevier Ltd
Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Copyright_xml – notice: 2014 Elsevier Ltd
– notice: Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
7TK
7T9
5PM
DOI 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)

Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
EISSN 1873-3514
EndPage 203
ExternalDocumentID PMC4596720
25446970
10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2014_10_037
S0028393214003972
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Journal Article
Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 HD050287
– fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: R01AG012674
– fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: P50 AG005131
– fundername: NIDCD NIH HHS
  grantid: R01DC011492
– fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 AG012674
– fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: K24AG026431
– fundername: NIDCD NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 DC011492
– fundername: NICHD NIH HHS
  grantid: HD050287
– fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: L30 AG028985
– fundername: NIA NIH HHS
  grantid: AG039177
GroupedDBID ---
--K
--M
--Z
-~X
.GJ
.~1
0R~
123
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29N
4.4
41~
457
4G.
53G
5RE
5VS
7-5
71M
8P~
9JM
9JO
AABNK
AACTN
AADFP
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAGJA
AAGUQ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AAXLA
AAXUO
AAYJJ
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABOYX
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACCUC
ACDAQ
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACIUM
ACNCT
ACRLP
ACXNI
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADIYS
ADMUD
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AETEA
AFFNX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AFYLN
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
C45
CS3
D0S
DU5
EBS
EFJIC
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
GBLVA
HMQ
HMW
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
IHE
J1W
KOM
L7B
M2V
M3V
M41
MO0
MOBAO
MVM
N9A
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OHT
OKEIE
OVD
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
Q38
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SCC
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SNS
SPCBC
SPS
SSB
SSN
SSY
SSZ
T5K
TEORI
TN5
UHB
UQL
WH7
WUQ
XJT
XOL
YQT
ZGI
ZKB
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADNMO
ADXHL
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
SSH
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EFKBS
EIF
NPM
7X8
7TK
7T9
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c569t-45fb662acf6c3d63e8ee84cb8546bf9a9ebc89f52b1239634e4e9c575196e91b3
IEDL.DBID .~1
ISSN 0028-3932
1873-3514
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:23:32 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 11:21:05 EDT 2025
Wed Jul 30 11:07:45 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 23:47:30 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 05:49:16 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:08:43 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 04:14:14 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:35:27 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue Jan
Keywords Task switching
fMRI
Bilingualism
Executive control
Linguistic control
Language English
License Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c569t-45fb662acf6c3d63e8ee84cb8546bf9a9ebc89f52b1239634e4e9c575196e91b3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037
PMID 25446970
PQID 1641018507
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 11
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4596720
proquest_miscellaneous_1738471595
proquest_miscellaneous_1660436581
proquest_miscellaneous_1641018507
pubmed_primary_25446970
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2014_10_037
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2014_10_037
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2014_10_037
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2015-01-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2015-01-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 01
  year: 2015
  text: 2015-01-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace England
PublicationPlace_xml – name: England
PublicationTitle Neuropsychologia
PublicationTitleAlternate Neuropsychologia
PublicationYear 2015
Publisher Elsevier Ltd
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Ltd
References Hyafil, Summerfield, Koechlin (bib36) 2009; 29
Costa, Hernández, Sebastián-Gallés (bib503) 2008; 106
Hull, Vaid (bib35) 2007; 45
Prior, Gollan (bib52) 2013; 25
Costa, Hernández, Costa-Faidella, Sebastián-Gallés (bib501) 2009; 113
Talairach, Tournoux (bib509) 1988
Hernandez, Martinez, Kohnert (bib31) 2000; 73
Wang, Kuhl, Chen, Dong (bib510) 2009; 47
Wang, Xue, Chen, Xue, Dong (bib58) 2007; 35
Petersen, van Mier, Fiez, Raichle (bib50) 1998; 95
Crosson (bib16) 1999; 40
Peterson, Kane, Alexander, Lacadie, Skudlarski, Leung, May, Gore (bib49) 2002; 13
Sohn, Ursu, Anderson, Stenger, Carter (bib56) 2000; 97
Christoffels, Firk, Schiller (bib500) 2007; 1147
Wang, Wang, Jiang, Wang, Wu (bib57) 2012; 19
Braver, Reynolds, Donaldson (bib10) 2003; 39
de Bruin, Roelofs, Dijkstra, FitzPatrick (bib18) 2014; 90
Prior, Gollan (bib51) 2011; 17
Corbetta, Shulman (bib14) 2002; 3
Fabbro, Peru, Skrap (bib22) 1997; 10
Oldfield (bib47) 1971; 9
Abutalebi, Green (bib3) 2007; 20
Hervais-Adelman, Moser-Mercer, Golestani (bib33) 2011; 2
Colomé (bib13) 2001; 45
MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, Carter (bib45) 2000; 288
Dosenbach, Visscher, Palmer, Miezin, Wenger, Kang, Petersen (bib20) 2006; 50
Hernandez (bib29) 2009; 109
Hernández, Martin, Barceló, Costa (bib32) 2013; 69
Suskauer, Simmonds, Caffo, Denckla, Pekar, Mostofsky (bib508) 2008; 47
Hosoda, Hanakawa, Nariai, Ohno, Honda (bib34) 2012; 25
Paap, Greenberg (bib507) 2013; 66
Gollan, Ferreira (bib26) 2009; 35
Meuter, Allport (bib506) 1999; 40
Weissberger, Wierenga, Bondi, Gollan (bib59) 2012; 27
Dove, Pollmann, Schubert, Wiggins, von Cramon (bib21) 2000; 9
Lungu, Binenstock, Pline, Yeaton, Carey (bib44) 2007; 27
Crinion, Turner, Grogan, Hanakawa, Noppeney, Devlin, Price (bib504) 2006; 312
Rodriguez-Fornells, van der Lugt, Rotte, Britti, Heinze, Münte (bib54) 2005; 17
Abutalebi, Miozzo, Cappa (bib4) 2000; 6
Klein, Milner, Zatorre, Meyer, Evans (bib39) 1995; 92
Liu, Banich, Jacobson, Tanabe (bib505) 2004; 22
Abutalebi, Annoni, Zimine, Pegna, Seghier, Lee-Jahnke, Lazeyras, Cappa, Kahteb (bib1) 2008; 18
Rubin, Meiran (bib55) 2005; 31
Prior, MacWhinney (bib53) 2010; 13
DiGirolamo, Kramer, Barad, Cepeda, Weissman, Milham, Wszalek, Cohen, Banich, Webb, Belopolsky, McAuley (bib19) 2001; 12
Hernandez, Dapretto, Mazziotta, Bookheimer (bib30) 2001; 14
Bialystok, Craik, Luk (bib7) 2008; 34
Birn, Saad, Bandettini (bib9) 2001; 14
Luk, Green, Abutalebi, Grady (bib43) 2012; 27
Calabria, Hernández, Branzi, Costa (bib11) 2012; 2
Fan, Flombaum, McCandliss, Thomas, Posner (bib23) 2003; 18
Guo, Liu, Misra, Kroll (bib28) 2011; 56
Kroll, Bobb, Misra, Guo (bib42) 2008; 2008
Cohen, MacWhinney, Flatt, Provost (bib12) 1993; 25
Cox (bib15) 1996; 29
Bialystok, Craik, Klein, Viswanathan (bib6) 2004; 19
Bialystok, Craik, Green, Gollan (bib5) 2009; 10
Paap (bib48) 2014; 26
Abutalebi, Della Rosa, Green, Hernandez, Scifo, Keim, cappa, Costa (bib2) 2012; 22
Crosson, Zawacki, Brinson, Lu, Sadek (bib17) 1997; 10
Jimura, Braver (bib37) 2010; 20
Wang (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib510) 2009; 47
Bialystok (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib6) 2004; 19
Hyafil (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib36) 2009; 29
Suskauer (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib508) 2008; 47
Rodriguez-Fornells (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib54) 2005; 17
Cohen (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib12) 1993; 25
Cox (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib15) 1996; 29
Corbetta (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib14) 2002; 3
Costa (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib501) 2009; 113
Weissberger (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib59) 2012; 27
Prior (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib52) 2013; 25
Oldfield (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib47) 1971; 9
Klein (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib39) 1995; 92
Abutalebi (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib1) 2008; 18
Bialystok (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib7) 2008; 34
Luk (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib43) 2012; 27
Hernández (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib32) 2013; 69
de Bruin (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib18) 2014; 90
Dosenbach (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib20) 2006; 50
DiGirolamo (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib19) 2001; 12
Talairach (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib509) 1988
Wang (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib57) 2012; 19
Abutalebi (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib2) 2012; 22
Crosson (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib16) 1999; 40
Birn (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib9) 2001; 14
Hernandez (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib31) 2000; 73
Gollan (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib26) 2009; 35
Kroll (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib42) 2008; 2008
Christoffels (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib500) 2007; 1147
Dove (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib21) 2000; 9
Prior (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib53) 2010; 13
Jimura (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib37) 2010; 20
Hernandez (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib29) 2009; 109
Crosson (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib17) 1997; 10
Hosoda (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib34) 2012; 25
Liu (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib505) 2004; 22
Meuter (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib506) 1999; 40
Sohn (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib56) 2000; 97
Abutalebi (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib3) 2007; 20
Petersen (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib50) 1998; 95
Wang (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib58) 2007; 35
Paap (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib507) 2013; 66
Bialystok (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib5) 2009; 10
Fan (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib23) 2003; 18
Abutalebi (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib4) 2000; 6
Guo (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib28) 2011; 56
MacDonald (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib45) 2000; 288
Paap (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib48) 2014; 26
Hervais-Adelman (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib33) 2011; 2
Fabbro (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib22) 1997; 10
Colomé (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib13) 2001; 45
Crinion (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib504) 2006; 312
Prior (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib51) 2011; 17
Calabria (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib11) 2012; 2
Lungu (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib44) 2007; 27
Rubin (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib55) 2005; 31
Hull (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib35) 2007; 45
Costa (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib503) 2008; 106
Peterson (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib49) 2002; 13
Braver (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib10) 2003; 39
Hernandez (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib30) 2001; 14
References_xml – volume: 288
  start-page: 1835
  year: 2000
  end-page: 1838
  ident: bib45
  article-title: Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 40
  start-page: 25
  year: 1999
  end-page: 40
  ident: bib506
  article-title: Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
– volume: 40
  start-page: 414
  year: 1999
  end-page: 438
  ident: bib16
  article-title: Subcortical mechanisms in language: lexical-semantic mechanisms and the thalamus
  publication-title: Brain Cogn.
– volume: 13
  start-page: 253
  year: 2010
  end-page: 262
  ident: bib53
  article-title: A bilingual advantage in task switching
  publication-title: Bilingualism: Lang. Cogn.
– volume: 31
  start-page: 1477
  year: 2005
  end-page: 1491
  ident: bib55
  article-title: On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn.
– volume: 6
  start-page: 51
  year: 2000
  end-page: 56
  ident: bib4
  article-title: Do subcortical structures control ‘Language Selection’ in polygots? Evidence from pathological language mixing
  publication-title: Neurocase
– volume: 1147
  start-page: 192
  year: 2007
  end-page: 208
  ident: bib500
  article-title: Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study
  publication-title: Brain Research
– volume: 27
  start-page: 3010
  year: 2007
  end-page: 3016
  ident: bib44
  article-title: Neural changes in control implementation of a continuous task
  publication-title: J. Neurosci.
– volume: 29
  start-page: 5135
  year: 2009
  end-page: 5142
  ident: bib36
  article-title: Two mechanisms for task switching in the prefrontal cortex
  publication-title: J. Neurosci.
– volume: 20
  start-page: 1420
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1431
  ident: bib37
  article-title: Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task-switching
  publication-title: Cereb. Cortex
– volume: 25
  start-page: 257
  year: 1993
  end-page: 271
  ident: bib12
  article-title: PsyScope: an interactive graphical system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers
  publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput.
– volume: 109
  start-page: 133
  year: 2009
  end-page: 140
  ident: bib29
  article-title: Language switching in the bilingual brain: What's next?
  publication-title: Brain Lang.
– volume: 73
  start-page: 421
  year: 2000
  end-page: 431
  ident: bib31
  article-title: In search of the language switch: an fMRI study of picture naming in Spanish–English bilinguals
  publication-title: Brain Lang.
– volume: 95
  start-page: 853
  year: 1998
  end-page: 860
  ident: bib50
  article-title: The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
– volume: 34
  start-page: 859
  year: 2008
  end-page: 873
  ident: bib7
  article-title: Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn.
– volume: 2008
  start-page: 416
  year: 2008
  end-page: 430
  ident: bib42
  article-title: Language selection in bilingual speech: evidence for inhibitory processes
  publication-title: Acta Psychol.
– volume: 27
  start-page: 959
  year: 2012
  end-page: 974
  ident: bib59
  article-title: Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals
  publication-title: Psychol. Aging
– volume: 20
  start-page: 242
  year: 2007
  end-page: 275
  ident: bib3
  article-title: Bilingual language production: the neurocognition of language representation and control
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
– volume: 35
  start-page: 640
  year: 2009
  end-page: 665
  ident: bib26
  article-title: Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn.
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1141
  year: 2008
  end-page: 1150
  ident: bib508
  article-title: fMRI of intrasubject variability in ADHD: Anomalous premotor activity with prefrontal compensation
  publication-title: J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
– volume: 50
  start-page: 799
  year: 2006
  end-page: 812
  ident: bib20
  article-title: A core system for the implementation of task sets
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 35
  start-page: 862
  year: 2007
  end-page: 870
  ident: bib58
  article-title: Neural bases of asymmetric language switching in second-language learners: an ER-fMRI study
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 3
  start-page: 201
  year: 2002
  end-page: 215
  ident: bib14
  article-title: Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain
  publication-title: Neuroscience
– volume: 19
  start-page: 290
  year: 2004
  end-page: 303
  ident: bib6
  article-title: Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task
  publication-title: Psychol. Aging
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1987
  year: 2007
  end-page: 2008
  ident: bib35
  article-title: Bilingual language lateralization: a meta-analytic tale of two hemispheres
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 39
  start-page: 713
  year: 2003
  end-page: 726
  ident: bib10
  article-title: Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 2
  start-page: 234
  year: 2011
  ident: bib33
  article-title: Executive control of language in the bilingual brain: integrating the evidence from neuroimaging to neuropsychology
  publication-title: Front. Psychol.
– volume: 12
  start-page: 2065
  year: 2001
  end-page: 2071
  ident: bib19
  article-title: General and task-specific frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: a fMRI investigation of task-switching
  publication-title: NeuroReport
– volume: 25
  start-page: 622
  year: 2013
  end-page: 645
  ident: bib52
  article-title: The elusive link between language control and executive control: a case of limited transfer
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Psychol.
– volume: 56
  start-page: 2309
  year: 2011
  ident: bib28
  article-title: Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 9
  start-page: 103
  year: 2000
  end-page: 109
  ident: bib21
  article-title: Prefrontal cortex in task switching: an event-related fMRI study
  publication-title: Cogn. Brain Res.
– volume: 10
  start-page: 277
  year: 1997
  end-page: 300
  ident: bib17
  article-title: Models of subcortical functions in language: current status
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
– volume: 13
  start-page: 427
  year: 2002
  end-page: 440
  ident: bib49
  article-title: An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks
  publication-title: Cogn. Brain Res.
– volume: 14
  start-page: 510
  year: 2001
  end-page: 520
  ident: bib30
  article-title: Language switching and language representation in Spanish–English bilinguals: an fMRI study
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1
  year: 2012
  end-page: 8
  ident: bib57
  article-title: Direct evidence of the left caudate's role in bilingual control: an intra-operative electrical stimulation study
  publication-title: Neurocase
– volume: 10
  start-page: 89
  year: 2009
  end-page: 129
  ident: bib5
  article-title: Bilingual minds
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci. Public Interest
– volume: 10
  start-page: 347
  year: 1997
  end-page: 367
  ident: bib22
  article-title: Language disorders in bilingual patients after thalamic lesions
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
– volume: 9
  start-page: 97
  year: 1971
  end-page: 113
  ident: bib47
  article-title: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 29
  start-page: 162
  year: 1996
  end-page: 173
  ident: bib15
  article-title: AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages
  publication-title: Comput. Biomed. Res.
– volume: 106
  start-page: 59
  year: 2008
  end-page: 86
  ident: bib503
  article-title: Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 17
  start-page: 422
  year: 2005
  end-page: 433
  ident: bib54
  article-title: Second language interferes with word production in fluent bilinguals: brain potential and functional imaging evidence
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci.
– volume: 47
  start-page: 414
  year: 2009
  end-page: 422
  ident: bib510
  article-title: Sustained and transient language control in the bilingual brain
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1097
  year: 2004
  end-page: 1106
  ident: bib505
  article-title: Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 26
  start-page: 242
  year: 2014
  end-page: 255
  ident: bib48
  article-title: The role of componential analysis, categorical hypothesizing, replicability and confirmation bias in testing for bilingual advantages in executive functioning
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Psychol.
– volume: 25
  start-page: 44
  year: 2012
  end-page: 61
  ident: bib34
  article-title: Neural mechanisms of language switch
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
– volume: 45
  start-page: 721
  year: 2001
  end-page: 736
  ident: bib13
  article-title: Lexical activation in bilingual's speech production: language-specific or language-independent?
  publication-title: J. Mem. Lang.
– volume: 90
  start-page: 348
  year: 2014
  end-page: 359
  ident: bib18
  article-title: Domain-general inhibition areas of the brain are involved in language switching: FMRI evidence from trilingual speakers
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 14
  start-page: 817
  year: 2001
  end-page: 826
  ident: bib9
  article-title: Spatial heterogeneity of the nonlinear dynamics in the FMRI BOLD response
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 69
  start-page: 257
  year: 2013
  end-page: 276
  ident: bib32
  article-title: Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching?
  publication-title: J. Mem. Lang.
– volume: 66
  start-page: 232
  year: 2013
  end-page: 258
  ident: bib507
  article-title: There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 92
  start-page: 2899
  year: 1995
  end-page: 2903
  ident: bib39
  article-title: The neural substrates underlying word generation: a bilingual functional-imaging study
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
– year: 1988
  ident: bib509
  publication-title: Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: An approach to cerebral imaging
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1496
  year: 2008
  end-page: 1505
  ident: bib1
  article-title: Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: an event-related fMRI study
  publication-title: Cereb. Cortex
– volume: 22
  start-page: 2076
  year: 2012
  end-page: 2086
  ident: bib2
  article-title: Bilingualism tunes theanterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring
  publication-title: Cereb. Cortex
– volume: 97
  start-page: 13448
  year: 2000
  end-page: 13453
  ident: bib56
  article-title: The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
– volume: 2
  start-page: 399
  year: 2012
  ident: bib11
  article-title: Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: evidence from task-switching
  publication-title: Front. Psychol.
– volume: 17
  start-page: 682
  year: 2011
  end-page: 691
  ident: bib51
  article-title: Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals
  publication-title: J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc.
– volume: 27
  start-page: 1479
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1788
  ident: bib43
  article-title: Cognitive control for language switching in bilinguals: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies
  publication-title: Lang. Cogn. Processes
– volume: 113
  start-page: 135
  year: 2009
  end-page: 149
  ident: bib501
  article-title: On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 312
  start-page: 1537
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1540
  ident: bib504
  article-title: Language control in the bilingual brain
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 18
  start-page: 42
  year: 2003
  end-page: 57
  ident: bib23
  article-title: Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 3
  start-page: 201
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib14
  article-title: Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain
  publication-title: Neuroscience
– volume: 92
  start-page: 2899
  year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib39
  article-title: The neural substrates underlying word generation: a bilingual functional-imaging study
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.7.2899
– volume: 25
  start-page: 622
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib52
  article-title: The elusive link between language control and executive control: a case of limited transfer
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.821993
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1496
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib1
  article-title: Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: an event-related fMRI study
  publication-title: Cereb. Cortex
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm182
– volume: 12
  start-page: 2065
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib19
  article-title: General and task-specific frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: a fMRI investigation of task-switching
  publication-title: NeuroReport
  doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00054
– volume: 14
  start-page: 510
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib30
  article-title: Language switching and language representation in Spanish–English bilinguals: an fMRI study
  publication-title: NeuroImage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0810
– volume: 39
  start-page: 713
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib10
  article-title: Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00466-5
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1097
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib505
  article-title: Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033
– volume: 40
  start-page: 414
  year: 1999
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib16
  article-title: Subcortical mechanisms in language: lexical-semantic mechanisms and the thalamus
  publication-title: Brain Cogn.
  doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1088
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib57
  article-title: Direct evidence of the left caudate's role in bilingual control: an intra-operative electrical stimulation study
  publication-title: Neurocase
– volume: 10
  start-page: 347
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib22
  article-title: Language disorders in bilingual patients after thalamic lesions
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
  doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(97)00017-1
– volume: 14
  start-page: 817
  issue: 4
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib9
  article-title: Spatial heterogeneity of the nonlinear dynamics in the FMRI BOLD response
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0873
– volume: 45
  start-page: 721
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib13
  article-title: Lexical activation in bilingual's speech production: language-specific or language-independent?
  publication-title: J. Mem. Lang.
  doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2793
– volume: 27
  start-page: 3010
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib44
  article-title: Neural changes in control implementation of a continuous task
  publication-title: J. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5051-06.2007
– volume: 29
  start-page: 5135
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib36
  article-title: Two mechanisms for task switching in the prefrontal cortex
  publication-title: J. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2828-08.2009
– volume: 10
  start-page: 277
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib17
  article-title: Models of subcortical functions in language: current status
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
  doi: 10.1016/S0911-6044(97)00018-3
– volume: 6
  start-page: 51
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib4
  article-title: Do subcortical structures control ‘Language Selection’ in polygots? Evidence from pathological language mixing
  publication-title: Neurocase
  doi: 10.1093/neucas/6.1.51
– volume: 13
  start-page: 427
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib49
  article-title: An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks
  publication-title: Cogn. Brain Res.
  doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00054-X
– volume: 18
  start-page: 42
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib23
  article-title: Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1319
– volume: 69
  start-page: 257
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib32
  article-title: Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching?
  publication-title: J. Mem. Lang.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.004
– volume: 25
  start-page: 257
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib12
  article-title: PsyScope: an interactive graphical system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers
  publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03204507
– volume: 56
  start-page: 2309
  issue: 2300
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib28
  article-title: Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 35
  start-page: 862
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib58
  article-title: Neural bases of asymmetric language switching in second-language learners: an ER-fMRI study
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.054
– volume: 9
  start-page: 103
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib21
  article-title: Prefrontal cortex in task switching: an event-related fMRI study
  publication-title: Cogn. Brain Res.
  doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00029-4
– volume: 29
  start-page: 162
  year: 1996
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib15
  article-title: AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages
  publication-title: Comput. Biomed. Res.
  doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014
– volume: 50
  start-page: 799
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib20
  article-title: A core system for the implementation of task sets
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031
– volume: 66
  start-page: 232
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib507
  article-title: There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
– volume: 113
  start-page: 135
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib501
  article-title: On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1141
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib508
  article-title: fMRI of intrasubject variability in ADHD: Anomalous premotor activity with prefrontal compensation
  publication-title: J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
  doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181825b1f
– volume: 9
  start-page: 97
  year: 1971
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib47
  article-title: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
– volume: 47
  start-page: 414
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib510
  article-title: Sustained and transient language control in the bilingual brain
  publication-title: NeuroImage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.055
– volume: 73
  start-page: 421
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib31
  article-title: In search of the language switch: an fMRI study of picture naming in Spanish–English bilinguals
  publication-title: Brain Lang.
  doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2278
– volume: 27
  start-page: 959
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib59
  article-title: Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals
  publication-title: Psychol. Aging
  doi: 10.1037/a0028281
– volume: 27
  start-page: 1479
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib43
  article-title: Cognitive control for language switching in bilinguals: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies
  publication-title: Lang. Cogn. Processes
  doi: 10.1080/01690965.2011.613209
– volume: 95
  start-page: 853
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib50
  article-title: The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.853
– volume: 19
  start-page: 290
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib6
  article-title: Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task
  publication-title: Psychol. Aging
  doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
– volume: 97
  start-page: 13448
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib56
  article-title: The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.240460497
– volume: 109
  start-page: 133
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib29
  article-title: Language switching in the bilingual brain: What's next?
  publication-title: Brain Lang.
  doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.12.005
– volume: 35
  start-page: 640
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib26
  article-title: Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn.
– volume: 288
  start-page: 1835
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib45
  article-title: Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
– volume: 25
  start-page: 44
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib34
  article-title: Neural mechanisms of language switch
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.08.007
– volume: 31
  start-page: 1477
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib55
  article-title: On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn.
– volume: 22
  start-page: 2076
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib2
  article-title: Bilingualism tunes theanterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring
  publication-title: Cereb. Cortex
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr287
– volume: 40
  start-page: 25
  year: 1999
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib506
  article-title: Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection
  publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language
  doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2602
– volume: 26
  start-page: 242
  year: 2014
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib48
  article-title: The role of componential analysis, categorical hypothesizing, replicability and confirmation bias in testing for bilingual advantages in executive functioning
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.891597
– volume: 10
  start-page: 89
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib5
  article-title: Bilingual minds
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci. Public Interest
  doi: 10.1177/1529100610387084
– volume: 2008
  start-page: 416
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib42
  article-title: Language selection in bilingual speech: evidence for inhibitory processes
  publication-title: Acta Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.001
– volume: 1147
  start-page: 192
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib500
  article-title: Bilingual language control: An event-related brain potential study
  publication-title: Brain Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.137
– volume: 90
  start-page: 348
  year: 2014
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib18
  article-title: Domain-general inhibition areas of the brain are involved in language switching: FMRI evidence from trilingual speakers
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.049
– volume: 20
  start-page: 1420
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib37
  article-title: Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task-switching
  publication-title: Cereb. Cortex
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp206
– volume: 34
  start-page: 859
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib7
  article-title: Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn. Mem. Cogn.
– volume: 20
  start-page: 242
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib3
  article-title: Bilingual language production: the neurocognition of language representation and control
  publication-title: J. Neuroling.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003
– volume: 17
  start-page: 422
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib54
  article-title: Second language interferes with word production in fluent bilinguals: brain potential and functional imaging evidence
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.1162/0898929053279559
– volume: 13
  start-page: 253
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib53
  article-title: A bilingual advantage in task switching
  publication-title: Bilingualism: Lang. Cogn.
  doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990526
– volume: 106
  start-page: 59
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib503
  article-title: Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013
– volume: 2
  start-page: 399
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib11
  article-title: Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: evidence from task-switching
  publication-title: Front. Psychol.
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00399
– year: 1988
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib509
– volume: 2
  start-page: 234
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib33
  article-title: Executive control of language in the bilingual brain: integrating the evidence from neuroimaging to neuropsychology
  publication-title: Front. Psychol.
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00234
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1987
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib35
  article-title: Bilingual language lateralization: a meta-analytic tale of two hemispheres
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.002
– volume: 312
  start-page: 1537
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib504
  article-title: Language control in the bilingual brain
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.1127761
– volume: 17
  start-page: 682
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037_bib51
  article-title: Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals
  publication-title: J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc.
  doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000580
SSID ssj0004528
Score 2.4283183
Snippet Bilinguals' ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task...
Bilinguals’ ability to control which language they speak and to switch between languages may rely on neurocognitive mechanisms shared with non-linguistic task...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 193
SubjectTerms Adult
Bilingualism
Brain
Brain - physiology
Brain Mapping
Code Switching
College Students
English
Executive control
Executive Function - physiology
Female
fMRI
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Humans
Interference
Linguistic control
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Male
Multilingualism
Spanish
Task switching
Verbal Tasks
Young Adult
Title Language and task switching in the bilingual brain: Bilinguals are staying, not switching, experts
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25446970
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1641018507
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1660436581
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1738471595
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4596720
Volume 66
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1LT-MwEB4hVkJcVuyysOUlI632RKBpbCdG4lCqRd0HnEDiFtmOIwJVWjWpuPHbmcmjtOwKcdhj7HGUeOyZz_b4G4BvLugm2tjE8wOtPB447anQSQ-RciqlsdxU-x2XV3J4w3_ditsVGLR3YSissrH9tU2vrHVTctL05skky-iOL7pGRYl26IJpSHaY85BG-fGTv8gYHrVMzCS9Bt9fYrwqzsjJ3NIQD5HPjynai_Ki_9tR_Q1EX8dTLjioiw342CBL1q8__hOsuPwzrF02Z-ebYP40W5NM5wkrdfHAisesrGIpWZYzRIKMImVJaMQMpY44ZedtQcH01DGEknQt6ojl4_Kl9RGr8gSUxRe4ufhxPRh6TYYFzwqpSo-L1EjZ0zaVNkhk4CLnIm5NJLg0qdLKGRupVPQMOjicqtxxpywd1SjplG-CLVjNx7n7CkwaVGyC6C1JCeRFJkptklh0dxbXdGnQgbO2O2Pb0I9TFoxR3MaZ3cev1RGTOqge1dGBcN5-UhNxvLtlv9VevDS0YvQa737HYav2GOcfHaro3I1nRYzLTSI9Q1j9lowkpn8R-W_IhAEBBaFEB7br4TT_T6KRkyrsYh8sDbS5AHGEL9fk2V3FFc6FkmGvu_Mf-mAX1vFJ1PtPe7BaTmduHxFZaQ6qKXcAH_o_fw-vngH97D2P
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3fT9swED6hIrG9oP1eGds8adoTGW1jOzHSHjo0VEbbJ5B4s2zH0QIorUjQ_v3dJU6hbEI87NU-R4nPvvti330H8NnHg8xYl0XD2KiIx95EKvEyQqScS2kdt815x2wuJ2f857k434DDLheGwiqD7W9temOtQ8t-mM39ZVFQji-6RkWFdijBNEE7vEnsVKIHm-Pjk8n8Lml42pEx04At-HIb5tXQRi5XxoaoiIb8KwV8UWn0f_uqv7Ho_ZDKOz7q6BlsB3DJxu37P4cNX76ArVm4Pn8JdhpOJ5kpM1ab6pJVv4u6CadkRckQDDIKliWhK2apesQB-941VMxce4ZokjKj9li5qG9H77GmVEBdvYKzox-nh5MoFFmInJCqjrjIrZQj43Lp4kzGPvU-5c6mgkubK6O8danKxciij8Pdyj33ytFtjZJeDW38GnrlovRvgUmLus0QwGU54bzUprnLMocez-FvXR734Vs3ndoFBnIqhHGlu1CzC31fHZrUQf2ojj4kq_HLlovj0SPHnfb02urS6Dge_YxPndo1bkG6VzGlX9xUGv84ifcMkfVDMpLI_kU6fEAmiQkrCCX68KZdTqvvJCY5qZIBzsHaQlsJEE34ek9Z_GrowrlQMhkNdv7DHHyEJ5PT2VRPj-cn7-Ap9oj2OGoXevX1jX-PAK22H8IG_AOscEBA
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Language+and+task+switching+in+the+bilingual+brain%3A+Bilinguals+are+staying%2C+not+switching%2C+experts&rft.jtitle=Neuropsychologia&rft.au=Weissberger%2C+Gali+H&rft.au=Gollan%2C+Tamar+H&rft.au=Bondi%2C+Mark+W&rft.au=Clark%2C+Lindsay+R&rft.date=2015-01-01&rft.eissn=1873-3514&rft.volume=66&rft.spage=193&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.neuropsychologia.2014.10.037&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F25446970&rft.externalDocID=25446970
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon