On-line Integration of Semantic Information from Speech and Gesture: Insights from Event-related Brain Potentials

During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often sponta...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inJournal of cognitive neuroscience Vol. 19; no. 4; pp. 605 - 616
Main Authors Özyürek, Aslı, Willems, Roel M., Kita, Sotaro, Hagoort, Peter
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA MIT Press 01.04.2007
MIT Press Journals, The
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often spontaneously co-occur with speech are processed in a similar fashion and integrated to previous sentence context in the same way as lexical meaning. Event-related potentials were measured while subjects listened to spoken sentences with a critical verb (e.g., ), which was accompanied by an iconic co-speech gesture (i.e., KNOCK). Verbal and/or gestural semantic content matched or mismatched the content of the preceding part of the sentence. Despite the difference in the modality and in the specificity of meaning conveyed by spoken words and gestures, the latency, amplitude, and topographical distribution of both word and gesture mismatches are found to be similar, indicating that the brain integrates both types of information simultaneously. This provides evidence for the claim that neural processing in language comprehension involves the simultaneous incorporation of information coming from a broader domain of cognition than only verbal semantics. The neural evidence for similar integration of information from speech and gesture emphasizes the tight interconnection between speech and co-speech gestures.
AbstractList During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often spontaneously co-occur with speech are processed in a similar fashion and integrated to previous sentence context in the same way as lexical meaning. Event-related potentials were measured while subjects listened to spoken sentences with a critical verb (e.g., knock), which was accompanied by an iconic co-speech gesture (i.e., KNOCK). Verbal and/or gestural semantic content matched or mismatched the content of the preceding part of the sentence. Despite the difference in the modality and in the specificity of meaning conveyed by spoken words and gestures, the latency, amplitude, and topographical distribution of both word and gesture mismatches are found to be similar, indicating that the brain integrates both types of information simultaneously. This provides evidence for the claim that neural processing in language comprehension involves the simultaneous incorporation of information coming from a broader domain of cognition than only verbal semantics. The neural evidence for similar integration of information from speech and gesture emphasizes the tight interconnection between speech and co-speech gestures.During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often spontaneously co-occur with speech are processed in a similar fashion and integrated to previous sentence context in the same way as lexical meaning. Event-related potentials were measured while subjects listened to spoken sentences with a critical verb (e.g., knock), which was accompanied by an iconic co-speech gesture (i.e., KNOCK). Verbal and/or gestural semantic content matched or mismatched the content of the preceding part of the sentence. Despite the difference in the modality and in the specificity of meaning conveyed by spoken words and gestures, the latency, amplitude, and topographical distribution of both word and gesture mismatches are found to be similar, indicating that the brain integrates both types of information simultaneously. This provides evidence for the claim that neural processing in language comprehension involves the simultaneous incorporation of information coming from a broader domain of cognition than only verbal semantics. The neural evidence for similar integration of information from speech and gesture emphasizes the tight interconnection between speech and co-speech gestures.
During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often spontaneously co-occur with speech are processed in a similar fashion and integrated to previous sentence context in the same way as lexical meaning. Event-related potentials were measured while subjects listened to spoken sentences with a critical verb (e.g., knock), which was accompanied by an iconic co-speech gesture (i.e., KNOCK). Verbal and/or gestural semantic content matched or mismatched the content of the preceding part of the sentence. Despite the difference in the modality and in the specificity of meaning conveyed by spoken words and gestures, the latency, amplitude, and topographical distribution of both word and gesture mismatches are found to be similar, indicating that the brain integrates both types of information simultaneously. This provides evidence for the claim that neural processing in language comprehension involves the simultaneous incorporation of information coming from a broader domain of cognition than only verbal semantics. The neural evidence for similar integration of information from speech and gesture emphasizes the tight interconnection between speech and co-speech gestures.
During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often spontaneously co-occur with speech are processed in a similar fashion and integrated to previous sentence context in the same way as lexical meaning. Event-related potentials were measured while subjects listened to spoken sentences with a critical verb (e.g., knock), which was accompanied by an iconic co-speech gesture (i.e., KNOCK). Verbal and/or gestural semantic content matched or mismatched the content of the preceding part of the sentence. Despite the difference in the modality and in the specificity of meaning conveyed by spoken words and gestures, the latency, amplitude, and topographical distribution of both word and gesture mismatches are found to be similar, indicating that the brain integrates both types of information simultaneously. This provides evidence for the claim that neural processing in language comprehension involves the simultaneous incorporation of information coming from a broader domain of cognition than only verbal semantics. The neural evidence for similar integration of information from speech and gesture emphasizes the tight interconnection between speech and co-speech gestures. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often spontaneously co-occur with speech are processed in a similar fashion and integrated to previous sentence context in the same way as lexical meaning. Event-related potentials were measured while subjects listened to spoken sentences with a critical verb (e.g., knock), which was accompanied by an iconic co-speech gesture (i.e., KNOCK). Verbal and/or gestural semantic content matched or mismatched the content of the preceding part of the sentence. Despite the difference in the modality and in the specificity of meaning conveyed by spoken words and gestures, the latency, amplitude, and topographical distribution of both word and gesture mismatches are found to be similar, indicating that the brain integrates both types of information simultaneously. This provides evidence for the claim that neural processing in language comprehension involves the simultaneous incorporation of information coming from a broader domain of cognition than only verbal semantics. The neural evidence for similar integration of information from speech and gesture emphasizes the tight interconnection between speech and co-speech gestures. Adapted from the source document
During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the meaning of each upcoming word into the unfolding message-level representation. Here we investigate whether communicative gestures that often spontaneously co-occur with speech are processed in a similar fashion and integrated to previous sentence context in the same way as lexical meaning. Event-related potentials were measured while subjects listened to spoken sentences with a critical verb (e.g., ), which was accompanied by an iconic co-speech gesture (i.e., KNOCK). Verbal and/or gestural semantic content matched or mismatched the content of the preceding part of the sentence. Despite the difference in the modality and in the specificity of meaning conveyed by spoken words and gestures, the latency, amplitude, and topographical distribution of both word and gesture mismatches are found to be similar, indicating that the brain integrates both types of information simultaneously. This provides evidence for the claim that neural processing in language comprehension involves the simultaneous incorporation of information coming from a broader domain of cognition than only verbal semantics. The neural evidence for similar integration of information from speech and gesture emphasizes the tight interconnection between speech and co-speech gestures.
Author Kita, Sotaro
Özyürek, Aslı
Willems, Roel M.
Hagoort, Peter
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Aslı
  surname: Özyürek
  fullname: Özyürek, Aslı
  organization: 2Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Roel M.
  surname: Willems
  fullname: Willems, Roel M.
  organization: 1F. C. Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Nijmegen
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Sotaro
  surname: Kita
  fullname: Kita, Sotaro
  organization: 3University of Birmingham, UK
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Peter
  surname: Hagoort
  fullname: Hagoort, Peter
  organization: 2Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
BackLink http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18708187$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17381252$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFklFrFDEQx4NU7LX6BXyQRdC3XSfJJpv4VktbC4UKVfAtZJNsm2M3e01yBf305ryTSiltHjIw_P6Tmcz_AO2FOTiE3mJoMObk03I2oSEAXYNl0zYc2Au0wIxCLYQUe2gBJdSSyJ_76CClJQAQxttXaB93VGDCyALdXoZ69MFV5yG766izn0M1D9WVm3TI3pT8MMdpmx_iPFVXK-fMTaWDrc5cyuvoPhco-eubnLbEyZ0LuY5u1NnZ6kvUPlTf5lySXo_pNXo5lODe7OIh-nF68v34a31xeXZ-fHRRG8Z5rgc9WGkt1xqcoRZL4bDtJbG2B0k7aqBnzAA3pjeGgWh1xxwD3HPbgqM9PUQft3VXcb5dl07V5JNx46iDm9dJCca7tnzIs2AHFFPM4FkQS054S2QB3z8Al_M6hjKtIoRCJ4HhAr3bQet-clatop90_KX-7aYAH3aATkaPQ9TB-HTPiQ5EuQpHtpyJc0rRDfcIqI1R1MYoamOU0qJqVTFKEYkHIuPz3y3nsq_xaenpVjr5_wbbgHdY-lZRIOUUEcGlhgKhfvvV44WaRwo98fIfbk7o3w
CODEN JCONEO
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2023_2295499
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2008_08_009
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2011_04_004
crossref_primary_10_1080_1357650X_2015_1007057
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pragma_2018_08_016
crossref_primary_10_1111_lang_12123
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2009_21053
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2019_1703016
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_20774
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2015_12_019
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470218_2015_1065282
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2014_06_029
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2020_104772
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2011_07_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2015_04_018
crossref_primary_10_1080_01690965_2011_573220
crossref_primary_10_1177_0142723707087583
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_0809609106
crossref_primary_10_1126_science_aax0289
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2008_12_005
crossref_primary_10_1075_gest_20028_kan
crossref_primary_10_1111_tops_12728
crossref_primary_10_1075_gest_18020_nic
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2024_105413
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10548_014_0392_4
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10919_009_0073_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2020_104191
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2018_04_004
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_017_0942_2
crossref_primary_10_1080_0163853X_2024_2413314
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_physbeh_2018_08_014
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2009_00850_x
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00429_018_1674_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2021_104311
crossref_primary_10_1177_01427237241292808
crossref_primary_10_1177_17456916221141422
crossref_primary_10_1097_WNR_0b013e32833904bb
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2020_146855
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2014_09_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2023_105327
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0084834
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_20576
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_022_01774_9
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2011_01100_x
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_00423
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1355_21_2021
crossref_primary_10_1080_01690965_2010_494846
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2023_108730
crossref_primary_10_1515_cogl_2011_017
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_013_0626_y
crossref_primary_10_1080_14427591_2011_575758
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_01938
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2021_0500
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0140525X15002885
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2020_147059
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2013_0296
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01125
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2019_03_002
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2018_1446543
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2018_01_003
crossref_primary_10_1080_01690960802365567
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0083206
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2009_01_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2019_104725
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2020_1737719
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2007_2159
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797615597671
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2008_20085
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0229486
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00963
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1156087
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2011_03_016
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2017_07_009
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_specom_2013_09_008
crossref_primary_10_3109_02699206_2013_813077
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2012_01_005
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1749_818X_2008_00067_x
crossref_primary_10_1097_AUD_0000000000000302
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10919_015_0215_7
crossref_primary_10_1109_LRA_2024_3359544
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2007_10_055
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2013_0397
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13428
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_22015
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01135_0
crossref_primary_10_1002_brb3_81
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_20753
crossref_primary_10_1177_13621688211044584
crossref_primary_10_1177_1745691610388764
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2012_02_005
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_01775
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2009_07_004
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2021_104979
crossref_primary_10_1590_1678_460x202258944
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2009_06_006
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2018_06_008
crossref_primary_10_1093_deafed_env056
crossref_primary_10_1177_2377616116652402
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2008_04_065
crossref_primary_10_1121_1_4986649
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0305000921000404
crossref_primary_10_1017_langcog_2014_25
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0069173
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2010_21462
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pscychresns_2010_08_008
crossref_primary_10_5334_labphon_75
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2017_02_003
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_017_0911_9
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_634074
crossref_primary_10_1002_aur_1785
crossref_primary_10_1515_cog_2016_0086
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2013_0300
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12868
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_093008_131123
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_00974
crossref_primary_10_1007_s41809_019_00047_z
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2012_02_014
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_22222
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2018_06_018
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2017_1356744
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_575991
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2009_21303
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2010_21486
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_01348
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_01109
crossref_primary_10_1093_scan_nsq050
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2011_04_009
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01301
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2021_634967
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2020_2419
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_25166
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2010_01_009
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2011_03_015
crossref_primary_10_1080_1068316X_2016_1174860
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2024_2346924
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2016_12_004
crossref_primary_10_1111_mbe_12047
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2007_19_7_1175
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2013_09_027
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0051207
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_52158_w
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797609357327
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2009_21254
crossref_primary_10_4074_S0013754516004067
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2012_04_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2010_01_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_08_008
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_021_01145_1
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2022_101100
crossref_primary_10_3917_enf1_164_0419
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2014_959533
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_06_008
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_26797
crossref_primary_10_4000_primatologie_1717
crossref_primary_10_5334_gjgl_334
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_9280_2008_02103_x
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2010_21498
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2009_01_070
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00688
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0142716418000061
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_20565
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2019_0301
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_00080
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10919_011_0122_5
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1748_17_2017
crossref_primary_10_5334_gjgl_580
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2012_0101
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_00033
crossref_primary_10_3233_ICA_180585
crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2017_1283425
crossref_primary_10_1075_lia_15024_fib
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2021_105730
crossref_primary_10_1080_0163853X_2023_2282895
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2021_104916
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2014_03_003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2023_108665
crossref_primary_10_1044_1092_4388_2009_08_0243
crossref_primary_10_1177_0267658315576822
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_22894
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1551_6709_2011_01183_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_learninstruc_2020_101331
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_024_02889_4
crossref_primary_10_1080_02643290801921707
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2021_108061
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2019_03_054
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2009_05_066
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2021_1997800
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2017_02_001
crossref_primary_10_1515_zaa_2020_2016
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_018_1159_8
crossref_primary_10_1093_scan_nsu047
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21073
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2017_12_008
Cites_doi 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00335-3
10.1038/307161a0
10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004
10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.615
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.007
10.1017/S0048577299971196
10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00356.x
10.1162/jocn.1996.8.2.89
10.1162/jocn.1992.4.1.15
10.1075/gest.4.2.02mel
10.1111/1469-8986.00016
10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00024-X
10.1126/science.7777863
10.1006/jmla.2001.2826
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.013
10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3
10.1037/0022-3514.61.5.743
10.1080/00207597508247319
10.1162/089892903322370807
10.2307/1132072
10.1016/0278-2626(90)90029-N
10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00053-8
10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00196-4
10.1111/1467-7687.00056
10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00244-6
10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00786.x
10.1126/science.1095455
10.1038/264746a0
10.1207/s15327973rlsi2703_4
10.1006/brcg.1994.1014
10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00129-X
10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01073-5
10.1162/089892999563724
10.1515/semi.1999.123.1-2.1
10.1126/science.7350657
10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.202
10.1016/0304-3940(91)90914-F
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2007 INIST-CNRS
Copyright MIT Press Journals Apr 2007
Copyright_xml – notice: 2007 INIST-CNRS
– notice: Copyright MIT Press Journals Apr 2007
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QR
7TK
8FD
FR3
K9.
P64
7X8
7T9
DOI 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.605
DatabaseName CrossRef
Pascal-Francis
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Chemoreception Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Engineering Research Database
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Chemoreception Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Technology Research Database
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic
CrossRef
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
Psychology
EISSN 1530-8898
EndPage 616
ExternalDocumentID 1252735711
17381252
18708187
10_1162_jocn_2007_19_4_605
jocn.2007.19.4.605.pdf
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
Feature
GroupedDBID -
0R
36B
4.4
53G
5GY
5RE
6IK
AAJGR
ABDBF
ABDNZ
ABIVO
ABPTK
ABTAH
ACGFO
ACHQT
ACIWK
ACPRK
AEGXH
AEILP
AENEX
AETEA
AFHIN
AHMBA
AIAGR
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AVWKF
AZFZN
BEFXN
BFFAM
BGNUA
BKEBE
BPEOZ
CAG
COF
CS3
DC
DZ
EAP
EAS
EBC
EBD
EBO
EBS
EJD
EMB
EMK
EPL
EPS
ESX
F5P
FAC
FEDTE
FNEHJ
HVGLF
HZ
I-F
IAO
IEA
IGS
IHR
IOF
IPLJI
IPY
JAVBF
MCG
MKJ
MVM
N9A
O9-
OCL
P2P
PK0
PQEST
PQQKQ
RMI
RWL
RXW
S10
SV3
TAE
TH9
TN5
TUS
UHB
UPT
WG8
WH7
X
XSW
YBU
YQT
ZA5
ZWS
ZY4
---
-DZ
-~X
.DC
0R~
AAYXX
ABAZT
ABVLG
ACUHS
CITATION
EMOBN
HZ~
MINIK
P0W
.GJ
29K
9M8
ACYGS
AI.
BKOMP
D-I
FAS
FJW
IQODW
ITC
M43
ROL
VH1
X7L
ZGI
ZXP
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
VXZ
Z5M
7QR
7TK
8FD
FR3
K9.
P64
7X8
7T9
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c566t-fafd9dd6aa0ec3d198e1db92ddb09373c0b55c06ccbcc5084a75e501b6d40e3b3
ISSN 0898-929X
IngestDate Thu Jul 10 20:30:56 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 00:36:45 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 11:27:10 EDT 2025
Sun Jun 29 15:58:20 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 01:41:45 EST 2025
Mon Jul 21 09:16:43 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:04:25 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:47:41 EDT 2025
Sun Jul 17 10:31:15 EDT 2022
Tue Mar 01 17:17:40 EST 2022
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 4
Keywords Human
Verbal comprehension
Information integration
Central nervous system
Electrophysiology
Cognition
Encephalon
Language
Semantics
Gesture
Insight
Speech
Event evoked potential
Language English
License CC BY 4.0
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c566t-fafd9dd6aa0ec3d198e1db92ddb09373c0b55c06ccbcc5084a75e501b6d40e3b3
Notes April, 2007
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink http://hdl.handle.net/11858/00-001M-0000-0013-1AD0-A
PMID 17381252
PQID 223079051
PQPubID 37146
PageCount 12
ParticipantIDs pubmed_primary_17381252
crossref_citationtrail_10_1162_jocn_2007_19_4_605
pascalfrancis_primary_18708187
mit_journals_jocnv19i4_302222_2021_11_08_zip_jocn_2007_19_4_605
proquest_miscellaneous_19626429
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2007_19_4_605
proquest_journals_223079051
mit_journals_10_1162_jocn_2007_19_4_605
proquest_miscellaneous_70313150
proquest_miscellaneous_85674025
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2007-04-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2007-04-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 04
  year: 2007
  text: 2007-04-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2000
PublicationPlace One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA
PublicationPlace_xml – name: One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA
– name: Cambridge, MA
– name: United States
– name: Cambridge
PublicationTitle Journal of cognitive neuroscience
PublicationTitleAlternate J Cogn Neurosci
PublicationYear 2007
Publisher MIT Press
MIT Press Journals, The
Publisher_xml – name: MIT Press
– name: MIT Press Journals, The
References p_27
p_28
p_45
p_24
p_25
p_47
p_26
p_41
p_20
p_42
p_21
p_43
p_22
p_44
p_40
p_16
p_38
p_17
p_39
p_2
p_1
Feyereisen P. (p_8) 1988; 8
p_19
p_34
p_3
p_13
p_35
p_6
p_14
p_36
p_15
p_37
p_7
p_31
p_10
p_32
p_11
p_33
References_xml – ident: p_22
  doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00335-3
– ident: p_27
  doi: 10.1038/307161a0
– ident: p_17
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.004
– ident: p_34
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.615
– ident: p_3
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.007
– ident: p_32
  doi: 10.1017/S0048577299971196
– ident: p_47
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00356.x
– ident: p_11
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.2.89
– ident: p_36
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.1.15
– ident: p_33
  doi: 10.1075/gest.4.2.02mel
– ident: p_41
  doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00016
– ident: p_6
  doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00024-X
– ident: p_42
  doi: 10.1126/science.7777863
– ident: p_38
  doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2826
– ident: p_16
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.013
– ident: p_24
  doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3
– ident: p_25
  doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.5.743
– ident: p_14
  doi: 10.1080/00207597508247319
– ident: p_15
  doi: 10.1162/089892903322370807
– ident: p_21
  doi: 10.2307/1132072
– ident: p_1
  doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(90)90029-N
– ident: p_35
  doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00053-8
– ident: p_44
  doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00196-4
– ident: p_13
  doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00056
– ident: p_10
  doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00244-6
– ident: p_40
  doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00786.x
– ident: p_19
  doi: 10.1126/science.1095455
– ident: p_28
  doi: 10.1038/264746a0
– volume: 8
  start-page: 3
  year: 1988
  ident: p_8
  publication-title: Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive
– ident: p_31
  doi: 10.1207/s15327973rlsi2703_4
– ident: p_20
  doi: 10.1006/brcg.1994.1014
– ident: p_45
  doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00129-X
– ident: p_37
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01073-5
– ident: p_43
  doi: 10.1162/089892999563724
– ident: p_2
  doi: 10.1515/semi.1999.123.1-2.1
– ident: p_26
  doi: 10.1126/science.7350657
– ident: p_7
  doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.202
– ident: p_39
  doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90914-F
SSID ssj0002564
Score 2.3637416
Snippet During language comprehension, listeners use the global semantic representation from previous sentence or discourse context to immediately integrate the...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
crossref
mit
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 605
SubjectTerms Adult
Behavioral psychophysiology
Biological and medical sciences
Brain
Brain research
Cognition & reasoning
Comprehension
Comprehension - physiology
Electrophysiology
Evoked Potentials - physiology
Evoked Responses
Female
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Gestures
Humans
Iconicity
Integration
Language
Language Tests
Male
Neurosciences
Nonverbal Communication
Production and perception of spoken language
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Semantic Processing
Semantics
Speech Perception - physiology
Visual Perception - physiology
Title On-line Integration of Semantic Information from Speech and Gesture: Insights from Event-related Brain Potentials
URI https://direct.mit.edu/jocn/article/doi/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.605
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17381252
https://www.proquest.com/docview/223079051
https://www.proquest.com/docview/19626429
https://www.proquest.com/docview/70313150
https://www.proquest.com/docview/85674025
Volume 19
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3db9MwELfY9rIXBBsfZTD8gOChSkkc26l5QR2sbDDWSeukvUWJ46DCmow2RWr_es6xm6TaOj5eoio9x1bu5_Pd5T4QeuUrUNt8RR1OUuJQRmKnS1nqMOVH3KcsUGWd7a-n_OiCfr5kl7VDv8wuKeKOXNyaV_I_XIV7wFedJfsPnK0eCjfgN_AXrsBhuP4VjweZU2qJx7bmg1X-ztUY3tdItm2ukQkn1Hkk59dKSZPL9gmOg9mkzEw_zqbaRJ8amkMdAemUOS6gjB7oFhLts7zQUUXR1XSNMltHITUKZFaYGajFfKYm6seK81S7epT19NRO2S8jo8-e17LxW27b3Z-t-CiCRmiLFWUCxKppmlvLXdHAF20IUe6yxnnMTS7mTVHPdenY77nMTCFKT3RopxrarKt9Ogj7Fycn4fDwcriBtggYFGQTbfUOPh70q1MbVL-y1NhypcsEK07e3pxjRYnZGI8KHVIbTWFXpaYdynp7pdRbhg_Qfcsj3DPoeYjuqWwH7fayqMjHc_walyHA5beVHbRdHYfzXfTTogs30IXzFC_RhRvowho52KALA7qwRdc7vMSWoVjBFi6xhWtsPUIX_cPhhyPHNuhwJFgBhZNGaSKShEeRq6SfeKKrvCQWJEliF9ReX7oxY9LlUsZSgiVAo4Ap5noxT6ir_Nh_jDazPFNPEQ5A-seKcMFil0oViVhy6fOoGyiSpkK2kLd846G01et1E5WrsLRiOQk1l3RT1SD0REhD4FILtasx16Z2y53Ub4CRod3e0zsp369QaopfnhjR0NfuEwLUxIPBodsNF6PrW5-wvwKXenlwfIIKHbTQ3hI_9TxEJ23oenot9LL6F04D_YkvylQ-g0ULDhYOEespdL8KH6zA9RRdxgMK-6GFnhjg1qsLQL8njDz74_x7aLuWA8_RZjGZqRegvRfxvt14vwFUPPDc
linkProvider EBSCOhost
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=On-line+Integration+of+Semantic+Information+from+Speech+and+Gesture%3A+Insights+from+Event-related+Brain+Potentials&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+cognitive+neuroscience&rft.au=Oezyuerek%2C+A&rft.au=Willems%2C+R+M&rft.au=Kita%2C+S&rft.au=Hagoort%2C+P&rft.date=2007-04-01&rft.issn=0898-929X&rft.volume=19&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=605&rft.epage=616&rft_id=info:doi/10.1162%2Fjocn.2007.19.4.605&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0898-929X&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0898-929X&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0898-929X&client=summon