Direct Instruction Improves Word Learning for Children With Developmental Language Disorder

Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD). Method: Participants included 36 children with DLD a...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inJournal of speech, language, and hearing research Vol. 65; no. 11; pp. 4228 - 4249
Main Authors Pomper, Ron, McGregor, Karla K., Arbisi-Kelm, Timothy, Eden, Nichole, Ohlmann, Nancy
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 01.11.2022
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD). Method: Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions). Results: In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning. Conclusions: These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations.
AbstractList The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD). Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age ( = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions). In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning. These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations.
Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD). Method: Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions). Results: In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning. Conclusions: These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations.
The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD).PURPOSEThe current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD).Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions).METHODParticipants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions).In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning.RESULTSIn alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning.These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations.CONCLUSIONSThese results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations.
Audience Grade 1
Primary Education
Elementary Education
Early Childhood Education
Professional
Academic
Author Eden, Nichole
Arbisi-Kelm, Timothy
Ohlmann, Nancy
Pomper, Ron
McGregor, Karla K.
AuthorAffiliation b Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City
a Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: b Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City
– name: a Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Ron
  orcidid: 0000-0001-5595-4192
  surname: Pomper
  fullname: Pomper, Ron
  organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Karla K.
  orcidid: 0000-0003-0612-0057
  surname: McGregor
  fullname: McGregor, Karla K.
  organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Timothy
  surname: Arbisi-Kelm
  fullname: Arbisi-Kelm, Timothy
  organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Nichole
  surname: Eden
  fullname: Eden, Nichole
  organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Nancy
  surname: Ohlmann
  fullname: Ohlmann, Nancy
  organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE
BackLink http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1368287$$DView record in ERIC
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36342854$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kk1r3DAQhkVJaZJtf0FpMRRKL05lSbblSyHsps0GQ6EN5NCD0Mpjr4osbSR7of--cjdfG5ZKBwnN874aZuYUHVlnAaG3GT7LMGOfCSZEXP2sL3-khKQYU4xfoJMsz3laZZgcxTuuSMoo58foNITfOK6MFa_QMS0oIzxnJ-jXQntQQ7K0YfCjGrSzybLfeLeFkNw43yQ1SG-17ZLW-WS-1qbxYJMbPayTBWzBuE0PdpAmqaXtRtlBstAhCsG_Ri9baQK8uTtn6PrrxfX8Mq2_f1vOz-tU5QUd0rbJGGUsB9zwFauaDK9koaRUnGecQFPifJVXTOW8KhijTcsJoyUuQTatJJLO0Jed7WZc9dComI2XRmy87qX_I5zUYj9i9Vp0biuqimFe5dHg052Bd7cjhEH0OigwRlpwYxCkpJTgHFc0oh92aCcNCG1bFx3VhIvzknBallNyM5QeoDqwEL-PTWx1fN7jzw7wcTfQa3VQ8PGJYA3SDOvgzDi1L-yD75_W5qEo9wMQgXc7ALxWD-GLq4wWnPAyxqtdXHkXgodWKD3I6Z-YojYiw2IaRvE4jCJe_g1j1NJn2nv__6n-AvTB3_A
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1177_23969415241275940
crossref_primary_10_1044_2024_JSLHR_23_00743
crossref_primary_10_1111_1460_6984_12967
crossref_primary_10_1080_17549507_2024_2445155
crossref_primary_10_1044_2024_JSLHR_23_00665
crossref_primary_10_1044_2023_PERSP_23_00075
Cites_doi 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934
10.1016/j.jml.2004.10.003
10.1080/17549507.2020.1750701
10.1037/a0029872
10.3102/0034654310377087
10.1371/journal.pone.0036399
10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100290
10.1542/peds.2013-3355
10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5
10.1177/2396941519899311
10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4
10.1016/j.jecp.2011.05.001
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01845.x
10.1186/s11689-021-09368-z
10.1111/cogs.13094
10.1177/001440290707300202
10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0221
10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.662
10.1017/S0140525X05000026
10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00186
10.1111/0023-8333.00110
10.1080/10888691.2013.748420
10.1177/1525740119827008
10.1017/S0142716403000201
10.1080/15475441.2010.484379
10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005
10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.013
10.1177/23969415211004109
10.1044/1092-4388(2005/030)
10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0233)
10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00397
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.04.003
10.1207/s15327035ex1203_3
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00926.x
10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.006
10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0031
10.1080/15250000701795598
10.1177/0265659011412247
10.1186/s40359-015-0062-7
10.1080/02699200410001669870
10.3109/17549507.2016.1159337
10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00418
10.3102/00346543071003393
10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.498
10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded
10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00062
10.1017/S1355617714000411
10.1037/xge0000020
10.1016/j.jml.2010.02.003
10.1111/1460-6984.12008
10.1002/wcs.1596
10.1044/1092-4388(2004/083)
10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00046
10.1080/13682820601116485
10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00265
10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.011
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00786.x
10.1177/0014402916651848
10.1111/1460-6984.12433
10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136
10.1044/1092-4388(2007/015)
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.01.001
10.1111/mono.12034
10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.005
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.009
10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.001
10.1080/13803390500350977
10.1080/09658210042000085
10.1037/rev0000338
10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0248)
10.1111/1460-6984.12072
10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00033.x
10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.02.001
10.2307/1130255
10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0071
10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0070
10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4
10.1044/jshr.3906.1274
10.1177/001440291107700202
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright COPYRIGHT 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Copyright © 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Copyright_xml – notice: COPYRIGHT 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
– notice: Copyright © 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00300
DatabaseName CrossRef
ERIC
ERIC (Ovid)
ERIC
ERIC
ERIC (Legacy Platform)
ERIC( SilverPlatter )
ERIC
ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)
Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)
ERIC
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
ERIC
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
ERIC

MEDLINE - Academic

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: ERI
  name: ERIC
  url: https://eric.ed.gov/
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Medicine
Languages & Literatures
Social Welfare & Social Work
EISSN 1558-9102
ERIC EJ1368287
EndPage 4249
ExternalDocumentID PMC9940895
A728377824
36342854
EJ1368287
10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300
Genre Randomized Controlled Trial
Journal Article
Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
GeographicLocations United States
GeographicLocations_xml – name: United States
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIDCD NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 DC011742
GroupedDBID ---
--Z
-W8
-~X
.GJ
.GO
0-V
04C
0R~
18M
29L
36B
4.4
5GY
6NX
6PF
7RV
7X7
85S
8A4
8G5
8R4
8R5
AAHSB
AAWTL
AAYXX
ABDBF
ABIVO
ABOPQ
ABPPZ
ABWJO
ABZEH
ACGFO
ACGOD
ACHQT
ACNCT
ACUHS
ACUXI
ADBBV
ADOJX
AENEX
AERSA
AFKRA
AGHSJ
AHMBA
AIKWM
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
ARALO
AZQEC
BENPR
BKEYQ
BMSDO
BPHCQ
BVXVI
CITATION
CJNVE
CPGLG
CRLPW
CS3
DU5
EAD
EAP
EAS
EBD
EBO
EBS
ECE
ECF
ECT
EDJ
EIHBH
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
ESX
EX3
F5P
F9R
FJW
FYUFA
GUQSH
H13
HCIFZ
HZ~
I-F
IAO
ICO
IEA
IER
IHR
IHW
IN-
INH
INIJC
INR
IOF
IPO
IPY
ITC
M0P
M1P
M2M
M2O
M2P
M2Q
M2R
MLAFT
O9-
P2P
PADUT
PCD
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
Q2X
QF4
QM7
QN7
QO5
RWL
S0X
SJA
SV3
TAE
TH9
TN5
TUS
TWZ
UHB
UKHRP
UPT
WH7
WOW
WQ9
YQT
ZCA
186
1HT
3EH
53G
7SW
88E
88I
8AF
8FI
8FJ
AAYRB
ABDPE
ABUWG
AI.
BCR
BJH
BLC
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
CCPQU
DWQXO
EJD
ERI
GNUQQ
HMCUK
H~9
MVM
NAPCQ
OHT
P-O
PEA
PET
PHGZM
PHGZT
PJZUB
PPXIY
PQEDU
PRQQA
QO4
REK
S10
VH1
VJK
WWN
YR5
ZCG
ZHY
ZXP
ZY4
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c563t-fd143445e0d8b49d10ba6caac88182ed705b594c5896443df8243707eadfa2a3
ISSN 1092-4388
1558-9102
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:37:08 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 09:43:42 EDT 2025
Tue Jun 17 21:40:45 EDT 2025
Thu Jun 12 23:45:02 EDT 2025
Tue Jun 10 20:38:25 EDT 2025
Thu May 22 21:09:02 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 06:01:55 EDT 2025
Fri Aug 01 12:19:06 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:11:27 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 02:19:08 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 11
Language English
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c563t-fd143445e0d8b49d10ba6caac88182ed705b594c5896443df8243707eadfa2a3
Notes ObjectType-Article-2
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Undefined-1
ObjectType-Feature-3
content type line 23
Editor: Megan York Roberts
Editor-in-Chief: Stephen M. Camarata
Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing financial or nonfinancial interests existed at the time of publication.
ORCID 0000-0003-0612-0057
0000-0001-5595-4192
OpenAccessLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/9940895
PMID 36342854
PQID 2733205093
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 22
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9940895
proquest_miscellaneous_2733205093
gale_infotracmisc_A728377824
gale_infotracgeneralonefile_A728377824
gale_infotracacademiconefile_A728377824
gale_healthsolutions_A728377824
pubmed_primary_36342854
eric_primary_EJ1368287
crossref_citationtrail_10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300
crossref_primary_10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2022-11-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2022-11-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 11
  year: 2022
  text: 2022-11-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
PublicationTitle Journal of speech, language, and hearing research
PublicationTitleAlternate J Speech Lang Hear Res
PublicationYear 2022
Publisher American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Publisher_xml – name: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
References McGregor K. K. (e_1_3_2_62_1) 2015
e_1_3_2_20_1
e_1_3_2_41_1
e_1_3_2_66_1
e_1_3_2_87_1
e_1_3_2_43_1
e_1_3_2_64_1
e_1_3_2_85_1
e_1_3_2_24_1
e_1_3_2_45_1
e_1_3_2_26_1
e_1_3_2_47_1
e_1_3_2_68_1
Kučera H. (e_1_3_2_49_1) 1967
e_1_3_2_83_1
e_1_3_2_60_1
R Studio Team (e_1_3_2_80_1) 2020
e_1_3_2_16_1
e_1_3_2_39_1
Wechsler D. (e_1_3_2_89_1) 2011
e_1_3_2_18_1
Gillam R. B. (e_1_3_2_28_1) 2004
e_1_3_2_31_1
e_1_3_2_54_1
e_1_3_2_77_1
Cable A. L. (e_1_3_2_7_1) 2011; 46
e_1_3_2_10_1
e_1_3_2_33_1
e_1_3_2_52_1
e_1_3_2_75_1
e_1_3_2_12_1
e_1_3_2_35_1
e_1_3_2_58_1
e_1_3_2_5_1
e_1_3_2_14_1
e_1_3_2_37_1
e_1_3_2_56_1
e_1_3_2_79_1
e_1_3_2_92_1
e_1_3_2_94_1
e_1_3_2_50_1
e_1_3_2_73_1
e_1_3_2_71_1
e_1_3_2_90_1
Gillam R. B. (e_1_3_2_29_1) 2017
e_1_3_2_27_1
e_1_3_2_42_1
e_1_3_2_65_1
e_1_3_2_88_1
e_1_3_2_21_1
e_1_3_2_44_1
e_1_3_2_63_1
e_1_3_2_86_1
e_1_3_2_23_1
e_1_3_2_46_1
e_1_3_2_69_1
e_1_3_2_25_1
e_1_3_2_48_1
e_1_3_2_67_1
Carey S. (e_1_3_2_9_1) 1978; 15
Alloway T. P. (e_1_3_2_2_1) 2007
e_1_3_2_61_1
e_1_3_2_84_1
e_1_3_2_40_1
e_1_3_2_82_1
Beck I. L. (e_1_3_2_3_1) 2013
e_1_3_2_17_1
e_1_3_2_38_1
e_1_3_2_8_1
e_1_3_2_19_1
Ezell H. K. (e_1_3_2_22_1) 2005
e_1_3_2_30_1
e_1_3_2_55_1
e_1_3_2_76_1
e_1_3_2_11_1
e_1_3_2_32_1
e_1_3_2_53_1
e_1_3_2_74_1
e_1_3_2_6_1
e_1_3_2_13_1
e_1_3_2_34_1
e_1_3_2_59_1
e_1_3_2_4_1
e_1_3_2_15_1
e_1_3_2_36_1
e_1_3_2_57_1
e_1_3_2_78_1
e_1_3_2_93_1
e_1_3_2_95_1
e_1_3_2_51_1
e_1_3_2_72_1
e_1_3_2_70_1
e_1_3_2_91_1
Rutter M. (e_1_3_2_81_1) 2003
References_xml – ident: e_1_3_2_15_1
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934
– ident: e_1_3_2_86_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.10.003
– ident: e_1_3_2_74_1
  doi: 10.1080/17549507.2020.1750701
– ident: e_1_3_2_68_1
  doi: 10.1037/a0029872
– ident: e_1_3_2_75_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_92_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_58_1
  doi: 10.3102/0034654310377087
– ident: e_1_3_2_48_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036399
– ident: e_1_3_2_70_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100290
– volume-title: Shared storybook reading: Building young children's language and emergent literacy skills
  year: 2005
  ident: e_1_3_2_22_1
– volume-title: Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction
  year: 2013
  ident: e_1_3_2_3_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_82_1
  doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3355
– ident: e_1_3_2_57_1
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5
– ident: e_1_3_2_60_1
  doi: 10.1177/2396941519899311
– ident: e_1_3_2_91_1
  doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4
– ident: e_1_3_2_94_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.05.001
– ident: e_1_3_2_19_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01845.x
– ident: e_1_3_2_53_1
  doi: 10.1186/s11689-021-09368-z
– ident: e_1_3_2_67_1
  doi: 10.1111/cogs.13094
– ident: e_1_3_2_17_1
  doi: 10.1177/001440290707300202
– ident: e_1_3_2_54_1
  doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0221
– ident: e_1_3_2_38_1
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.662
– ident: e_1_3_2_87_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000026
– ident: e_1_3_2_34_1
  doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00186
– ident: e_1_3_2_16_1
  doi: 10.1111/0023-8333.00110
– volume-title: RStudio: Integrated development for R
  year: 2020
  ident: e_1_3_2_80_1
– volume-title: TNL-2: Test of Narrative Language–Second Edition
  year: 2017
  ident: e_1_3_2_29_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_95_1
  doi: 10.1080/10888691.2013.748420
– ident: e_1_3_2_84_1
  doi: 10.1177/1525740119827008
– volume-title: TNL: Test of Narrative Language–Second Edition
  year: 2004
  ident: e_1_3_2_28_1
– volume-title: Computational analysis of present day American English
  year: 1967
  ident: e_1_3_2_49_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_88_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0142716403000201
– ident: e_1_3_2_8_1
  doi: 10.1080/15475441.2010.484379
– ident: e_1_3_2_14_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005
– ident: e_1_3_2_13_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.013
– volume-title: Automated Working Memory Assessment
  year: 2007
  ident: e_1_3_2_2_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_44_1
  doi: 10.1177/23969415211004109
– ident: e_1_3_2_69_1
  doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/030)
– ident: e_1_3_2_65_1
  doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0233)
– volume-title: The Social Communication Questionnaire: Manual
  year: 2003
  ident: e_1_3_2_81_1
– start-page: 247
  volume-title: School-age language intervention: Evidence-based practices
  year: 2015
  ident: e_1_3_2_62_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_35_1
  doi: 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00397
– ident: e_1_3_2_37_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.04.003
– ident: e_1_3_2_12_1
  doi: 10.1207/s15327035ex1203_3
– ident: e_1_3_2_42_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00926.x
– ident: e_1_3_2_24_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.006
– ident: e_1_3_2_64_1
  doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0031
– ident: e_1_3_2_41_1
  doi: 10.1080/15250000701795598
– ident: e_1_3_2_85_1
  doi: 10.1177/0265659011412247
– ident: e_1_3_2_5_1
  doi: 10.1186/s40359-015-0062-7
– ident: e_1_3_2_76_1
  doi: 10.1080/02699200410001669870
– ident: e_1_3_2_59_1
  doi: 10.3109/17549507.2016.1159337
– ident: e_1_3_2_63_1
  doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00418
– ident: e_1_3_2_72_1
– ident: e_1_3_2_20_1
  doi: 10.3102/00346543071003393
– ident: e_1_3_2_4_1
  doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.498
– volume: 15
  start-page: 17
  year: 1978
  ident: e_1_3_2_9_1
  article-title: Acquiring a single new word
  publication-title: Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference
– ident: e_1_3_2_90_1
  doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded
– ident: e_1_3_2_6_1
  doi: 10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00062
– ident: e_1_3_2_26_1
  doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000411
– ident: e_1_3_2_11_1
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000020
– ident: e_1_3_2_40_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.02.003
– ident: e_1_3_2_66_1
  doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12008
– ident: e_1_3_2_93_1
  doi: 10.1002/wcs.1596
– ident: e_1_3_2_33_1
  doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/083)
– ident: e_1_3_2_31_1
  doi: 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00046
– ident: e_1_3_2_10_1
  doi: 10.1080/13682820601116485
– ident: e_1_3_2_83_1
  doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00265
– ident: e_1_3_2_21_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.011
– ident: e_1_3_2_78_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00786.x
– ident: e_1_3_2_56_1
  doi: 10.1177/0014402916651848
– ident: e_1_3_2_51_1
  doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12433
– ident: e_1_3_2_18_1
  doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136
– ident: e_1_3_2_32_1
  doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/015)
– ident: e_1_3_2_52_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.01.001
– ident: e_1_3_2_27_1
  doi: 10.1111/mono.12034
– ident: e_1_3_2_50_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.005
– ident: e_1_3_2_77_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.009
– ident: e_1_3_2_71_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.001
– ident: e_1_3_2_23_1
  doi: 10.1080/13803390500350977
– ident: e_1_3_2_39_1
  doi: 10.1080/09658210042000085
– ident: e_1_3_2_45_1
  doi: 10.1037/rev0000338
– ident: e_1_3_2_47_1
  doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0248)
– ident: e_1_3_2_46_1
  doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12072
– ident: e_1_3_2_79_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00033.x
– ident: e_1_3_2_43_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.02.001
– volume: 46
  start-page: 138
  issue: 2
  year: 2011
  ident: e_1_3_2_7_1
  article-title: Systematic review of the literature on the treatment of children with late language emergence
  publication-title: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
– ident: e_1_3_2_25_1
  doi: 10.2307/1130255
– ident: e_1_3_2_36_1
  doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0071
– ident: e_1_3_2_55_1
  doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0070
– ident: e_1_3_2_61_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4
– ident: e_1_3_2_30_1
  doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1274
– ident: e_1_3_2_73_1
  doi: 10.1177/001440291107700202
– volume-title: WASI-II: Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence
  year: 2011
  ident: e_1_3_2_89_1
SSID ssj0000146
Score 2.4167614
Snippet Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with...
The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
gale
pubmed
eric
crossref
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
StartPage 4228
SubjectTerms Attention Control
Care and treatment
Child
Children
Cognitive Measurement
Developmental Disabilities
Diagnosis
Direct Instruction
Elementary School Students
Grade 1
Health aspects
Humans
Instructional Effectiveness
Language
Language acquisition
Language Development
Language Development Disorders - psychology
Language Development Disorders - therapy
Language disorders
Language Impairments
Language Tests
Memory
Methods
Phonology
Receptive Language
Retention (Psychology)
Semantics
Study and teaching
Verbal Learning
Vocabulary
Vocabulary Development
Word (Linguistics)
Word Recognition
Title Direct Instruction Improves Word Learning for Children With Developmental Language Disorder
URI http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1368287
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36342854
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2733205093
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9940895
Volume 65
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Nb9NAEF2FVEJcEARaAqEsEiqH4tRer2P7mJZWUdogBEGtxMHy2muIiJKqTi_8Mn4eM96P2CVUlItl2WNn7XmZnR3PvCHkTeBngWC557Co8LAkJ3VEyFPHYyz1wTgWscB658mHwegLH18EF63Wr1rW0vVK9LOfG-tK_kercAz0ilWyd9CsvSkcgH3QL2xBw7D9Jx0re1V98NcssPsqSCBLjILnhj1V5Uoembrtc4y91rKFqli2CltaNs6_OK3lpZSqeZQJdJr0T-yMjb-k2YNslPkjOuY6idsCcZKp2hhdjjZP90_7a-iJWTlzTqWK1mowWc9f20lAMHb2rQctYL3r2aCFsrNujMVaqqNfX2rbG6DtdRvGWTWSMCD0aqYWuctq0zZnivr0jynB5RyrXmAQyfjz2eiTA6NB0-auZ0Dz1f_GxGjTFasP9Zwn65sksFPd5B7ZYrBAYW2yNTx8f3hSoy5TlW3mQQ3jFecHG8bS8Ip04v1NF6HmIzXzd2sO0fQReahBQYcKlo9JSy46ZMcAqaR79MxSdpcdcn-iszg6pKdqwum5nBfplQRJc2B59eMJ-apgTWuwpgbWKJJTA2sKsKYG1hRhTRuwpmY01MD6KZmeHE-PRo7uAeJkwcBfOUUODj3ngXTzSPA491yRDrI0zSLwNJnMQzcQQcyzIIrBs_fzIkKGTTcEA1mkYG62SXuxXMhnhHLJRJRLRcpXxHCRCGNYrYs4B9Bxv0uYUUGSaX58bNMyT25Rf5e8sxddKnqY28W3UbdW9Hjs-QNsOdElr1DbiSp-trYpGYbITgVePO-St5UEYhuGlaW6fAYeDhncGpJ7Dclvir9-k2CvIQgTS9Y4_dpAL8FTmI25kMvrMoElj8-QOApe2o6Con0kf-BzrMrukrABUiuAfPbNM4vZ94rXPo65G8XB87u90xfkwdrC9EgbkClfwkJhJXb1f3K3Cor9BgU0Djo
linkProvider EBSCOhost
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Direct+Instruction+Improves+Word+Learning+for+Children+With+Developmental+Language+Disorder&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+speech%2C+language%2C+and+hearing+research&rft.au=Pomper%2C+Ron&rft.au=McGregor%2C+Karla+K.&rft.au=Arbisi-Kelm%2C+Timothy&rft.au=Eden%2C+Nichole&rft.date=2022-11-01&rft.issn=1092-4388&rft.eissn=1558-9102&rft.volume=65&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=4228&rft.epage=4249&rft_id=info:doi/10.1044%2F2022_JSLHR-22-00300&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1092-4388&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1092-4388&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1092-4388&client=summon