Direct Instruction Improves Word Learning for Children With Developmental Language Disorder
Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD). Method: Participants included 36 children with DLD a...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of speech, language, and hearing research Vol. 65; no. 11; pp. 4228 - 4249 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
01.11.2022
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD). Method: Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions). Results: In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning. Conclusions: These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations. |
---|---|
AbstractList | The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD).
Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (
= 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions).
In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning.
These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations. Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD). Method: Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions). Results: In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning. Conclusions: These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations. The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD).PURPOSEThe current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and children with typical language development (TLD).Participants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions).METHODParticipants included 36 children with DLD and 45 children with TLD. All children were in the first grade and 6-8 years of age (Mdn = 7 years; 2 months). Using a between-subjects design, children were randomly assigned to be exposed to novel words and their unfamiliar referents via either direct instruction (each referent presented in isolation with an explicit goal of learning) or indirect exposure (multiple referents presented with the goal of answering yes/no questions).In alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning.RESULTSIn alternative forced-choice measures of recognition, children with DLD were less accurate than their TLD peers in linking words to referents, encoding semantic categories for words, and encoding detailed representations of word forms. These differences in word learning were accounted for by a constellation of cognitive measures, including receptive vocabulary, phonological memory, visuospatial memory, and sustained attention. All children were similarly accurate in retaining word forms over a 24- to 48-hr delay. Children with TLD were more accurate in all aspects of word learning following direct instruction compared to indirect exposure. Benefits from direct instruction were observed for children with DLD in link and semantic, but not word form, learning.These results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations.CONCLUSIONSThese results suggest that vocabulary interventions with direct instruction can help children with DLD learn some, but not all, aspects of novel words. Additional support is necessary to help children with DLD encode rich phonological representations. |
Audience | Grade 1 Primary Education Elementary Education Early Childhood Education Professional Academic |
Author | Eden, Nichole Arbisi-Kelm, Timothy Ohlmann, Nancy Pomper, Ron McGregor, Karla K. |
AuthorAffiliation | b Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City a Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: b Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City – name: a Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Ron orcidid: 0000-0001-5595-4192 surname: Pomper fullname: Pomper, Ron organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE – sequence: 2 givenname: Karla K. orcidid: 0000-0003-0612-0057 surname: McGregor fullname: McGregor, Karla K. organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City – sequence: 3 givenname: Timothy surname: Arbisi-Kelm fullname: Arbisi-Kelm, Timothy organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE – sequence: 4 givenname: Nichole surname: Eden fullname: Eden, Nichole organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE – sequence: 5 givenname: Nancy surname: Ohlmann fullname: Ohlmann, Nancy organization: Center for Childhood Deafness, Language and Learning, Boys Town National Research Hospital, NE |
BackLink | http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1368287$$DView record in ERIC https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36342854$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kk1r3DAQhkVJaZJtf0FpMRRKL05lSbblSyHsps0GQ6EN5NCD0Mpjr4osbSR7of--cjdfG5ZKBwnN874aZuYUHVlnAaG3GT7LMGOfCSZEXP2sL3-khKQYU4xfoJMsz3laZZgcxTuuSMoo58foNITfOK6MFa_QMS0oIzxnJ-jXQntQQ7K0YfCjGrSzybLfeLeFkNw43yQ1SG-17ZLW-WS-1qbxYJMbPayTBWzBuE0PdpAmqaXtRtlBstAhCsG_Ri9baQK8uTtn6PrrxfX8Mq2_f1vOz-tU5QUd0rbJGGUsB9zwFauaDK9koaRUnGecQFPifJVXTOW8KhijTcsJoyUuQTatJJLO0Jed7WZc9dComI2XRmy87qX_I5zUYj9i9Vp0biuqimFe5dHg052Bd7cjhEH0OigwRlpwYxCkpJTgHFc0oh92aCcNCG1bFx3VhIvzknBallNyM5QeoDqwEL-PTWx1fN7jzw7wcTfQa3VQ8PGJYA3SDOvgzDi1L-yD75_W5qEo9wMQgXc7ALxWD-GLq4wWnPAyxqtdXHkXgodWKD3I6Z-YojYiw2IaRvE4jCJe_g1j1NJn2nv__6n-AvTB3_A |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1177_23969415241275940 crossref_primary_10_1044_2024_JSLHR_23_00743 crossref_primary_10_1111_1460_6984_12967 crossref_primary_10_1080_17549507_2024_2445155 crossref_primary_10_1044_2024_JSLHR_23_00665 crossref_primary_10_1044_2023_PERSP_23_00075 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934 10.1016/j.jml.2004.10.003 10.1080/17549507.2020.1750701 10.1037/a0029872 10.3102/0034654310377087 10.1371/journal.pone.0036399 10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100290 10.1542/peds.2013-3355 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5 10.1177/2396941519899311 10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.05.001 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01845.x 10.1186/s11689-021-09368-z 10.1111/cogs.13094 10.1177/001440290707300202 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0221 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.662 10.1017/S0140525X05000026 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00186 10.1111/0023-8333.00110 10.1080/10888691.2013.748420 10.1177/1525740119827008 10.1017/S0142716403000201 10.1080/15475441.2010.484379 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.013 10.1177/23969415211004109 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/030) 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0233) 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00397 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.04.003 10.1207/s15327035ex1203_3 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00926.x 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.006 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0031 10.1080/15250000701795598 10.1177/0265659011412247 10.1186/s40359-015-0062-7 10.1080/02699200410001669870 10.3109/17549507.2016.1159337 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00418 10.3102/00346543071003393 10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.498 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded 10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00062 10.1017/S1355617714000411 10.1037/xge0000020 10.1016/j.jml.2010.02.003 10.1111/1460-6984.12008 10.1002/wcs.1596 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/083) 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00046 10.1080/13682820601116485 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00265 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.011 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00786.x 10.1177/0014402916651848 10.1111/1460-6984.12433 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/015) 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.01.001 10.1111/mono.12034 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.005 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.009 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.001 10.1080/13803390500350977 10.1080/09658210042000085 10.1037/rev0000338 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0248) 10.1111/1460-6984.12072 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00033.x 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.02.001 10.2307/1130255 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0071 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0070 10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4 10.1044/jshr.3906.1274 10.1177/001440291107700202 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | COPYRIGHT 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Copyright © 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association |
Copyright_xml | – notice: COPYRIGHT 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association – notice: Copyright © 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00300 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ERIC ERIC (Ovid) ERIC ERIC ERIC (Legacy Platform) ERIC( SilverPlatter ) ERIC ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform) Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) ERIC Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef ERIC MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE ERIC MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: ERI name: ERIC url: https://eric.ed.gov/ sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Languages & Literatures Social Welfare & Social Work |
EISSN | 1558-9102 |
ERIC | EJ1368287 |
EndPage | 4249 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC9940895 A728377824 36342854 EJ1368287 10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300 |
Genre | Randomized Controlled Trial Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GeographicLocations | United States |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: United States |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIDCD NIH HHS grantid: R01 DC011742 |
GroupedDBID | --- --Z -W8 -~X .GJ .GO 0-V 04C 0R~ 18M 29L 36B 4.4 5GY 6NX 6PF 7RV 7X7 85S 8A4 8G5 8R4 8R5 AAHSB AAWTL AAYXX ABDBF ABIVO ABOPQ ABPPZ ABWJO ABZEH ACGFO ACGOD ACHQT ACNCT ACUHS ACUXI ADBBV ADOJX AENEX AERSA AFKRA AGHSJ AHMBA AIKWM ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ARALO AZQEC BENPR BKEYQ BMSDO BPHCQ BVXVI CITATION CJNVE CPGLG CRLPW CS3 DU5 EAD EAP EAS EBD EBO EBS ECE ECF ECT EDJ EIHBH EMB EMK EMOBN ESX EX3 F5P F9R FJW FYUFA GUQSH H13 HCIFZ HZ~ I-F IAO ICO IEA IER IHR IHW IN- INH INIJC INR IOF IPO IPY ITC M0P M1P M2M M2O M2P M2Q M2R MLAFT O9- P2P PADUT PCD PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ Q2X QF4 QM7 QN7 QO5 RWL S0X SJA SV3 TAE TH9 TN5 TUS TWZ UHB UKHRP UPT WH7 WOW WQ9 YQT ZCA 186 1HT 3EH 53G 7SW 88E 88I 8AF 8FI 8FJ AAYRB ABDPE ABUWG AI. BCR BJH BLC BNH BNI BNJ BNO CCPQU DWQXO EJD ERI GNUQQ HMCUK H~9 MVM NAPCQ OHT P-O PEA PET PHGZM PHGZT PJZUB PPXIY PQEDU PRQQA QO4 REK S10 VH1 VJK WWN YR5 ZCG ZHY ZXP ZY4 CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c563t-fd143445e0d8b49d10ba6caac88182ed705b594c5896443df8243707eadfa2a3 |
ISSN | 1092-4388 1558-9102 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:37:08 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 09:43:42 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 17 21:40:45 EDT 2025 Thu Jun 12 23:45:02 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 10 20:38:25 EDT 2025 Thu May 22 21:09:02 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:01:55 EDT 2025 Fri Aug 01 12:19:06 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:11:27 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:19:08 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 11 |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c563t-fd143445e0d8b49d10ba6caac88182ed705b594c5896443df8243707eadfa2a3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Undefined-1 ObjectType-Feature-3 content type line 23 Editor: Megan York Roberts Editor-in-Chief: Stephen M. Camarata Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing financial or nonfinancial interests existed at the time of publication. |
ORCID | 0000-0003-0612-0057 0000-0001-5595-4192 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/9940895 |
PMID | 36342854 |
PQID | 2733205093 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 22 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9940895 proquest_miscellaneous_2733205093 gale_infotracmisc_A728377824 gale_infotracgeneralonefile_A728377824 gale_infotracacademiconefile_A728377824 gale_healthsolutions_A728377824 pubmed_primary_36342854 eric_primary_EJ1368287 crossref_citationtrail_10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300 crossref_primary_10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2022-11-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2022-11-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 11 year: 2022 text: 2022-11-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Journal of speech, language, and hearing research |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Speech Lang Hear Res |
PublicationYear | 2022 |
Publisher | American Speech-Language-Hearing Association |
Publisher_xml | – name: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association |
References | McGregor K. K. (e_1_3_2_62_1) 2015 e_1_3_2_20_1 e_1_3_2_41_1 e_1_3_2_66_1 e_1_3_2_87_1 e_1_3_2_43_1 e_1_3_2_64_1 e_1_3_2_85_1 e_1_3_2_24_1 e_1_3_2_45_1 e_1_3_2_26_1 e_1_3_2_47_1 e_1_3_2_68_1 Kučera H. (e_1_3_2_49_1) 1967 e_1_3_2_83_1 e_1_3_2_60_1 R Studio Team (e_1_3_2_80_1) 2020 e_1_3_2_16_1 e_1_3_2_39_1 Wechsler D. (e_1_3_2_89_1) 2011 e_1_3_2_18_1 Gillam R. B. (e_1_3_2_28_1) 2004 e_1_3_2_31_1 e_1_3_2_54_1 e_1_3_2_77_1 Cable A. L. (e_1_3_2_7_1) 2011; 46 e_1_3_2_10_1 e_1_3_2_33_1 e_1_3_2_52_1 e_1_3_2_75_1 e_1_3_2_12_1 e_1_3_2_35_1 e_1_3_2_58_1 e_1_3_2_5_1 e_1_3_2_14_1 e_1_3_2_37_1 e_1_3_2_56_1 e_1_3_2_79_1 e_1_3_2_92_1 e_1_3_2_94_1 e_1_3_2_50_1 e_1_3_2_73_1 e_1_3_2_71_1 e_1_3_2_90_1 Gillam R. B. (e_1_3_2_29_1) 2017 e_1_3_2_27_1 e_1_3_2_42_1 e_1_3_2_65_1 e_1_3_2_88_1 e_1_3_2_21_1 e_1_3_2_44_1 e_1_3_2_63_1 e_1_3_2_86_1 e_1_3_2_23_1 e_1_3_2_46_1 e_1_3_2_69_1 e_1_3_2_25_1 e_1_3_2_48_1 e_1_3_2_67_1 Carey S. (e_1_3_2_9_1) 1978; 15 Alloway T. P. (e_1_3_2_2_1) 2007 e_1_3_2_61_1 e_1_3_2_84_1 e_1_3_2_40_1 e_1_3_2_82_1 Beck I. L. (e_1_3_2_3_1) 2013 e_1_3_2_17_1 e_1_3_2_38_1 e_1_3_2_8_1 e_1_3_2_19_1 Ezell H. K. (e_1_3_2_22_1) 2005 e_1_3_2_30_1 e_1_3_2_55_1 e_1_3_2_76_1 e_1_3_2_11_1 e_1_3_2_32_1 e_1_3_2_53_1 e_1_3_2_74_1 e_1_3_2_6_1 e_1_3_2_13_1 e_1_3_2_34_1 e_1_3_2_59_1 e_1_3_2_4_1 e_1_3_2_15_1 e_1_3_2_36_1 e_1_3_2_57_1 e_1_3_2_78_1 e_1_3_2_93_1 e_1_3_2_95_1 e_1_3_2_51_1 e_1_3_2_72_1 e_1_3_2_70_1 e_1_3_2_91_1 Rutter M. (e_1_3_2_81_1) 2003 |
References_xml | – ident: e_1_3_2_15_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934 – ident: e_1_3_2_86_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.10.003 – ident: e_1_3_2_74_1 doi: 10.1080/17549507.2020.1750701 – ident: e_1_3_2_68_1 doi: 10.1037/a0029872 – ident: e_1_3_2_75_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_92_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_58_1 doi: 10.3102/0034654310377087 – ident: e_1_3_2_48_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036399 – ident: e_1_3_2_70_1 doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100290 – volume-title: Shared storybook reading: Building young children's language and emergent literacy skills year: 2005 ident: e_1_3_2_22_1 – volume-title: Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction year: 2013 ident: e_1_3_2_3_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_82_1 doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3355 – ident: e_1_3_2_57_1 doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5 – ident: e_1_3_2_60_1 doi: 10.1177/2396941519899311 – ident: e_1_3_2_91_1 doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4 – ident: e_1_3_2_94_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.05.001 – ident: e_1_3_2_19_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01845.x – ident: e_1_3_2_53_1 doi: 10.1186/s11689-021-09368-z – ident: e_1_3_2_67_1 doi: 10.1111/cogs.13094 – ident: e_1_3_2_17_1 doi: 10.1177/001440290707300202 – ident: e_1_3_2_54_1 doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0221 – ident: e_1_3_2_38_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.662 – ident: e_1_3_2_87_1 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000026 – ident: e_1_3_2_34_1 doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00186 – ident: e_1_3_2_16_1 doi: 10.1111/0023-8333.00110 – volume-title: RStudio: Integrated development for R year: 2020 ident: e_1_3_2_80_1 – volume-title: TNL-2: Test of Narrative Language–Second Edition year: 2017 ident: e_1_3_2_29_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_95_1 doi: 10.1080/10888691.2013.748420 – ident: e_1_3_2_84_1 doi: 10.1177/1525740119827008 – volume-title: TNL: Test of Narrative Language–Second Edition year: 2004 ident: e_1_3_2_28_1 – volume-title: Computational analysis of present day American English year: 1967 ident: e_1_3_2_49_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_88_1 doi: 10.1017/S0142716403000201 – ident: e_1_3_2_8_1 doi: 10.1080/15475441.2010.484379 – ident: e_1_3_2_14_1 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 – ident: e_1_3_2_13_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.013 – volume-title: Automated Working Memory Assessment year: 2007 ident: e_1_3_2_2_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_44_1 doi: 10.1177/23969415211004109 – ident: e_1_3_2_69_1 doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/030) – ident: e_1_3_2_65_1 doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0233) – volume-title: The Social Communication Questionnaire: Manual year: 2003 ident: e_1_3_2_81_1 – start-page: 247 volume-title: School-age language intervention: Evidence-based practices year: 2015 ident: e_1_3_2_62_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_35_1 doi: 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00397 – ident: e_1_3_2_37_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.04.003 – ident: e_1_3_2_12_1 doi: 10.1207/s15327035ex1203_3 – ident: e_1_3_2_42_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00926.x – ident: e_1_3_2_24_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.006 – ident: e_1_3_2_64_1 doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0031 – ident: e_1_3_2_41_1 doi: 10.1080/15250000701795598 – ident: e_1_3_2_85_1 doi: 10.1177/0265659011412247 – ident: e_1_3_2_5_1 doi: 10.1186/s40359-015-0062-7 – ident: e_1_3_2_76_1 doi: 10.1080/02699200410001669870 – ident: e_1_3_2_59_1 doi: 10.3109/17549507.2016.1159337 – ident: e_1_3_2_63_1 doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00418 – ident: e_1_3_2_72_1 – ident: e_1_3_2_20_1 doi: 10.3102/00346543071003393 – ident: e_1_3_2_4_1 doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.498 – volume: 15 start-page: 17 year: 1978 ident: e_1_3_2_9_1 article-title: Acquiring a single new word publication-title: Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference – ident: e_1_3_2_90_1 doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded – ident: e_1_3_2_6_1 doi: 10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00062 – ident: e_1_3_2_26_1 doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000411 – ident: e_1_3_2_11_1 doi: 10.1037/xge0000020 – ident: e_1_3_2_40_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.02.003 – ident: e_1_3_2_66_1 doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12008 – ident: e_1_3_2_93_1 doi: 10.1002/wcs.1596 – ident: e_1_3_2_33_1 doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/083) – ident: e_1_3_2_31_1 doi: 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00046 – ident: e_1_3_2_10_1 doi: 10.1080/13682820601116485 – ident: e_1_3_2_83_1 doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00265 – ident: e_1_3_2_21_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.011 – ident: e_1_3_2_78_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00786.x – ident: e_1_3_2_56_1 doi: 10.1177/0014402916651848 – ident: e_1_3_2_51_1 doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12433 – ident: e_1_3_2_18_1 doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136 – ident: e_1_3_2_32_1 doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/015) – ident: e_1_3_2_52_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.01.001 – ident: e_1_3_2_27_1 doi: 10.1111/mono.12034 – ident: e_1_3_2_50_1 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.005 – ident: e_1_3_2_77_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.009 – ident: e_1_3_2_71_1 doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.001 – ident: e_1_3_2_23_1 doi: 10.1080/13803390500350977 – ident: e_1_3_2_39_1 doi: 10.1080/09658210042000085 – ident: e_1_3_2_45_1 doi: 10.1037/rev0000338 – ident: e_1_3_2_47_1 doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0248) – ident: e_1_3_2_46_1 doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12072 – ident: e_1_3_2_79_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00033.x – ident: e_1_3_2_43_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.02.001 – volume: 46 start-page: 138 issue: 2 year: 2011 ident: e_1_3_2_7_1 article-title: Systematic review of the literature on the treatment of children with late language emergence publication-title: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders – ident: e_1_3_2_25_1 doi: 10.2307/1130255 – ident: e_1_3_2_36_1 doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0071 – ident: e_1_3_2_55_1 doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0070 – ident: e_1_3_2_61_1 doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4 – ident: e_1_3_2_30_1 doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1274 – ident: e_1_3_2_73_1 doi: 10.1177/001440291107700202 – volume-title: WASI-II: Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence year: 2011 ident: e_1_3_2_89_1 |
SSID | ssj0000146 |
Score | 2.4167614 |
Snippet | Purpose: The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with... The current study compared the effects of direct instruction versus indirect exposure on multiple aspects of novel word learning for children with... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest gale pubmed eric crossref |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 4228 |
SubjectTerms | Attention Control Care and treatment Child Children Cognitive Measurement Developmental Disabilities Diagnosis Direct Instruction Elementary School Students Grade 1 Health aspects Humans Instructional Effectiveness Language Language acquisition Language Development Language Development Disorders - psychology Language Development Disorders - therapy Language disorders Language Impairments Language Tests Memory Methods Phonology Receptive Language Retention (Psychology) Semantics Study and teaching Verbal Learning Vocabulary Vocabulary Development Word (Linguistics) Word Recognition |
Title | Direct Instruction Improves Word Learning for Children With Developmental Language Disorder |
URI | http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1368287 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36342854 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2733205093 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9940895 |
Volume | 65 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Nb9NAEF2FVEJcEARaAqEsEiqH4tRer2P7mJZWUdogBEGtxMHy2muIiJKqTi_8Mn4eM96P2CVUlItl2WNn7XmZnR3PvCHkTeBngWC557Co8LAkJ3VEyFPHYyz1wTgWscB658mHwegLH18EF63Wr1rW0vVK9LOfG-tK_kercAz0ilWyd9CsvSkcgH3QL2xBw7D9Jx0re1V98NcssPsqSCBLjILnhj1V5Uoembrtc4y91rKFqli2CltaNs6_OK3lpZSqeZQJdJr0T-yMjb-k2YNslPkjOuY6idsCcZKp2hhdjjZP90_7a-iJWTlzTqWK1mowWc9f20lAMHb2rQctYL3r2aCFsrNujMVaqqNfX2rbG6DtdRvGWTWSMCD0aqYWuctq0zZnivr0jynB5RyrXmAQyfjz2eiTA6NB0-auZ0Dz1f_GxGjTFasP9Zwn65sksFPd5B7ZYrBAYW2yNTx8f3hSoy5TlW3mQQ3jFecHG8bS8Ip04v1NF6HmIzXzd2sO0fQReahBQYcKlo9JSy46ZMcAqaR79MxSdpcdcn-iszg6pKdqwum5nBfplQRJc2B59eMJ-apgTWuwpgbWKJJTA2sKsKYG1hRhTRuwpmY01MD6KZmeHE-PRo7uAeJkwcBfOUUODj3ngXTzSPA491yRDrI0zSLwNJnMQzcQQcyzIIrBs_fzIkKGTTcEA1mkYG62SXuxXMhnhHLJRJRLRcpXxHCRCGNYrYs4B9Bxv0uYUUGSaX58bNMyT25Rf5e8sxddKnqY28W3UbdW9Hjs-QNsOdElr1DbiSp-trYpGYbITgVePO-St5UEYhuGlaW6fAYeDhncGpJ7Dclvir9-k2CvIQgTS9Y4_dpAL8FTmI25kMvrMoElj8-QOApe2o6Con0kf-BzrMrukrABUiuAfPbNM4vZ94rXPo65G8XB87u90xfkwdrC9EgbkClfwkJhJXb1f3K3Cor9BgU0Djo |
linkProvider | EBSCOhost |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Direct+Instruction+Improves+Word+Learning+for+Children+With+Developmental+Language+Disorder&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+speech%2C+language%2C+and+hearing+research&rft.au=Pomper%2C+Ron&rft.au=McGregor%2C+Karla+K.&rft.au=Arbisi-Kelm%2C+Timothy&rft.au=Eden%2C+Nichole&rft.date=2022-11-01&rft.issn=1092-4388&rft.eissn=1558-9102&rft.volume=65&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=4228&rft.epage=4249&rft_id=info:doi/10.1044%2F2022_JSLHR-22-00300&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_1044_2022_JSLHR_22_00300 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1092-4388&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1092-4388&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1092-4388&client=summon |