How Many Participants? How Many Trials? Maximizing the Power of Reaction Time Studies

Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experim...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBehavior research methods Vol. 56; no. 3; pp. 2398 - 2421
Main Author Miller, Jeff
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published New York Springer US 01.03.2024
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1554-3528
1554-351X
1554-3528
DOI10.3758/s13428-023-02155-9

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first N T trials from N P randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, N P × N T . The [ N P , N T ] combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.
AbstractList Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first NT trials from NP randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, NP×NT. The [NP,NT] combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.
Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first trials from randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, . The combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.
Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first $$N_T$$ N T trials from $$N_P$$ N P randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, $$N_P \! \times \! N_T$$ N P × N T . The $$[N_P,N_T]$$ [ N P , N T ] combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.
Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first N T trials from N P randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, N P × N T . The [ N P , N T ] combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.
Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first N T trials from N P randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, N P × N T . The [ N P , N T ] combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first N T trials from N P randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, N P × N T . The [ N P , N T ] combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.
Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few participants with relatively many trials each or testing relatively many participants with relatively few trials each. To compare the experimental power that would be obtained under each of these options, I simulated virtual experiments using subsets of participants and trials from eight large real RT datasets examining 19 experimental effects. The simulations compared designs using the first \documentclass[12pt]{minimal} \usepackage{amsmath} \usepackage{wasysym} \usepackage{amsfonts} \usepackage{amssymb} \usepackage{amsbsy} \usepackage{mathrsfs} \usepackage{upgreek} \setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt} \begin{document}$$N_T$$\end{document} N T trials from \documentclass[12pt]{minimal} \usepackage{amsmath} \usepackage{wasysym} \usepackage{amsfonts} \usepackage{amssymb} \usepackage{amsbsy} \usepackage{mathrsfs} \usepackage{upgreek} \setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt} \begin{document}$$N_P$$\end{document} N P randomly selected participants, holding constant the total number of trials across all participants, \documentclass[12pt]{minimal} \usepackage{amsmath} \usepackage{wasysym} \usepackage{amsfonts} \usepackage{amssymb} \usepackage{amsbsy} \usepackage{mathrsfs} \usepackage{upgreek} \setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt} \begin{document}$$N_P \! \times \! N_T$$\end{document} N P × N T . The \documentclass[12pt]{minimal} \usepackage{amsmath} \usepackage{wasysym} \usepackage{amsfonts} \usepackage{amssymb} \usepackage{amsbsy} \usepackage{mathrsfs} \usepackage{upgreek} \setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt} \begin{document}$$[N_P,N_T]$$\end{document} [ N P , N T ] combination maximizing the power to detect each effect depended on how the mean and variability of that effect changed with practice. For most effects, power was greater in designs having many participants with few trials each rather than the reverse, suggesting that researchers should usually try to recruit large numbers of participants for short experimental sessions. In some cases, power for a fixed total number of trials across all participants was maximized by having as few as two trials per participant in each condition. Where researchers can make plausible predictions about how their effects will change over the course of a session, they can use those predictions to increase their experimental power.
Author Miller, Jeff
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Jeff
  surname: Miller
  fullname: Miller, Jeff
  email: miller@psy.otago.ac.nz
  organization: Department of Psychology, University of Otago
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37537492$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kUtP3TAQhS1EVR7tH2CBLLHpJuDnjbNCCLWlEqiIXtaW40wuRol9sZ1S-utrenkvWFhjjc93dDyzhdZ98IDQDiX7vJbqIFEumKoI4-VQKatmDW2WKioumVp_cd9AWyldE8IVo-Ij2ig4r0XDNtHlSbjFZ8bf4XMTs7NuaXxOh_ipPY_ODKVxZv640f11foHzFeDzcAsRhx5fgLHZBY_nbgT8K0-dg_QJfegLBZ8f6jaaf_s6Pz6pTn9-_3F8dFpZKViugHEgFgztQZqWS9FIymTdUtUDIVYIYpRQXW1Yp2as7UjHlTQg2nIxkvBtdLiyXU7tCJ0Fn6MZ9DK60cQ7HYzTr1-8u9KL8FtT0jSUzFhx-PLgEMPNBCnr0SULw2A8hClppsSsYYLwWZHuvZFehyn68j3NCac150TKotp9Gekpy-PEi0CtBDaGlCL02rps7idYErqhRLvXKr1ari7L1f-Xq5uCsjfoo_u7EF9BqYj9AuJz7Heof75XtkA
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1038_s43588_024_00703_7
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_0575_24_2024
crossref_primary_10_56038_ejrnd_v4i3_475
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2024_1359007
crossref_primary_10_3390_diagnostics14171918
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_025_02076_6
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2024_1391271
Cites_doi 10.5334/joc.85
10.3758/s13428-021-01734-y
10.3758/s13428-016-0720-6
10.3758/s13421-014-0446-6
10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.3
10.3758/s13428-021-01573-x
10.1177/0018720818787126
10.1037/met0000337
10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
10.1037/xge0000014
10.3758/s13428-016-0783-4
10.3758/s13428-013-0442-y
10.1177/2515245917745058
10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3
10.1037/xge0001188
10.3758/s13428-015-0678-9
10.3758/s13428-012-0304-z
10.3758/s13423-018-1451-8
10.1136/jnnp.53.8.702
10.1037/0096-1523.21.5.1015
10.5334/joc.10
10.1186/s41235-021-00352-8
10.1002/9781119095910.ch2
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright The Author(s) 2023
2023. The Author(s).
The Author(s) 2023. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Copyright_xml – notice: The Author(s) 2023
– notice: 2023. The Author(s).
– notice: The Author(s) 2023. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
DBID C6C
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
4T-
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.3758/s13428-023-02155-9
DatabaseName SpringerOpen Free (Free internet resource, activated by CARLI)
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Docstoc
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Neurosciences Abstracts
Docstoc
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE
CrossRef

MEDLINE - Academic

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: C6C
  name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals
  url: http://www.springeropen.com/
  sourceTypes: Publisher
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1554-3528
EndPage 2421
ExternalDocumentID PMC10991062
37537492
10_3758_s13428_023_02155_9
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: University of Otago
GroupedDBID ---
-55
-5G
-BR
-DZ
-EM
-ET
-~C
-~X
0-V
06D
0R~
0VY
199
1N0
203
23N
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2VQ
30V
3V.
4.4
406
408
40E
53G
5GY
7X7
875
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8G5
8TC
8UJ
95.
96X
AAAVM
AABHQ
AACDK
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AAKPC
AANZL
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
AAZMS
ABAKF
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABJUD
ABKCH
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABPPZ
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABUWG
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACBXY
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIWK
ACKIV
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACPIV
ACPRK
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETCA
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFFNX
AFKRA
AFQWF
AFRAH
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHMBA
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARALO
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
AZQEC
B-.
BAWUL
BENPR
BGNMA
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C1A
C6C
CAG
CCPQU
COF
CSCUP
DDRTE
DIK
DNIVK
DPUIP
DWQXO
E3Z
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMOBN
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FYUFA
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ3
GQ6
GQ7
GUQSH
H13
HF~
HMCUK
HMJXF
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
IAO
IHR
IKXTQ
INH
IPY
IRVIT
ITC
ITM
IWAJR
J-C
JBSCW
JZLTJ
KOV
LLZTM
M1P
M2M
M2O
M2R
M4Y
MVM
N2Q
N9A
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9G
O9J
OHT
OK1
P2P
P9L
PADUT
PF-
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT4
R9I
RIG
ROL
RPV
RSV
S16
S1Z
S27
S3B
SBS
SBU
SCLPG
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TN5
TR2
TSG
TUC
TUS
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UOJIU
UPT
UTJUX
UZXMN
VFIZW
VXZ
W48
WH7
WK8
XJT
XOL
XSW
Z7R
Z7S
Z7W
Z81
Z83
Z88
Z8N
Z92
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
ZUP
AAPKM
AAYXX
ABBRH
ABDBE
ABFSG
ACSTC
AEZWR
AFDZB
AFHIU
AFOHR
AHPBZ
AHWEU
AIXLP
ATHPR
AYFIA
CITATION
ABRTQ
ADHKG
AGQPQ
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
PHGZM
PHGZT
PJZUB
PPXIY
PRQQA
4T-
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c542t-e23e0cea1fe5ab354951257b18fe00c440a848d7a2d862bd0d385ae4b0d3a503
IEDL.DBID U2A
ISSN 1554-3528
1554-351X
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:34:42 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 10:13:47 EDT 2025
Wed Sep 03 07:11:02 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 06:00:48 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:06:02 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:13:47 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:41:09 EST 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 3
Keywords Within-subjects designs
Reaction times
Sample size
Practice effects
Statistical power
Number of trials
Language English
License 2023. The Author(s).
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c542t-e23e0cea1fe5ab354951257b18fe00c440a848d7a2d862bd0d385ae4b0d3a503
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13428-023-02155-9
PMID 37537492
PQID 3031733055
PQPubID 976348
PageCount 24
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_10991062
proquest_miscellaneous_2846924036
proquest_journals_3031733055
pubmed_primary_37537492
crossref_citationtrail_10_3758_s13428_023_02155_9
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_023_02155_9
springer_journals_10_3758_s13428_023_02155_9
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2024-03-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2024-03-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 03
  year: 2024
  text: 2024-03-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace New York
PublicationPlace_xml – name: New York
– name: United States
PublicationTitle Behavior research methods
PublicationTitleAbbrev Behav Res
PublicationTitleAlternate Behav Res Methods
PublicationYear 2024
Publisher Springer US
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer US
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Mazor, Fleming (CR15) 2022; 151
De Wit, Kinoshita (CR7) 2015; 43
Hutchison, Balota, Neely, Cortese, Cohen-Shikora, Tse, Yap, Bengson, Niemeyer, Buchanan (CR11) 2013; 45
Semmelmann, Weigelt (CR21) 2017; 49
Rouder, Haaf (CR19) 2018; 1
Den Heyer, Briand, Dannenbring (CR8) 1983; 11
Bazilinskyy, De Winter (CR3) 2018; 60
Goh, Yap, Chee (CR9) 2020; 52
Westfall, Kenny, Judd (CR25) 2014; 143
Kochari (CR13) 2019; 2
Baker, Vilidaite, Lygo, Smith, Flack, Gouws, Andrews (CR2) 2021; 26
CR14
CR12
Hilbig (CR10) 2016; 48
Shiffrin, Schneider (CR22) 1977; 84
Clark (CR6) 1973; 12
Ratcliff, Hendrickson (CR18) 2021; 53
Pexman, Heard, Lloyd, Yap (CR17) 2017; 49
Adelman, Johnson, McCormick, McKague, Kinoshita, Bowers, Perry, Lupker, Forster, Cortese, Scaltritti, Aschenbrenner, Coane, White, Yap, Davis, Kim, Davis (CR1) 2014; 46
CR4
CR5
Miguel-Abella, Pérez-Sánchez, Cuetos, Marín, González-Nosti (CR16) 2022; 54
Worringham, Stelmach (CR26) 1990; 53
CR24
Smith, Little (CR23) 2018; 25
Ruthruff, Johnston, Van Selst (CR20) 2001; 27
K Semmelmann (2155_CR21) 2017; 49
DH Baker (2155_CR2) 2021; 26
BE Hilbig (2155_CR10) 2016; 48
RS Miguel-Abella (2155_CR16) 2022; 54
JS Adelman (2155_CR1) 2014; 46
2155_CR4
2155_CR5
B De Wit (2155_CR7) 2015; 43
2155_CR12
AR Kochari (2155_CR13) 2019; 2
KA Hutchison (2155_CR11) 2013; 45
HH Clark (2155_CR6) 1973; 12
2155_CR14
PL Smith (2155_CR23) 2018; 25
JN Rouder (2155_CR19) 2018; 1
WD Goh (2155_CR9) 2020; 52
J Westfall (2155_CR25) 2014; 143
M Mazor (2155_CR15) 2022; 151
CJ Worringham (2155_CR26) 1990; 53
R Ratcliff (2155_CR18) 2021; 53
ED Ruthruff (2155_CR20) 2001; 27
K Den Heyer (2155_CR8) 1983; 11
RM Shiffrin (2155_CR22) 1977; 84
P Bazilinskyy (2155_CR3) 2018; 60
PM Pexman (2155_CR17) 2017; 49
2155_CR24
References_xml – volume: 2
  start-page: 1
  issue: 1:39
  year: 2019
  end-page: 21
  ident: CR13
  article-title: Conducting web-based experiments for numerical cognition research
  publication-title: Journal of Cognition
  doi: 10.5334/joc.85
– volume: 54
  start-page: 2640
  year: 2022
  end-page: 2664
  ident: CR16
  article-title: SpaVerb-WN–a megastudy of naming times for 4562 Spanish verbs: Effects of psycholinguistic and motor content variables
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01734-y
– volume: 49
  start-page: 407
  issue: 2
  year: 2017
  end-page: 417
  ident: CR17
  article-title: The Calgary semantic decision project: Concrete/abstract decision data for 10,000 English words
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0720-6
– ident: CR4
– volume: 43
  start-page: 99
  issue: 1
  year: 2015
  end-page: 110
  ident: CR7
  article-title: An RT distribution analysis of relatedness proportion effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization reveals different mechanisms
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0446-6
– ident: CR14
– ident: CR12
– volume: 27
  start-page: 3
  issue: 1
  year: 2001
  end-page: 21
  ident: CR20
  article-title: Why practice reduces dual-task interference
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.3
– volume: 53
  start-page: 2302
  issue: 6
  year: 2021
  end-page: 2325
  ident: CR18
  article-title: Do data from mechanical Turk subjects replicate accuracy, response time, and diffusion modeling results?
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01573-x
– volume: 60
  start-page: 1192
  issue: 8
  year: 2018
  end-page: 1206
  ident: CR3
  article-title: Crowdsourced measurement of reaction times to audiovisual stimuli with various degrees of asynchrony
  publication-title: Human Factors
  doi: 10.1177/0018720818787126
– volume: 26
  start-page: 295
  issue: 3
  year: 2021
  end-page: 314
  ident: CR2
  article-title: Power contours: Optimising sample size and precision in experimental psychology and human neuroscience
  publication-title: Psychological Methods
  doi: 10.1037/met0000337
– volume: 84
  start-page: 127
  year: 1977
  end-page: 190
  ident: CR22
  article-title: Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
– volume: 143
  start-page: 2020
  issue: 5
  year: 2014
  end-page: 2045
  ident: CR25
  article-title: Statistical power and optimal design in experiments in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000014
– volume: 49
  start-page: 1241
  issue: 4
  year: 2017
  end-page: 1260
  ident: CR21
  article-title: Online psychophysics: reaction time effects in cognitive experiments
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0783-4
– volume: 46
  start-page: 1052
  issue: 4
  year: 2014
  end-page: 1067
  ident: CR1
  article-title: A behavioral database for masked form priming
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0442-y
– volume: 11
  start-page: 374
  year: 1983
  end-page: 381
  ident: CR8
  article-title: Strategic factors in a lexical-decision task: Evidence for automatic and attention-driven processes
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0446-6
– volume: 1
  start-page: 19
  issue: 1
  year: 2018
  end-page: 26
  ident: CR19
  article-title: Power, dominance, and constraint: A note on the appeal of different design traditions
  publication-title: Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/2515245917745058
– volume: 12
  start-page: 335
  issue: 4
  year: 1973
  end-page: 359
  ident: CR6
  article-title: The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3
– volume: 151
  start-page: 2494
  issue: 10
  year: 2022
  end-page: 2510
  ident: CR15
  article-title: Efficient search termination without task experience
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0001188
– volume: 48
  start-page: 1718
  issue: 4
  year: 2016
  end-page: 1724
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Reaction time effects in lab- versus web-based research: Experimental evidence
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0678-9
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1099
  issue: 4
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1114
  ident: CR11
  article-title: The Semantic Priming Project
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0304-z
– volume: 25
  start-page: 2083
  issue: 6
  year: 2018
  end-page: 2101
  ident: CR23
  article-title: Small is beautiful: In defense of the small-N design
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1451-8
– ident: CR5
– volume: 53
  start-page: 702
  year: 1990
  end-page: 704
  ident: CR26
  article-title: Practice effects on the preprogramming of discrete movements in Parkinson’s disease
  publication-title: Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry
  doi: 10.1136/jnnp.53.8.702
– ident: CR24
– volume: 52
  start-page: 2202
  issue: 5
  year: 2020
  end-page: 2231
  ident: CR9
  article-title: The Auditory English Lexicon Project: A multi-talker, multi-region psycholinguistic database of 10,170 spoken words and nonwords
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0678-9
– volume: 48
  start-page: 1718
  issue: 4
  year: 2016
  ident: 2155_CR10
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0678-9
– volume: 25
  start-page: 2083
  issue: 6
  year: 2018
  ident: 2155_CR23
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1451-8
– ident: 2155_CR12
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.5.1015
– volume: 12
  start-page: 335
  issue: 4
  year: 1973
  ident: 2155_CR6
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3
– ident: 2155_CR4
  doi: 10.5334/joc.10
– volume: 54
  start-page: 2640
  year: 2022
  ident: 2155_CR16
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01734-y
– volume: 1
  start-page: 19
  issue: 1
  year: 2018
  ident: 2155_CR19
  publication-title: Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/2515245917745058
– volume: 53
  start-page: 2302
  issue: 6
  year: 2021
  ident: 2155_CR18
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01573-x
– volume: 11
  start-page: 374
  year: 1983
  ident: 2155_CR8
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0446-6
– volume: 27
  start-page: 3
  issue: 1
  year: 2001
  ident: 2155_CR20
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.3
– volume: 53
  start-page: 702
  year: 1990
  ident: 2155_CR26
  publication-title: Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry
  doi: 10.1136/jnnp.53.8.702
– volume: 26
  start-page: 295
  issue: 3
  year: 2021
  ident: 2155_CR2
  publication-title: Psychological Methods
  doi: 10.1037/met0000337
– volume: 46
  start-page: 1052
  issue: 4
  year: 2014
  ident: 2155_CR1
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0442-y
– ident: 2155_CR24
– ident: 2155_CR14
  doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00352-8
– volume: 49
  start-page: 1241
  issue: 4
  year: 2017
  ident: 2155_CR21
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0783-4
– volume: 84
  start-page: 127
  year: 1977
  ident: 2155_CR22
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
– volume: 60
  start-page: 1192
  issue: 8
  year: 2018
  ident: 2155_CR3
  publication-title: Human Factors
  doi: 10.1177/0018720818787126
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1099
  issue: 4
  year: 2013
  ident: 2155_CR11
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0304-z
– volume: 2
  start-page: 1
  issue: 1:39
  year: 2019
  ident: 2155_CR13
  publication-title: Journal of Cognition
  doi: 10.5334/joc.85
– volume: 43
  start-page: 99
  issue: 1
  year: 2015
  ident: 2155_CR7
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0446-6
– volume: 52
  start-page: 2202
  issue: 5
  year: 2020
  ident: 2155_CR9
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0678-9
– volume: 49
  start-page: 407
  issue: 2
  year: 2017
  ident: 2155_CR17
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0720-6
– volume: 143
  start-page: 2020
  issue: 5
  year: 2014
  ident: 2155_CR25
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0000014
– ident: 2155_CR5
  doi: 10.1002/9781119095910.ch2
– volume: 151
  start-page: 2494
  issue: 10
  year: 2022
  ident: 2155_CR15
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/xge0001188
SSID ssj0038214
Score 2.4728444
Snippet Due to limitations in the resources available for carrying out reaction time (RT) experiments, researchers often have to choose between testing relatively few...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
springer
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 2398
SubjectTerms Behavioral Science and Psychology
Cognitive Psychology
Humans
Psychology
Reaction Time
Response time
Title How Many Participants? How Many Trials? Maximizing the Power of Reaction Time Studies
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13428-023-02155-9
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37537492
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3031733055
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2846924036
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC10991062
Volume 56
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3daxQxEB9s-9IX0fq1Wo8Ivuni5muTfSrH0XoolSJ3UJ9CsjuHB7on7hU__non-1XOUsG3_UjI7mSSmR8z8wvAy4p8-pzgccpNEKnSQaahQEKt3q9Qcm9CS_Z8_iGfL9W7S33ZF4U1Q7b7EJJsd-qIK8mpfdNwqSKbsohxR7KCabEHBzpid9LipZgO-6-0gquuPOaWfrsm6IZfeTM98q8YaWt6zu7B3d5nZNNuku_DHayP4HDcun49gOV884Od07pmF77Pk663zQkbHy9aPTuhm5_rr-vfNAwjz49dxDPS2GbFPmJX4MBiSQjrkwsfwuLsdDGbp_2BCWmpldimKCRmJXq-Qu2DJOhH5lybwO0Ks6xUKvNW2cp4URGQCVVWSas9qkAXXmfyEezXmxqfADOmtMGailcBlYikbKasEKUyuuTIbQJ8EKErezLxeKbFF0egIorddWJ3JHbXit0VCbwa-3zrqDT-2fp4mBnXL6vGkb3lRkaSsgRejK9pQcQoh69xc9U4srd5EVkG8wQedxM5DkdjSaMKkYDdmeKxQSTb3n1Trz-3pNsxgkjwmbq-HrTh-rtu_42n_9f8GRySRqsuye0Y9rffr_A5eT3bMIGD6dtP708nsDfLZ5NW5f8Acy78Jg
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3fb9MwED5BeaAv04AB2QoYaW8jIv6R2HmapoqpwDpNUyf1zbKTq6gE6bR22uCv3zlJA2UaEm9JbMvJnZ27T3f3GWC_JJ8-I3gcc-1FrFIvY58joVbnZii5074mex6fZqML9WWaTluanFAL80f8XpIr-3HJpQocyiJEG8n2xfljeKIIKYf0vWE2XP91pRFcNUUxD4zbNDz3vMn7SZF_RUZrg3O8DVutp8iOGtU-g0dYPYd-98P6-QIuRosbNqbdzM5cmx1drZaHrHs8qVfXId3czn_Mf9E0jPw9dhZORmOLGTvHpqyBhUIQ1qYU7sDk-NNkOIrbYxLiIlViFaOQmBTo-AxT5yUBPjLiqfbczDBJCqUSZ5QptRMlwRdfJqU0qUPl6cKliXwJvWpR4WtgWhfGG13y0qMSgYpNFyWiVDotOHITAV-L0BYthXg4yeK7JSgRxG4bsVsSu63FbvMIDroxlw2Bxj97D9aase1mWlqyslzLQE0WwfuumbZBiG24ChfXS0tWNssDt2AWwatGkd10NJfUKhcRmA0Vdx0CxfZmSzX_VlNth7ghgWYa-mG9Gn6_18Ofsft_3d_B09FkfGJPPp9-3YO-IL-pSXMbQG91dY1vyO9Z-bf1gr8DNbb4hw
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3dSxwxEB-sQvFFrP3aamsKfWsXNx-7yT5JufY4bZWjnOBbSDZZetDuibdS7V_fyX61pyj0bXeTkOxMJjPDzPwC8M6hTZ-hexxTaVksUstjm3v0Wo0pPadG2gbs-eQ0m5yJ4_P0_J8q_ibbvQ9JtjUNAaWpqg8uXBlEnKOBe7CkXARkZRZikKgR4_wRbKCn0gRqR9moP4u5YlS0pTL3jFtVR3dszLupkrfipY0aGm_DVmc_ko8tw5_Amq92YHM4xm6ewtlk8YucoIyTqelypqt6eUiGz7Nmzx3iy_X85_w3TkPQCiTTcF8aWZTkm2-LHUgoDyFdouEzmI0_z0aTuLs8IS5SwerYM-6Twhta-tRYjm4gqvZUWqpKnySFEIlRQjlpmEOnxrrEcZUaLyw-mDThz2G9WlT-JRApC2WVdNRZL1gAaJOF854LmRbUUxUB7Umoiw5YPNxv8UOjgxHIrluyayS7bsiu8wjeD2MuWliNB3vv9ZzRnYgtNepeKnkALIvg7dCMwhEiHqbyi6ulRt2b5QFxMIvgRcvIYTqci0uRswjUCouHDgF4e7Wlmn9vALhDNBFdaRz6od8Nf9d1_2-8-r_u-_B4-mmsvx6dftmFTYbGVJv7tgfr9eWVf43GUG3fNPv9D-xnAN0
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=How+Many+Participants%3F+How+Many+Trials%3F+Maximizing+the+Power+of+Reaction+Time+Studies&rft.jtitle=Behavior+research+methods&rft.au=Miller%2C+Jeff&rft.date=2024-03-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.eissn=1554-3528&rft.volume=56&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=2398&rft.epage=2421&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13428-023-02155-9&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon