Quantifying accuracy and precision from continuous response data in studies of spatial perception and crossmodal recalibration

The ability to detect the absolute location of sensory stimuli can be quantified with either error-based metrics derived from single-trial localization errors or regression-based metrics derived from a linear regression of localization responses on the true stimulus locations. Here we tested the agr...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBehavior research methods Vol. 56; no. 4; pp. 3814 - 3830
Main Authors Bruns, Patrick, Thun, Caroline, Röder, Brigitte
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published New York Springer US 01.06.2024
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1554-3528
1554-351X
1554-3528
DOI10.3758/s13428-024-02416-1

Cover

Abstract The ability to detect the absolute location of sensory stimuli can be quantified with either error-based metrics derived from single-trial localization errors or regression-based metrics derived from a linear regression of localization responses on the true stimulus locations. Here we tested the agreement between these two approaches in estimating accuracy and precision in a large sample of 188 subjects who localized auditory stimuli from different azimuthal locations. A subsample of 57 subjects was subsequently exposed to audiovisual stimuli with a consistent spatial disparity before performing the sound localization test again, allowing us to additionally test which of the different metrics best assessed correlations between the amount of crossmodal spatial recalibration and baseline localization performance. First, our findings support a distinction between accuracy and precision. Localization accuracy was mainly reflected in the overall spatial bias and was moderately correlated with precision metrics. However, in our data, the variability of single-trial localization errors (variable error in error-based metrics) and the amount by which the eccentricity of target locations was overestimated (slope in regression-based metrics) were highly correlated, suggesting that intercorrelations between individual metrics need to be carefully considered in spatial perception studies. Secondly, exposure to spatially discrepant audiovisual stimuli resulted in a shift in bias toward the side of the visual stimuli (ventriloquism aftereffect) but did not affect localization precision. The size of the aftereffect shift in bias was at least partly explainable by unspecific test repetition effects, highlighting the need to account for inter-individual baseline differences in studies of spatial learning.
AbstractList The ability to detect the absolute location of sensory stimuli can be quantified with either error-based metrics derived from single-trial localization errors or regression-based metrics derived from a linear regression of localization responses on the true stimulus locations. Here we tested the agreement between these two approaches in estimating accuracy and precision in a large sample of 188 subjects who localized auditory stimuli from different azimuthal locations. A subsample of 57 subjects was subsequently exposed to audiovisual stimuli with a consistent spatial disparity before performing the sound localization test again, allowing us to additionally test which of the different metrics best assessed correlations between the amount of crossmodal spatial recalibration and baseline localization performance. First, our findings support a distinction between accuracy and precision. Localization accuracy was mainly reflected in the overall spatial bias and was moderately correlated with precision metrics. However, in our data, the variability of single-trial localization errors (variable error in error-based metrics) and the amount by which the eccentricity of target locations was overestimated (slope in regression-based metrics) were highly correlated, suggesting that intercorrelations between individual metrics need to be carefully considered in spatial perception studies. Secondly, exposure to spatially discrepant audiovisual stimuli resulted in a shift in bias toward the side of the visual stimuli (ventriloquism aftereffect) but did not affect localization precision. The size of the aftereffect shift in bias was at least partly explainable by unspecific test repetition effects, highlighting the need to account for inter-individual baseline differences in studies of spatial learning.
The ability to detect the absolute location of sensory stimuli can be quantified with either error-based metrics derived from single-trial localization errors or regression-based metrics derived from a linear regression of localization responses on the true stimulus locations. Here we tested the agreement between these two approaches in estimating accuracy and precision in a large sample of 188 subjects who localized auditory stimuli from different azimuthal locations. A subsample of 57 subjects was subsequently exposed to audiovisual stimuli with a consistent spatial disparity before performing the sound localization test again, allowing us to additionally test which of the different metrics best assessed correlations between the amount of crossmodal spatial recalibration and baseline localization performance. First, our findings support a distinction between accuracy and precision. Localization accuracy was mainly reflected in the overall spatial bias and was moderately correlated with precision metrics. However, in our data, the variability of single-trial localization errors (variable error in error-based metrics) and the amount by which the eccentricity of target locations was overestimated (slope in regression-based metrics) were highly correlated, suggesting that intercorrelations between individual metrics need to be carefully considered in spatial perception studies. Secondly, exposure to spatially discrepant audiovisual stimuli resulted in a shift in bias toward the side of the visual stimuli (ventriloquism aftereffect) but did not affect localization precision. The size of the aftereffect shift in bias was at least partly explainable by unspecific test repetition effects, highlighting the need to account for inter-individual baseline differences in studies of spatial learning.The ability to detect the absolute location of sensory stimuli can be quantified with either error-based metrics derived from single-trial localization errors or regression-based metrics derived from a linear regression of localization responses on the true stimulus locations. Here we tested the agreement between these two approaches in estimating accuracy and precision in a large sample of 188 subjects who localized auditory stimuli from different azimuthal locations. A subsample of 57 subjects was subsequently exposed to audiovisual stimuli with a consistent spatial disparity before performing the sound localization test again, allowing us to additionally test which of the different metrics best assessed correlations between the amount of crossmodal spatial recalibration and baseline localization performance. First, our findings support a distinction between accuracy and precision. Localization accuracy was mainly reflected in the overall spatial bias and was moderately correlated with precision metrics. However, in our data, the variability of single-trial localization errors (variable error in error-based metrics) and the amount by which the eccentricity of target locations was overestimated (slope in regression-based metrics) were highly correlated, suggesting that intercorrelations between individual metrics need to be carefully considered in spatial perception studies. Secondly, exposure to spatially discrepant audiovisual stimuli resulted in a shift in bias toward the side of the visual stimuli (ventriloquism aftereffect) but did not affect localization precision. The size of the aftereffect shift in bias was at least partly explainable by unspecific test repetition effects, highlighting the need to account for inter-individual baseline differences in studies of spatial learning.
Author Röder, Brigitte
Thun, Caroline
Bruns, Patrick
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Patrick
  orcidid: 0000-0002-2977-8874
  surname: Bruns
  fullname: Bruns, Patrick
  email: patrick.bruns@uni-hamburg.de
  organization: Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Hamburg
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Caroline
  surname: Thun
  fullname: Thun, Caroline
  organization: Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Hamburg
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Brigitte
  surname: Röder
  fullname: Röder, Brigitte
  organization: Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Hamburg
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38684625$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kkuL1TAYhoOMOBf9Ay4k4MZNtbk2WYkM3mBABF2HNP16zNAmNUmFs_G3m54zjuMsZhES-J73zXc7RychBkDoOWlfs06oN5kwTlXTUr4dIhvyCJ0RIXjDBFUnd96n6Dzn67ZlihL-BJ0yJRWXVJyh319XG4of9z7ssHVuTdbtsQ0DXhI4n30MeExxxi5WLKxxzThBXmLIgAdbLPYB57IOHjKOI86LLd5OeIHkYCmbfDNzKeY8x6FGqq2dfJ_sFnyKHo92yvDs5r5A3z-8_3b5qbn68vHz5burxglOSzNYwoV2fATSjh2VmrRC6172I2MdcEm0bnUPI3VWst4R7piyIKgbmNbSMnaB3h59l7WfYXAQSrKTWZKfbdqbaL35PxL8D7OLvwwhhDFCZHV4deOQ4s8VcjGzzw6myQaoTTGs5bqjlIuuoi_voddxTaHWVylJpWJSiUq9uJvSbS5_Z1MBegQOzUsw3iKkNdsCmOMCmDp8c1gAQ6pI3RM5Xw6trmX56WEpO0pz_SfsIP1L-wHVH3ojx_c
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2024_1408073
Cites_doi 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220
10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004649
10.1038/nn999
10.1097/WNR.0000000000000521
10.3758/s13414-020-02083-2
10.1068/p130581
10.1016/j.heares.2007.11.006
10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.048
10.1016/s0378-5955(97)00161-5
10.1038/srep40567
10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.001031
10.1016/0278-2626(92)90107-w
10.3758/s13414-014-0699-y
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2359-19.2020
10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029
10.1121/1.424371
10.7554/eLife.61238
10.1016/j.heares.2009.04.009
10.1121/1.421222
10.1016/j.heares.2013.07.008
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06910.x
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6427-09.2010
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.002
10.1038/s41598-018-34512-6
10.1038/s41598-018-25119-y
10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00141-2
10.1037/a0027339
10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093544
10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104170
10.1002/ana.20862
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2732-11.2011
10.1163/22134808-bja10053
10.1037/h0075482
10.1016/s0926-6410(01)00042-8
10.1177/0956797616628860
10.1371/journal.pone.0018344
10.3389/fnint.2019.00074
10.1121/1.399186
10.3758/s13414-013-0475-4
10.1038/417322a
10.1111/j.1749-6632.1951.tb27345.x
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01597.2000
10.1101/lm.51402
10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.006
10.1177/0956797611416254
10.1098/rstb.2008.0230
10.1162/089892903321107792
10.1111/ejn.15001
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2091-20.2020
10.1163/187847611X620883
10.1121/1.395155
10.1177/0956797616689369
10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.013
10.1073/pnas.95.3.869
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0048-04.2004
10.3758/s13428-021-01633-2
10.1016/j.cortex.2019.03.026
10.1121/1.397558
10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7
10.25592/uhhfdm.948
10.1037/xhp0000642
10.3389/fnint.2019.00051
10.1007/978-3-030-10461-0_11
10.1111/ejn.14779
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright The Author(s) 2024
2024. The Author(s).
The Author(s) 2024. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Copyright_xml – notice: The Author(s) 2024
– notice: 2024. The Author(s).
– notice: The Author(s) 2024. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
DBID C6C
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
4T-
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.3758/s13428-024-02416-1
DatabaseName Springer Nature OA Free Journals
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Docstoc
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Neurosciences Abstracts
Docstoc
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
MEDLINE - Academic
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
CrossRef


Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: C6C
  name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals
  url: http://www.springeropen.com/
  sourceTypes: Publisher
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1554-3528
EndPage 3830
ExternalDocumentID PMC11133116
38684625
10_3758_s13428_024_02416_1
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Universität Hamburg (1037)
– fundername: Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
  grantid: TRR 169/A1
  funderid: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001659
– fundername: Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
  grantid: TRR 169/A1
GroupedDBID ---
-55
-5G
-BR
-DZ
-EM
-ET
-~C
-~X
0-V
06D
0R~
0VY
199
1N0
203
23N
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2VQ
30V
3V.
4.4
406
408
40E
53G
5GY
7X7
875
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8G5
8TC
8UJ
95.
96X
AAAVM
AABHQ
AACDK
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AAKPC
AANZL
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
AAZMS
ABAKF
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABJUD
ABKCH
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABPPZ
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABUWG
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACBXY
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIWK
ACKIV
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACPIV
ACPRK
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETCA
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFFNX
AFKRA
AFQWF
AFRAH
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHMBA
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARALO
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
AZQEC
B-.
BAWUL
BENPR
BGNMA
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C1A
C6C
CAG
CCPQU
COF
CSCUP
DDRTE
DIK
DNIVK
DPUIP
DWQXO
E3Z
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMOBN
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FYUFA
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ3
GQ6
GQ7
GUQSH
H13
HF~
HMCUK
HMJXF
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
IAO
IHR
IKXTQ
INH
IPY
IRVIT
ITC
ITM
IWAJR
J-C
JBSCW
JZLTJ
KOV
LLZTM
M1P
M2M
M2O
M2R
M4Y
MVM
N2Q
N9A
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9G
O9J
OHT
OK1
P2P
P9L
PADUT
PF-
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT4
R9I
RIG
ROL
RPV
RSV
S16
S1Z
S27
S3B
SBS
SBU
SCLPG
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TN5
TR2
TSG
TUC
TUS
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UOJIU
UPT
UTJUX
UZXMN
VFIZW
VXZ
W48
WH7
WK8
XJT
XOL
XSW
Z7R
Z7S
Z7W
Z81
Z83
Z88
Z8N
Z92
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
ZUP
AAPKM
AAYXX
ABBRH
ABDBE
ABFSG
ACSTC
AEZWR
AFDZB
AFHIU
AFOHR
AHPBZ
AHWEU
AIXLP
ATHPR
AYFIA
CITATION
ABRTQ
ADHKG
AGQPQ
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
PHGZM
PHGZT
PJZUB
PPXIY
PRQQA
4T-
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c542t-da1459c4fe10f726910599b6bf337e4619909bef2ca63bc14c38ae52cd3996a33
IEDL.DBID AGYKE
ISSN 1554-3528
1554-351X
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:33:42 EDT 2025
Fri Sep 05 04:27:36 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 04:18:58 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 05:59:54 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:58:38 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:06:03 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:42:37 EST 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 4
Keywords Audiovisual
Ventriloquism aftereffect
Sensorimotor tasks
Psychophysics
Localization errors
Language English
License 2024. The Author(s).
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c542t-da1459c4fe10f726910599b6bf337e4619909bef2ca63bc14c38ae52cd3996a33
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0002-2977-8874
OpenAccessLink https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13428-024-02416-1
PMID 38684625
PQID 3062683685
PQPubID 976348
PageCount 17
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11133116
proquest_miscellaneous_3049722457
proquest_journals_3062683685
pubmed_primary_38684625
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_024_02416_1
crossref_citationtrail_10_3758_s13428_024_02416_1
springer_journals_10_3758_s13428_024_02416_1
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2024-06-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2024-06-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 06
  year: 2024
  text: 2024-06-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace New York
PublicationPlace_xml – name: New York
– name: United States
PublicationTitle Behavior research methods
PublicationTitleAbbrev Behav Res
PublicationTitleAlternate Behav Res Methods
PublicationYear 2024
Publisher Springer US
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer US
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Ege, van Opstal, van Wanrooij (CR20) 2018; 8
Badde, Ley, Rajendran, Shareef, Kekunnaya, Röder (CR4) 2020; 9
Wightman, Kistler (CR65) 1989; 85
Lewald (CR32) 2002; 9
Zaidel, Turner, Angelaki (CR66) 2011; 31
Schmidt, Lee, Winstein, Wulf, Zelaznik (CR57) 2019
Carlile, Leong, Hyams (CR16) 1997; 114
Kramer, Röder, Bruns (CR31) 2020; 13
Perrott, Ambarsoom, Tucker (CR47) 1987; 82
Bruns, Maiworm, Röder (CR12) 2014; 76
Lewald, Ehrenstein (CR34) 1998; 104
Zwiers, Van Opstal, Paige (CR68) 2003; 6
Strelnikov, Rosito, Barone (CR60) 2011; 6
Huttenlocher, Hedges, Vevea (CR28) 2000; 129
Burge, Girshick, Banks (CR15) 2010; 30
Passamonti, Frissen, Làdavas (CR46) 2009; 30
Odegaard, Wozny, Shams (CR43) 2015; 11
CR8
CR7
CR9
Recanzone (CR50) 1998; 95
Lewald (CR33) 2007; 45
Bruns, Liebnau, Röder (CR11) 2011; 22
Lewald, Dörrscheidt, Ehrenstein (CR36) 2000; 108
Garcia, Jones, Rubin, Nardini (CR23) 2017; 7
Badde, Navarro, Landy (CR5) 2020; 197
Grabot, van Wassenhove (CR26) 2017; 28
Meijer, Veselič, Calafiore, Noppeney (CR38) 2019; 119
Blauert (CR6) 1997
Knudsen (CR30) 2002; 417
Odegaard, Shams (CR42) 2016; 27
Van Wanrooij, Van Opstal (CR62) 2004; 24
Chen, Vroomen (CR18) 2013; 75
Recanzone, Makhamra, Guard (CR53) 1998; 103
Hairston, Wallace, Vaughan, Stein, Norris, Schirillo (CR27) 2003; 15
King (CR29) 2009; 364
Ahveninen, Kopčo, Jääskeläinen (CR1) 2014; 307
Oldfield, Parker (CR44) 1984; 13
Lewald, Ehrenstein (CR35) 2001; 12
Middlebrooks, Green (CR39) 1991; 42
Samad, Shams (CR56) 2016; 27
CR14
CR13
Chapanis (CR17) 1951; 51
Dinse, Kleibel, Kalisch, Ragert, Wilimzig, Tegenthoff (CR19) 2006; 60
CR10
Makous, Middlebrooks (CR37) 1990; 87
Grabot, Kayser (CR25) 2020; 40
Aston, Negen, Nardini, Beierholm (CR3) 2022; 54
Rohlf, Li, Bruns, Röder (CR55) 2020; 30
Alais, Burr (CR2) 2004; 14
Stevenson, Zemtsov, Wallace (CR58) 2012; 38
Vroomen, Stekelenburg (CR63) 2021; 53
Rohlf, Bruns, Röder (CR54) 2021; 34
Recanzone, Sutter (CR52) 2008; 59
Moore, Tollin, Yin (CR40) 2008; 238
Pinek, Brouchon (CR48) 1992; 18
Recanzone (CR51) 2009; 258
Godde, Stauffenberg, Spengler, Dinse (CR24) 2000; 20
Park, Kayser (CR45) 2021; 41
Zierul, Röder, Tempelmann, Bruns, Noesselt (CR67) 2017; 162
Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, Lang (CR21) 2009; 41
CR64
Tong, Li, Bruns, Röder (CR61) 2020; 82
Polti, Martin, van Wassenhove (CR49) 2018; 8
Stratton (CR59) 1897; 4
Frissen, Vroomen, de Gelder (CR22) 2012; 25
Ocklenburg, Hirnstein, Hausmann, Lewald (CR41) 2010; 72
S Rohlf (2416_CR55) 2020; 30
A Zaidel (2416_CR66) 2011; 31
HR Dinse (2416_CR19) 2006; 60
2416_CR13
F Faul (2416_CR21) 2009; 41
2416_CR14
2416_CR10
J Lewald (2416_CR33) 2007; 45
GH Recanzone (2416_CR52) 2008; 59
I Frissen (2416_CR22) 2012; 25
C Passamonti (2416_CR46) 2009; 30
MP Zwiers (2416_CR68) 2003; 6
L Grabot (2416_CR26) 2017; 28
J Vroomen (2416_CR63) 2021; 53
J Lewald (2416_CR34) 1998; 104
S Aston (2416_CR3) 2022; 54
M Samad (2416_CR56) 2016; 27
S Carlile (2416_CR16) 1997; 114
AJ King (2416_CR29) 2009; 364
B Zierul (2416_CR67) 2017; 162
JC Makous (2416_CR37) 1990; 87
I Polti (2416_CR49) 2018; 8
D Meijer (2416_CR38) 2019; 119
B Godde (2416_CR24) 2000; 20
MM Van Wanrooij (2416_CR62) 2004; 24
RA Stevenson (2416_CR58) 2012; 38
S Badde (2416_CR5) 2020; 197
J Lewald (2416_CR36) 2000; 108
J Burge (2416_CR15) 2010; 30
GH Recanzone (2416_CR53) 1998; 103
B Pinek (2416_CR48) 1992; 18
H Park (2416_CR45) 2021; 41
J Ahveninen (2416_CR1) 2014; 307
SE Garcia (2416_CR23) 2017; 7
RA Schmidt (2416_CR57) 2019
K Strelnikov (2416_CR60) 2011; 6
J Lewald (2416_CR32) 2002; 9
DR Perrott (2416_CR47) 1987; 82
D Alais (2416_CR2) 2004; 14
2416_CR8
2416_CR7
2416_CR9
GH Recanzone (2416_CR50) 1998; 95
GM Stratton (2416_CR59) 1897; 4
P Bruns (2416_CR11) 2011; 22
P Bruns (2416_CR12) 2014; 76
A Chapanis (2416_CR17) 1951; 51
A Kramer (2416_CR31) 2020; 13
FL Wightman (2416_CR65) 1989; 85
S Badde (2416_CR4) 2020; 9
B Odegaard (2416_CR43) 2015; 11
JC Middlebrooks (2416_CR39) 1991; 42
S Ocklenburg (2416_CR41) 2010; 72
L Chen (2416_CR18) 2013; 75
B Odegaard (2416_CR42) 2016; 27
L Grabot (2416_CR25) 2020; 40
S Rohlf (2416_CR54) 2021; 34
2416_CR64
J Blauert (2416_CR6) 1997
J Tong (2416_CR61) 2020; 82
SR Oldfield (2416_CR44) 1984; 13
JM Moore (2416_CR40) 2008; 238
GH Recanzone (2416_CR51) 2009; 258
J Huttenlocher (2416_CR28) 2000; 129
R Ege (2416_CR20) 2018; 8
WD Hairston (2416_CR27) 2003; 15
EI Knudsen (2416_CR30) 2002; 417
J Lewald (2416_CR35) 2001; 12
References_xml – volume: 129
  start-page: 220
  year: 2000
  end-page: 241
  ident: CR28
  article-title: Why do categories affect stimulus judgment?
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220
– volume: 11
  start-page: e1004649
  year: 2015
  ident: CR43
  article-title: Biases in visual, auditory, and audiovisual perception of space
  publication-title: PloS Computational Biology
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004649
– volume: 6
  start-page: 175
  year: 2003
  end-page: 181
  ident: CR68
  article-title: Plasticity in human sound localization induced by compressed spatial vision
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/nn999
– volume: 27
  start-page: 180
  year: 2016
  end-page: 185
  ident: CR56
  article-title: Visual-somatotopic interactions in spatial perception
  publication-title: NeuroReport
  doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000521
– volume: 82
  start-page: 3490
  year: 2020
  end-page: 3506
  ident: CR61
  article-title: Crossmodal associations modulate multisensory spatial integration
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-020-02083-2
– volume: 13
  start-page: 581
  year: 1984
  end-page: 600
  ident: CR44
  article-title: Acuity of sound localization: A topography of auditory space. I. Normal hearing conditions
  publication-title: Perception
  doi: 10.1068/p130581
– volume: 238
  start-page: 94
  year: 2008
  end-page: 109
  ident: CR40
  article-title: Can measures of sound localization acuity be related to the precision of absolute location estimates?
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2007.11.006
– volume: 30
  start-page: 1726
  year: 2020
  end-page: 1732
  ident: CR55
  article-title: Multisensory integration develops prior to crossmodal recalibration
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.048
– volume: 114
  start-page: 179
  year: 1997
  end-page: 196
  ident: CR16
  article-title: The nature and distribution of errors in sound localization by human listeners
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(97)00161-5
– volume: 7
  start-page: 40567
  year: 2017
  ident: CR23
  article-title: Auditory localisation biases increase with sensory uncertainty
  publication-title: Scientific Reports
  doi: 10.1038/srep40567
– volume: 42
  start-page: 135
  year: 1991
  end-page: 159
  ident: CR39
  article-title: Sound localization by human listeners
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.001031
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1
  year: 1992
  end-page: 11
  ident: CR48
  article-title: Head turning versus manual pointing to auditory targets in normal subjects and in subjects with right parietal damage
  publication-title: Brain and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90107-w
– ident: CR8
– volume: 76
  start-page: 1815
  year: 2014
  end-page: 1827
  ident: CR12
  article-title: Reward expectation influences audiovisual spatial integration
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0699-y
– volume: 40
  start-page: 3443
  year: 2020
  end-page: 3454
  ident: CR25
  article-title: Alpha activity reflects the magnitude of an individual bias in human perception
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2359-19.2020
– volume: 14
  start-page: 257
  year: 2004
  end-page: 262
  ident: CR2
  article-title: The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029
– volume: 104
  start-page: 1586
  year: 1998
  end-page: 1597
  ident: CR34
  article-title: Auditory-visual spatial integration: A new psychophysical approach using laser pointing to acoustic targets
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.424371
– volume: 9
  start-page: e61238
  year: 2020
  ident: CR4
  article-title: Sensory experience during early sensitive periods shapes cross-modal temporal biases
  publication-title: eLife
  doi: 10.7554/eLife.61238
– volume: 258
  start-page: 89
  year: 2009
  end-page: 99
  ident: CR51
  article-title: Interactions of auditory and visual stimuli in space and time
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.04.009
– volume: 103
  start-page: 1085
  year: 1998
  end-page: 1097
  ident: CR53
  article-title: Comparison of relative and absolute sound localization ability in humans
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.421222
– volume: 307
  start-page: 86
  year: 2014
  end-page: 97
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Psychophysics and neuronal bases of sound localization in humans
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.07.008
– volume: 30
  start-page: 1141
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1150
  ident: CR46
  article-title: Visual recalibration of auditory spatial perception: Two separate neural circuits for perceptual learning
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06910.x
– volume: 30
  start-page: 7714
  year: 2010
  end-page: 7721
  ident: CR15
  article-title: Visual-haptic adaptation is determined by relative reliability
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6427-09.2010
– volume: 162
  start-page: 257
  year: 2017
  end-page: 268
  ident: CR67
  article-title: The role of auditory cortex in the spatial ventriloquism aftereffect
  publication-title: NeuroImage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.002
– volume: 8
  start-page: 16399
  year: 2018
  ident: CR20
  article-title: Accuracy-precision trade-off in human sound localization
  publication-title: Scientific Reports
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34512-6
– volume: 8
  start-page: 6690
  year: 2018
  ident: CR49
  article-title: The effect of attention and working memory on the estimation of elapsed time
  publication-title: Scientific Reports
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25119-y
– volume: 108
  start-page: 105
  year: 2000
  end-page: 125
  ident: CR36
  article-title: Sound localization with eccentric head position
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
  doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00141-2
– ident: CR9
– volume: 38
  start-page: 1517
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1529
  ident: CR58
  article-title: Individual differences in the multisensory temporal binding window predict susceptibility to audiovisual illusions
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0027339
– volume: 59
  start-page: 119
  year: 2008
  end-page: 142
  ident: CR52
  article-title: The biological basis of audition
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093544
– volume: 197
  start-page: 104170
  year: 2020
  ident: CR5
  article-title: Modality-specific attention attenuates visual-tactile integration and recalibration effects by reducing prior expectations of a common source for vision and touch
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104170
– volume: 60
  start-page: 88
  year: 2006
  end-page: 94
  ident: CR19
  article-title: Tactile coactivation resets age-related decline of human tactile discrimination
  publication-title: Annals of Neurology
  doi: 10.1002/ana.20862
– ident: CR64
– volume: 31
  start-page: 13949
  year: 2011
  end-page: 13962
  ident: CR66
  article-title: Multisensory calibration is independent of cue reliability
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2732-11.2011
– volume: 34
  start-page: 743
  year: 2021
  end-page: 761
  ident: CR54
  article-title: The effects of cue reliability on crossmodal recalibration in adults and children
  publication-title: Multisensory Research
  doi: 10.1163/22134808-bja10053
– volume: 4
  start-page: 341
  year: 1897
  end-page: 360
  ident: CR59
  article-title: Vision without inversion of the retinal image
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/h0075482
– volume: 12
  start-page: 153
  year: 2001
  end-page: 159
  ident: CR35
  article-title: Spatial coordinates of human auditory working memory
  publication-title: Cognitive Brain Research
  doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(01)00042-8
– volume: 27
  start-page: 583
  year: 2016
  end-page: 591
  ident: CR42
  article-title: The brain’s tendency to bind audiovisual signals is stable but not general
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797616628860
– volume: 6
  start-page: e18344
  year: 2011
  ident: CR60
  article-title: Effect of audiovisual training on monaural spatial hearing in horizontal plane
  publication-title: PloS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018344
– ident: CR14
– volume: 13
  start-page: 74
  year: 2020
  ident: CR31
  article-title: Feedback modulates audio-visual spatial recalibration
  publication-title: Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00074
– year: 2019
  ident: CR57
  publication-title: Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis
– volume: 87
  start-page: 2188
  year: 1990
  end-page: 2200
  ident: CR37
  article-title: Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.399186
– volume: 75
  start-page: 790
  year: 2013
  end-page: 811
  ident: CR18
  article-title: Internsensory binding across space and time: A tutorial review
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0475-4
– ident: CR10
– volume: 417
  start-page: 322
  year: 2002
  end-page: 328
  ident: CR30
  article-title: Instructed learning in the auditory localization pathway of the barn owl
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/417322a
– volume: 51
  start-page: 1179
  year: 1951
  end-page: 1203
  ident: CR17
  article-title: Theory and methods for analyzing errors in man-machine systems
  publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
  doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1951.tb27345.x
– volume: 20
  start-page: 1597
  year: 2000
  end-page: 1604
  ident: CR24
  article-title: Tactile coactivation-induced changes in spatial discrimination performance
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01597.2000
– volume: 9
  start-page: 268
  year: 2002
  end-page: 278
  ident: CR32
  article-title: Rapid adaptation to auditory-visual spatial disparity
  publication-title: Learning & Memory
  doi: 10.1101/lm.51402
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1149
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1160
  ident: CR21
  article-title: Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1215
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1222
  ident: CR33
  article-title: More accurate sound localization induced by short-term light deprivation
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.006
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1120
  year: 2011
  end-page: 1126
  ident: CR11
  article-title: Cross-modal training induces changes in spatial representations early in the auditory processing pathway
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797611416254
– volume: 364
  start-page: 331
  year: 2009
  end-page: 339
  ident: CR29
  article-title: Visual influences on auditory spatial learning
  publication-title: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0230
– volume: 15
  start-page: 20
  year: 2003
  end-page: 29
  ident: CR27
  article-title: Visual localization ability influences cross-modal bias
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/089892903321107792
– volume: 53
  start-page: 3637
  year: 2021
  end-page: 3639
  ident: CR63
  article-title: Comment on “Differential effects of the temporal and spatial distribution of audiovisual stimuli on cross-modal spatial recalibration”
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/ejn.15001
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1068
  year: 2021
  end-page: 1079
  ident: CR45
  article-title: The neurophysiological basis of the trial-wise and cumulative ventriloquism aftereffects
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2091-20.2020
– ident: CR13
– volume: 25
  start-page: 1
  year: 2012
  end-page: 14
  ident: CR22
  article-title: The aftereffects of ventriloquism: The time course of the visual recalibration of auditory localization
  publication-title: Seeing and Perceiving
  doi: 10.1163/187847611X620883
– volume: 82
  start-page: 1637
  year: 1987
  end-page: 1645
  ident: CR47
  article-title: Changes in head position as a measure of auditory localization performance: Auditory psychomotor coordination under monaural and binaural listening conditions
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.395155
– volume: 28
  start-page: 670
  year: 2017
  end-page: 678
  ident: CR26
  article-title: Time order as psychological bias
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797616689369
– volume: 72
  start-page: 210
  year: 2010
  end-page: 217
  ident: CR41
  article-title: Auditory space perception in left- and right-handers
  publication-title: Brain and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.013
– year: 1997
  ident: CR6
  publication-title: Spatial hearing: The psychophysics of human sound localization
– ident: CR7
– volume: 95
  start-page: 869
  year: 1998
  end-page: 875
  ident: CR50
  article-title: Rapidly induced auditory plasticity: The ventriloquism aftereffect
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.869
– volume: 24
  start-page: 1463
  year: 2004
  end-page: 1471
  ident: CR62
  article-title: Contribution of head shadow and pinna cues to chronic monaural sound localization
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0048-04.2004
– volume: 54
  start-page: 508
  year: 2022
  end-page: 521
  ident: CR3
  article-title: Central tendency biases must be accounted for to consistently capture Bayesian cue combination in continuous response data
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01633-2
– volume: 119
  start-page: 74
  year: 2019
  end-page: 88
  ident: CR38
  article-title: Integration of audiovisual spatial signals is not consistent with maximum likelihood estimation
  publication-title: Cortex
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.03.026
– volume: 85
  start-page: 868
  year: 1989
  end-page: 878
  ident: CR65
  article-title: Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: Psychophysical validation
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.397558
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1149
  year: 2009
  ident: 2416_CR21
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
– volume: 31
  start-page: 13949
  year: 2011
  ident: 2416_CR66
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2732-11.2011
– volume: 30
  start-page: 1726
  year: 2020
  ident: 2416_CR55
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.02.048
– volume: 72
  start-page: 210
  year: 2010
  ident: 2416_CR41
  publication-title: Brain and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.013
– volume: 13
  start-page: 581
  year: 1984
  ident: 2416_CR44
  publication-title: Perception
  doi: 10.1068/p130581
– volume: 364
  start-page: 331
  year: 2009
  ident: 2416_CR29
  publication-title: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0230
– volume: 258
  start-page: 89
  year: 2009
  ident: 2416_CR51
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.04.009
– volume: 6
  start-page: e18344
  year: 2011
  ident: 2416_CR60
  publication-title: PloS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018344
– volume: 108
  start-page: 105
  year: 2000
  ident: 2416_CR36
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
  doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00141-2
– volume: 22
  start-page: 1120
  year: 2011
  ident: 2416_CR11
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797611416254
– volume: 24
  start-page: 1463
  year: 2004
  ident: 2416_CR62
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0048-04.2004
– volume: 30
  start-page: 7714
  year: 2010
  ident: 2416_CR15
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6427-09.2010
– volume: 15
  start-page: 20
  year: 2003
  ident: 2416_CR27
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/089892903321107792
– volume: 114
  start-page: 179
  year: 1997
  ident: 2416_CR16
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(97)00161-5
– volume: 75
  start-page: 790
  year: 2013
  ident: 2416_CR18
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0475-4
– volume: 28
  start-page: 670
  year: 2017
  ident: 2416_CR26
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797616689369
– volume: 82
  start-page: 3490
  year: 2020
  ident: 2416_CR61
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-020-02083-2
– volume: 7
  start-page: 40567
  year: 2017
  ident: 2416_CR23
  publication-title: Scientific Reports
  doi: 10.1038/srep40567
– volume: 238
  start-page: 94
  year: 2008
  ident: 2416_CR40
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2007.11.006
– volume: 85
  start-page: 868
  year: 1989
  ident: 2416_CR65
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.397558
– volume: 27
  start-page: 180
  year: 2016
  ident: 2416_CR56
  publication-title: NeuroReport
  doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000521
– volume-title: Spatial hearing: The psychophysics of human sound localization
  year: 1997
  ident: 2416_CR6
– volume: 307
  start-page: 86
  year: 2014
  ident: 2416_CR1
  publication-title: Hearing Research
  doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.07.008
– volume: 12
  start-page: 153
  year: 2001
  ident: 2416_CR35
  publication-title: Cognitive Brain Research
  doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(01)00042-8
– ident: 2416_CR64
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7
– volume: 417
  start-page: 322
  year: 2002
  ident: 2416_CR30
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/417322a
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1215
  year: 2007
  ident: 2416_CR33
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.006
– volume: 76
  start-page: 1815
  year: 2014
  ident: 2416_CR12
  publication-title: Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0699-y
– ident: 2416_CR13
  doi: 10.25592/uhhfdm.948
– volume: 34
  start-page: 743
  year: 2021
  ident: 2416_CR54
  publication-title: Multisensory Research
  doi: 10.1163/22134808-bja10053
– volume: 60
  start-page: 88
  year: 2006
  ident: 2416_CR19
  publication-title: Annals of Neurology
  doi: 10.1002/ana.20862
– volume: 20
  start-page: 1597
  year: 2000
  ident: 2416_CR24
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01597.2000
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1068
  year: 2021
  ident: 2416_CR45
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2091-20.2020
– volume: 9
  start-page: e61238
  year: 2020
  ident: 2416_CR4
  publication-title: eLife
  doi: 10.7554/eLife.61238
– volume: 119
  start-page: 74
  year: 2019
  ident: 2416_CR38
  publication-title: Cortex
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.03.026
– volume: 30
  start-page: 1141
  year: 2009
  ident: 2416_CR46
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06910.x
– volume: 40
  start-page: 3443
  year: 2020
  ident: 2416_CR25
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2359-19.2020
– volume: 129
  start-page: 220
  year: 2000
  ident: 2416_CR28
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.220
– volume: 14
  start-page: 257
  year: 2004
  ident: 2416_CR2
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029
– volume: 104
  start-page: 1586
  year: 1998
  ident: 2416_CR34
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.424371
– volume: 103
  start-page: 1085
  year: 1998
  ident: 2416_CR53
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.421222
– ident: 2416_CR7
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000642
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1
  year: 1992
  ident: 2416_CR48
  publication-title: Brain and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90107-w
– ident: 2416_CR8
  doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00051
– volume: 8
  start-page: 16399
  year: 2018
  ident: 2416_CR20
  publication-title: Scientific Reports
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34512-6
– volume: 53
  start-page: 3637
  year: 2021
  ident: 2416_CR63
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/ejn.15001
– volume: 13
  start-page: 74
  year: 2020
  ident: 2416_CR31
  publication-title: Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience
  doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00074
– volume: 197
  start-page: 104170
  year: 2020
  ident: 2416_CR5
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104170
– volume: 9
  start-page: 268
  year: 2002
  ident: 2416_CR32
  publication-title: Learning & Memory
  doi: 10.1101/lm.51402
– volume: 4
  start-page: 341
  year: 1897
  ident: 2416_CR59
  publication-title: Psychological Review
  doi: 10.1037/h0075482
– volume: 59
  start-page: 119
  year: 2008
  ident: 2416_CR52
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093544
– ident: 2416_CR10
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000642
– volume: 27
  start-page: 583
  year: 2016
  ident: 2416_CR42
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797616628860
– volume: 6
  start-page: 175
  year: 2003
  ident: 2416_CR68
  publication-title: Nature Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/nn999
– volume: 54
  start-page: 508
  year: 2022
  ident: 2416_CR3
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01633-2
– volume: 38
  start-page: 1517
  year: 2012
  ident: 2416_CR58
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0027339
– volume: 25
  start-page: 1
  year: 2012
  ident: 2416_CR22
  publication-title: Seeing and Perceiving
  doi: 10.1163/187847611X620883
– volume: 162
  start-page: 257
  year: 2017
  ident: 2416_CR67
  publication-title: NeuroImage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.002
– ident: 2416_CR9
  doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-10461-0_11
– volume: 11
  start-page: e1004649
  year: 2015
  ident: 2416_CR43
  publication-title: PloS Computational Biology
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004649
– volume-title: Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis
  year: 2019
  ident: 2416_CR57
– ident: 2416_CR14
  doi: 10.1111/ejn.14779
– volume: 95
  start-page: 869
  year: 1998
  ident: 2416_CR50
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.869
– volume: 51
  start-page: 1179
  year: 1951
  ident: 2416_CR17
  publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
  doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1951.tb27345.x
– volume: 42
  start-page: 135
  year: 1991
  ident: 2416_CR39
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.001031
– volume: 82
  start-page: 1637
  year: 1987
  ident: 2416_CR47
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.395155
– volume: 8
  start-page: 6690
  year: 2018
  ident: 2416_CR49
  publication-title: Scientific Reports
  doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25119-y
– volume: 87
  start-page: 2188
  year: 1990
  ident: 2416_CR37
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.399186
SSID ssj0038214
Score 2.4416883
Snippet The ability to detect the absolute location of sensory stimuli can be quantified with either error-based metrics derived from single-trial localization errors...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
springer
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 3814
SubjectTerms Accuracy
Acoustic Stimulation - methods
Adult
Auditory Perception - physiology
Auditory stimuli
Behavioral Science and Psychology
Cognitive Psychology
Cross-modal
Female
Humans
Localization
Male
Original Manuscript
Perception
Photic Stimulation
Psychology
Sensorimotor integration
Sensory integration
Sensory stimuli
Sound Localization
Space Perception - physiology
Spatial discrimination learning
Visual Perception - physiology
Visual stimuli
Young Adult
Title Quantifying accuracy and precision from continuous response data in studies of spatial perception and crossmodal recalibration
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13428-024-02416-1
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38684625
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3062683685
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3049722457
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC11133116
Volume 56
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Lb9QwEB7B9tIL70egrYzEDVLqR5z4uFr1oSKQkFipnCzHccSqkF1tNody4Lczk1e1rYrUQ3LxezK2v4m_GQO8d86Rx6eKvSpUrFKlYyNzH5fK-CAK3BEKcnD-8lWfzdX5RXLRO4XVA9t9OJJsV2qyKxHUfqq5VBRNWSh6uI7R5tlJeGayCexMT398Ph5WYJkJrjoHmTtKbm9Ct5DlbYLkjVPSdvM5eQzzodsd5-TysNnkh_7PjYiO9x3XE3jUo1E27dTnKTwI1TPYHRfFq-fw91vjiE9E3lDMed-snb9irirYat1fz8PIRYUR531RNcumZuuOdxsY0U_ZomJ1R1Zky5LVxODGJlcjoaatrJXT72WBKVita414SnwB85Pj77OzuL-zIfaJEpu4cFwlxqsy8KMyFdoQfjO5zksp06DQXDNHJg-l8E6jTnDlZeZCInyBSEk7KV_CpFpW4TWwFC0t5fIEq8iUUIVB01IkiEAQshlRygj48BGt7wOa070avywaNiRa24nWolhtK1rLI_gwlll14Tz-m3tv0A3bT-3aoo0ldEZx-yN4NybjpKSTFlcFlLOls8sUwVGSRvCqU6WxOZlpxHwCS2dbSjZmoIDf2ynV4mcb-Bv3JSk51xF8HFTpul93D-PN_bK_hV3RaiP9atqDyWbdhH1EXpv8oJ9oB_Bwpmf4novpP2T5KRw
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Lb9QwELZQObQXBJRC2gJG4tZG1PbEiY8VolqgrYTUlXqzHMcRK7XZ1Wb30Au_vTPOA22rInHIJX4kzvjxffE3Y8Y-O-fI4xNSDxWkkINOjSp9WoPxQVa4IlTk4HxxqSdT-HGdXfdOYe2gdh-2JONMTbwSQe2XViigaMoS6BI6Rc7zHMFAQecWTOXpMP_iDQGde8wT5TaXoEe48rE88sEeaVx6zl6yFz1m5KedkV-xZ6F5zXbGqetul_35tXak-iGfJe68Xy-dv-Ouqfhi2R-iw8mRhJMyfdaske7zZaeODZxEonzW8LaTFPJ5zVvSWeMjF6PsJVYW23M7rzAFq3WRalPiGzY9-3b1dZL2JyukPgO5SisnIDMe6iBO6lxqQyjLlLqslcoDIKkyJ6YMtfROo-UEeFW4kElfIZ7RTqk9ttXMm_CO8Rz5ELgywyoKkFAZJIAyQ5yAwMrIWiVMDB_b-j7sOJ1-cWORfpCBbGcgi8ax0UBWJOxoLLPogm78M_fhYEPbD8DWIhOSuqDo-gn7NCbj0KH9ENcE_M6WdhhzhDBZnrC3ncnHx6lCIzKTWLrY6AxjBgrLvZnSzH7H8Ny4eiglhE7Y8dBv_r7X083Y_7_sH9n25Ori3J5_v_x5wHZk7N_0c-iQba2W6_AesdKq_BCHxj2xJA02
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Lb9QwELZQkVAviDeBAkbiBlFre-LER1RYlVcFEpV6sxzbESuBd7WPQy_8dmacByxVkTjkNLYTezyebzIPM_bCOUcZn1B6CFBCDbo0qvVlB8ZHGVAjBEpw_nSqT87g_Xl1_kcWf452H12SfU4DVWlKm8Nl6EjEFQLcw7VQQJWVJdAjdIn2z3Ug1UfuWn08nsWqkQL6VJkr-u2qo0sY83Ko5F_-0qyGZrfYzQE_8tc9w2-zazHdYfvTMXZxl_38snUUAUT5S9x5v105f8FdCny5Gi7U4ZRUwmnK87RF05-v-kjZyClglM8TX_fhhXzR8TXFXOMrl1MITB4sz-fHIiAFh3XZ7CbiPXY2e_v1-KQcblkofQVyUwYnoDIeuiiOulpqQ4jLtLrtlKojoIFljkwbO-mdRi4K8KpxsZI-ILbRTqn7bC8tUnzIeI22Ebi2wiEakBAMGoOyQsyAIMvIThVMjItt_VCCnG7C-G7RFCEG2Z5BFpljM4OsKNjLqc-yL8Dxz9YHIw_tIIxri1aR1A1V2i_Y84mMYkS-EZcirrMlb2ONcKaqC_agZ_n0OtVoRGkSezc7m2FqQCW6dylp_i2X6kZNopQQumCvxn3z-7uunsaj_2v-jN34_GZmP747_fCY7cu8vek_0QHb26y28QnCpk37NEvGL6frEVw
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Quantifying+accuracy+and+precision+from+continuous+response+data+in+studies+of+spatial+perception+and+crossmodal+recalibration&rft.jtitle=Behavior+research+methods&rft.au=Bruns%2C+Patrick&rft.au=Thun%2C+Caroline&rft.au=R%C3%B6der+Brigitte&rft.date=2024-06-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.eissn=1554-3528&rft.volume=56&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=3814&rft.epage=3830&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13428-024-02416-1&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon