Consistency of noncognitive skills and their relation to educational outcomes in a UK cohort
Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range...
Saved in:
Published in | Translational psychiatry Vol. 11; no. 1; pp. 563 - 10 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
London
Nature Publishing Group UK
05.11.2021
Nature Publishing Group |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 2158-3188 2158-3188 |
DOI | 10.1038/s41398-021-01661-8 |
Cover
Abstract | Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability (
h
S
N
P
2
= 0.1–0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Abstract Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability ( $$h_{SNP}^2$$ h S N P 2 = 0.1–0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability ( \documentclass[12pt]{minimal} \usepackage{amsmath} \usepackage{wasysym} \usepackage{amsfonts} \usepackage{amssymb} \usepackage{amsbsy} \usepackage{mathrsfs} \usepackage{upgreek} \setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt} \begin{document}$$h_{SNP}^2$$\end{document} h S N P 2 = 0.1–0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability (hSNP2 = 0.1–0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability ([Formula: see text] = 0.1-0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures.Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability ([Formula: see text] = 0.1-0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability ( h S N P 2 = 0.1–0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability ([Formula: see text] = 0.1-0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have been unable to assess the longitudinal consistency of noncognitive skill measures. Using data from a UK birth cohort, we investigated a range of noncognitive skills: behavioural problems, social skills, communication, self-esteem, persistence, locus of control, empathy, impulsivity and personality. We assessed their consistency over a 17-year period throughout childhood and adolescence (age 6 months to 18 years), their genomic architecture, and their associations with socioeconomic outcomes. We found high longitudinal measurement consistency for behavioural and communication skills, but low consistency for other noncognitive skills, suggesting a high noise to signal ratio. We observed consistent non-zero heritability estimates and genetic correlations for only behavioural difficulties. Using aggregate measures of each skill over time, we found evidence of phenotypic correlations and heritability ( $$h_{SNP}^2$$ h S N P 2 = 0.1–0.2) for behaviour, communication, self-esteem and locus of control. Associations between noncognitive skills and educational outcomes were observed for skills measured in mid to late childhood but these were at most a third of the size of IQ-education associations. These results suggest that measures designed to capture noncognitive skills may be subject to considerable response heterogeneity or measurement error. Aggregate measures that leverage repeat responses from longitudinal data may offer researchers more reliable measures that better identify underlying noncognitive skills than cross sectional measures. |
ArticleNumber | 563 |
Author | Davies, Neil M. Morris, Tim T. van den Berg, Gerard Davey Smith, George |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Tim T. orcidid: 0000-0001-8178-6815 surname: Morris fullname: Morris, Tim T. email: Tim.Morris@bristol.ac.uk organization: MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol – sequence: 2 givenname: George orcidid: 0000-0002-1407-8314 surname: Davey Smith fullname: Davey Smith, George organization: MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol – sequence: 3 givenname: Gerard surname: van den Berg fullname: van den Berg, Gerard organization: Department of Economics, University of Bristol, Department of Economics, University of Groningen, Department of Epidemiology, University Medical Center Groningen – sequence: 4 givenname: Neil M. orcidid: 0000-0002-2460-0508 surname: Davies fullname: Davies, Neil M. organization: MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34741011$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9Ustu1DAUjVARLaU_wAJZYsMm4OtH4myQ0IhHRSU2dIdkOc7NjIeMXWynUv8ez6SFtot649c5x-f6npfVkQ8eq-o10PdAufqQBPBO1ZRBTaFpoFbPqhMGUtUclDq6tz6uzlLa0jKkUNDCi-qYi1YABTipfq2CTy5l9PaGhJGUV2xYe5fdNZL0201TIsYPJG_QRRJxMtkFT3IgOMz2sDETCXO2YYeJOE8MufxObNiEmF9Vz0czJTy7nU-ryy-ff66-1Rc_vp6vPl3UVgqa69GMRmGpiglZTHI2WkFHqQwHOVBFTW_kINsOeuQDMjvSoVOCNQ2VvWhNz0-r80V3CGarr6LbmXijg3H6cBDiWpuYnZ1Qd8xwCwCyUIUwvGdCYde3I1iFjVJF6-OidTX3Oxws-hzN9ED04Y13G70O11rJFljTFoF3twIx_JkxZb1zyeI0GY9hTprJTrBOlaYV6NtH0G2YY_nQA4q3QgqxF3xz39E_K3dNLAC1AGwMKUUctXX50Jpi0E0aqN5HRi-R0SUy-hAZva-WPaLeqT9J4gspFbBfY_xv-wnWXzb40tA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1086_733487 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pnpbp_2024_110986 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ehb_2023_101272 crossref_primary_10_5926_arepj_63_227 |
Cites_doi | 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02034.x 10.1086/504455 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x 10.1038/s41588-018-0178-9 10.1037/pspp0000098 10.1126/science.aan6877 10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x 10.1037/a0037091 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3 10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017 10.1016/j.labeco.2012.05.014 10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262 10.2139/ssrn.1858066 10.1038/s41562-018-0461-x 10.1111/ecoj.12069 10.1002/icd.581 10.1038/s41398-018-0269-5 10.2307/2346101 10.1080/00223890802634290 10.2307/3090251 10.1111/jopy.12230 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x 10.1007/s10519-006-9060-0 10.1016/j.ajhg.2012.10.010 10.1177/1745691611421204 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 10.1596/1813-9450-8000 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.006 10.1007/s00439-014-1514-5 10.1007/s10519-018-9887-1 10.1023/A:1012294324713 10.1787/19939019 10.1111/jopy.12186 10.1093/ije/dys066 10.1177/0956797615612727 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.029 10.1177/0956797619831612 10.1375/twin.11.3.275 10.1037/pspp0000224 10.1093/ije/dyy060 10.1093/ije/dys064 10.1101/2021.02.15.21251308 10.1038/mp.2014.105 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.012 10.1038/s41467-018-08219-1 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.013 10.1192/bjp.175.6.559 10.1017/S0033291707000840 10.1080/21582041.2016.1138502 10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author(s) 2021 2021. The Author(s). The Author(s) 2021. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author(s) 2021 – notice: 2021. The Author(s). – notice: The Author(s) 2021. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
DBID | C6C AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 3V. 7X7 7XB 88E 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABUWG AFKRA AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO FYUFA GHDGH K9. M0S M1P PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PJZUB PKEHL PPXIY PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.1038/s41398-021-01661-8 |
DatabaseName | Springer Nature OA Free Journals CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed ProQuest Central (Corporate) Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) Hospital Premium Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland ProQuest Central Essentials Local Electronic Collection Information ProQuest Central ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Korea Health Research Premium Collection (UHCL Subscription) Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Health & Medical Collection (Alumni) Proquest Medical Database ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection Health Research Premium Collection Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest Medical Library ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | Publicly Available Content Database MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: C6C name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals url: http://www.springeropen.com/ sourceTypes: Publisher – sequence: 2 dbid: DOA name: Open Access: DOAJ - Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 3 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 4 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 5 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: http://www.proquest.com/pqcentral?accountid=15518 sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine |
EISSN | 2158-3188 |
EndPage | 10 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_92a3c11155b444a3b248e9b7f1c8e688 PMC8571267 34741011 10_1038_s41398_021_01661_8 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GeographicLocations | United Kingdom United Kingdom--UK |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: United Kingdom – name: United Kingdom--UK |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: RCUK | Medical Research Council (MRC) grantid: MC_UU_00011/1; MC_UU_12013/1; MC_UU_12013/9; MC_UU_00011/1; MC_UU_12013/1; MC_UU_12013/9; MC_UU_12011/1 funderid: https://doi.org/10.13039/501100000265 – fundername: RCUK | Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) grantid: ES/S011021/1; ES/N000757/1 funderid: https://doi.org/10.13039/501100000269 – fundername: Wellcome Trust – fundername: Versus Arthritis grantid: 17702 – fundername: Medical Research Council grantid: MC_PC_21000 – fundername: Medical Research Council grantid: MC_U147585824 – fundername: Medical Research Council grantid: MC_PC_21001 – fundername: Medical Research Council grantid: MC_U147585819 – fundername: Medical Research Council grantid: MC_U147585827 – fundername: Medical Research Council grantid: MR/J000094/1 – fundername: RCUK | Medical Research Council (MRC) grantid: MC_UU_12013/1 – fundername: Medical Research Council grantid: G0400491 – fundername: ; grantid: ES/S011021/1; ES/N000757/1 – fundername: ; grantid: MC_UU_00011/1; MC_UU_12013/1; MC_UU_12013/9; MC_UU_00011/1; MC_UU_12013/1; MC_UU_12013/9; MC_UU_12011/1 |
GroupedDBID | --- 0R~ 3V. 53G 5VS 7X7 88E 8FI 8FJ AAJSJ AAKDD ABUWG ACGFO ACGFS ACMJI ACSMW ADBBV ADFRT AENEX AFKRA AHMBA AJTQC ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMTXH AOIJS BAWUL BCNDV BENPR BPHCQ BVXVI C6C CCPQU DIK EBLON EBS EMOBN FYUFA GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HMCUK HYE KQ8 LGEZI LOTEE M1P M~E NADUK NAO NXXTH OK1 PIMPY PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO RNS RNT RNTTT RPM SNYQT UKHRP AASML AAYXX CITATION PHGZM PHGZT CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7XB 8FK AARCD AZQEC DWQXO K9. PJZUB PKEHL PPXIY PQEST PQUKI PRINS 7X8 PUEGO 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c540t-fafa8e10324500532fc40f58a315d080aba5d5791be3de2cf0d98426605b47ab3 |
IEDL.DBID | AAJSJ |
ISSN | 2158-3188 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:27:48 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 14:05:30 EDT 2025 Thu Sep 04 19:04:27 EDT 2025 Wed Aug 13 06:22:57 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:03:18 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:55:18 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:03:51 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 21 02:38:51 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Language | English |
License | 2021. The Author(s). Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c540t-fafa8e10324500532fc40f58a315d080aba5d5791be3de2cf0d98426605b47ab3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-1407-8314 0000-0001-8178-6815 0000-0002-2460-0508 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.nature.com/articles/s41398-021-01661-8 |
PMID | 34741011 |
PQID | 2593745447 |
PQPubID | 2041978 |
PageCount | 10 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_92a3c11155b444a3b248e9b7f1c8e688 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_8571267 proquest_miscellaneous_2594298016 proquest_journals_2593745447 pubmed_primary_34741011 crossref_citationtrail_10_1038_s41398_021_01661_8 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41398_021_01661_8 springer_journals_10_1038_s41398_021_01661_8 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2021-11-05 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2021-11-05 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 11 year: 2021 text: 2021-11-05 day: 05 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | London |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: London – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Translational psychiatry |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Transl Psychiatry |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Transl Psychiatry |
PublicationYear | 2021 |
Publisher | Nature Publishing Group UK Nature Publishing Group |
Publisher_xml | – name: Nature Publishing Group UK – name: Nature Publishing Group |
References | SotoCJHow replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life outcomes? The life outcomes of personality replication projectPsychol Sci2019307112710.1177/0956797619831612 BorghansLGolsteynBHHHeckmanJHumphriesJEIdentification problems in personality psychologyPers Individ Dif2011513152010.1016/j.paid.2011.03.029217311703126096 St PourcainBHaworthCMDavisOSWangKTimpsonNJEvansDMHeritability and genome-wide analyses of problematic peer relationships during childhood and adolescenceHum Genet20151345395110.1007/s00439-014-1514-5 GoodmanALampingDLPloubidisGBWhen to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ): data from british parents, teachers and childrenJ Abnorm Child Psychol20103811799110.1007/s10802-010-9434-x BowlesSGintisHSchooling in capitalist America revisitedSociol Educ197675110.2307/3090251 Gutman LM, Schoon I. The impact of non-cognitive skills on outcomes for young people: literature review. Educ Endow Found London Engl. pp. 58 2013. https://educationendowmentfoundation.org.uk/education-evidence/evidence-reviews/essential-life-skills. KongAThorleifssonGFriggeMLVilhjalmssonBJYoungAIThorgeirssonTEThe nature of nurture: effects of parental genotypesScience (80-)201835942481:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXhsVGhsrg%3D10.1126/science.aan6877 KovasYGaron-CarrierGBoivinMPetrillSAPlominRMalykhSBWhy children differ in motivation to learn: Insights from over 13,000 twins from 6 countriesPers Individ Dif201580516310.1016/j.paid.2015.02.006260521744372262 McGueMRustichiniAIaconoWGCognitive, noncognitive, and family background contributions to college attainment: a behavioral genetic perspectiveJ Pers201585657810.1111/jopy.12230 Tucker-DrobEMBrileyDAEngelhardtLEMannFDHardenKPGenetically-mediated associations between measures of childhood character and academic achievementJ Pers Soc Psychol201611179081510.1037/pspp0000098 YoungAIFriggeMLGudbjartssonDFThorleifssonGBjornsdottirGSulemPRelatedness disequilibrium regression estimates heritability without environmental biasNat Genet2018501304101:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXhsFSqtbfO10.1038/s41588-018-0178-9301047646130754 SpeedDHemaniGJohnsonMRBaldingDJImproved heritability estimation from genome-wide SNPsAm J Hum Genet2012911011211:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XhvVWis7bP10.1016/j.ajhg.2012.10.010 WillemsYEDolanCVvan BeijsterveldtCde ZeeuwELBoomsmaDIBartelsMGenetic and environmental influences on self-control: assessing self-control with the ASEBA self-control scaleBehav Genet2018481354610.1007/s10519-018-9887-1 HeckmanJJStixrudJUrzuaSThe effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behaviorJ Labor Econ2006244118210.1086/504455 Duckworth AL, Quinn PD. Development and validation of the short Grit Scale (Grit-S). J Pers Assess. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890802634290. BenjaminiYHochbergYControlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testingJ R Stat Soc Ser B19955728930010.2307/2346101 BlandenJGreggPMacmillanLAccounting for intergenerational income persistence: noncognitive skills, ability and educationEcon J2007117C436010.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02034.x Gidziela A, Rimfeld K, Malanchini M, Allegrini AG, McMillan A, Selzam S, et al. Using DNA to predict behaviour problems from preschool to adulthood. medRxiv 2021. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.15.21251308. Kautz T, Heckman J, Diris R, ter Weel B, Borghans L. Fostering and measuring skills: improving cognitive and non-cognitive skills to promote lifetime success. 2014 https://doi.org/10.1787/19939019. WillemsYEBoesenNLiJFinkenauerCBartelsMThe heritability of self-control: a meta-analysisNeurosci Biobehav Rev2019100324341:STN:280:DC%2BB3cfpt1yitg%3D%3D10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.012 BrileyDATucker-DrobEMComparing the developmental genetics of cognition and personality over the life spanJ Pers201785516410.1111/jopy.12186 CheesmanRMajor Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric GenomicsCPurvesKLPingaultJBBreenGRijsdij KFExtracting stability increases the SNP heritability of emotional problems in young peopleTransl Psychiatry2018810.1038/s41398-018-0269-5 ReddenDTAllisonDBThe effect of assortative mating upon genetic association studies: Spurious associations and population substructure in the absence of admixtureBehav Genet2006366788610.1007/s10519-006-9060-016514473 Von StummSHellBChamorro-PremuzicTThe hungry mind: Intellectual curiosity is the third pillar of academic performancePerspect Psychol Sci201165748810.1177/1745691611421204 RaevuoriADickDMKeski-RahkonenAPulkkinenLRoseRJRissanenAGenetic and environmental factors affecting self-esteem from age 14 to 17: a longitudinal study of Finnish twinsPsychol Med20073716253310.1017/S0033291707000840 MalanchiniMEngelhardtLEGrotzingerADHardenKPTucker-DrobEM‘Same but different’: associations between multiple aspects of self-regulation, cognition, and academic abilitiesJ Pers Soc Psychol201911711648810.1037/pspp0000224 RobertsBWDelVecchioWFThe rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studiesPsychol Bull20001263251:STN:280:DC%2BD3c7jtlGhtQ%3D%3D10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.310668348 DuncanGJDowsettCJClaessensAMagnusonKHustonACKlebanovPSchool readiness and later achievementDev Psychol20074314284610.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 Carneiro P, Crawford C, Goodman A. The impact of early cognitive and non-cognitive skills on later outcomes. Center for the Economics of Education Discussion Paper 92. 2007. WainwrightMAWrightMJLucianoMGeffenGMMartinNGGenetic covariation among facets of openness to experience and general cognitive abilityTwin Res Hum Genet2008112758610.1375/twin.11.3.27518498206 Cobb-ClarkDASchurerSTwo economists’ musings on the stability of locus of controlEcon J2013123F35840010.1111/ecoj.12069 HaworthSMitchellRCorbinLWadeKHDuddingTBudu-AggreyAApparent latent structure within the UK Biobank sample has implications for epidemiological analysisNat Commun20191010.1038/s41467-018-08219-1 SchmidtFLHunterJEThe validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findingsPsychol Bull19981242627410.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262 SmithersLGSawyerACPChittleboroughCRDaviesNMDavey SmithGLynchJWA systematic review and meta-analysis of effects of early life non-cognitive skills on academic, psychosocial, cognitive and health outcomesNat Hum Behav201828678010.1038/s41562-018-0461-x O’ConnorMCPaunonenSVBig Five personality predictors of post-secondary academic performancePers Individ Dif2007439719010.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017 Laajaj R, Macours K. Measuring Skills in Developing Countries. Policy Res Work Paper. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1596/1813-9450-8000. Lemery-ChalfantKDoelgerLGoldsmithHHGenetic relations between effortful and attentional control and symptoms of psychopathology in middle childhoodInfant Child Dev Int J Res Pr2008173658510.1002/icd.581 FraserAMacdonald-WallisCTillingKBoydAGoldingJDavey SmithGCohort profile: The avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohortInt J Epidemiol2013429711010.1093/ije/dys066 GoodmanRThe Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research noteJ child Psychol psychiatry19973858161:STN:280:DyaK2szpvFKjsA%3D%3D10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x HeckmanJJKautzTHard evidence on soft skillsLabour Econ2012194516410.1016/j.labeco.2012.05.014235596943612993 Brunello G, Schlotter M. Non Cognitive Skills and Personality Traits: Labour Market Relevance and their Development in Education & Training Systems. IZA Discussion Paper No. 5743. 2011. BarrickMRMountMKThe big five personality dimensions and job performance: a meta-analysisPers Psychol19914412610.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x PlominRDearyIJGenetics and intelligence differences: five special findingsMol Psychiatry201520981081:STN:280:DC%2BC2M7hsFKlsg%3D%3D10.1038/mp.2014.10525224258 MorrisTTDorlingDDavey SmithGHow well can we predict educational outcomes? Examining the roles of cognitive ability and social position in educational attainmentContemp Soc Sci20161111510.1080/21582041.2016.1138502 DohmenTFalkAHuffmanDSundeUSchuppJWagnerGGIndividual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequencesJ Eur Econ Assoc201195225010.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x YangJLeeSHGoddardMEVisscherPMGCTA: A tool for genome-wide complex trait analysisAm J Hum Genet20118876821:CAS:528:DC%2BC3MXktVejtg%3D%3D10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011 Fleche S. Teacher Quality, Test Scores and Non-Cognitive Skills: Evidence from Primary School Teachers in the UK. 2017. http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/83602/. LucianoMWainwrightMAWrightMJMartinNGThe heritability of conscientiousness facets and their relationship to IQ and academic achievementPers Individ Dif20064011899910.1016/j.paid.2005.10.013 BoydAGoldingJMacleodJLawlorDAFraserAHendersonJCohort Profile: the’children of the 90s’–the index offspring of the avon longitudinal study of parents and childrenInt J Epidemiol2013421112710.1093/ije/dys064 TaylorAEJonesHJSallisHEuesdenJStergiakouliEDaviesNMExploring the association of genetic factors with participation in the avon longitudinal study of parents and childrenInt J Epidemiol20184712071610.1093/ije/dyy060 ScourfieldJMartinNLewisGMcGuffinPHeritability of social cognitive skills in children and adolescentsBr J Psychiatry1999175559641:STN:280:DC%2BD3c3kvFygsg%3D%3D10.1192/bjp.175.6.559 Tucker-DrobEMHardenKPA behavioral genetic perspective on noncognitive factors and academic achievementPsychological Sci2015271384910.1177/0956797615612727 BouchardTJLoehlinJCGenes, evolution, and personalityBehav Genet2001312437310.1023/A:101229432471311699599 BrileyDATucker-DrobEMGenetic and environmental continuity in personality development: a meta-analysisPsychol Bull201414013033110.1037/a0037091249561224152379 BorghansLDuckworthALHeckmanJJTer WeelBThe economics and psychology of personality traitsJ Hum Resour2008439721059. YE Willems (1661_CR26) 2018; 48 MC O’Connor (1661_CR14) 2007; 43 JJ Heckman (1661_CR4) 2012; 19 CJ Soto (1661_CR19) 2019; 30 1661_CR23 J Scourfield (1661_CR49) 1999; 175 A Boyd (1661_CR33) 2013; 42 YE Willems (1661_CR50) 2019; 100 L Borghans (1661_CR2) 2008; 43 L Borghans (1661_CR13) 2011; 51 M McGue (1661_CR9) 2015; 85 Y Benjamini (1661_CR40) 1995; 57 R Cheesman (1661_CR47) 2018; 8 DA Cobb-Clark (1661_CR21) 2013; 123 GJ Duncan (1661_CR5) 2007; 43 1661_CR8 1661_CR6 Y Kovas (1661_CR30) 2015; 80 MA Wainwright (1661_CR28) 2008; 11 1661_CR10 1661_CR1 1661_CR12 1661_CR11 M Malanchini (1661_CR18) 2019; 117 TT Morris (1661_CR39) 2016; 11 EM Tucker-Drob (1661_CR17) 2016; 111 M Luciano (1661_CR29) 2006; 40 A Goodman (1661_CR36) 2010; 38 D Speed (1661_CR53) 2012; 91 S Von Stumm (1661_CR16) 2011; 6 DT Redden (1661_CR55) 2006; 36 DA Briley (1661_CR51) 2017; 85 LG Smithers (1661_CR20) 2018; 2 K Lemery-Chalfant (1661_CR27) 2008; 17 1661_CR42 R Plomin (1661_CR44) 2015; 20 J Yang (1661_CR41) 2011; 88 AI Young (1661_CR52) 2018; 50 TJ Bouchard (1661_CR25) 2001; 31 R Goodman (1661_CR35) 1997; 38 A Kong (1661_CR43) 2018; 359 FL Schmidt (1661_CR15) 1998; 124 BW Roberts (1661_CR22) 2000; 126 AE Taylor (1661_CR45) 2018; 47 S Bowles (1661_CR3) 1976; 75 MR Barrick (1661_CR38) 1991; 44 T Dohmen (1661_CR24) 2011; 9 S Haworth (1661_CR54) 2019; 10 DA Briley (1661_CR31) 2014; 140 JJ Heckman (1661_CR7) 2006; 24 B St Pourcain (1661_CR46) 2015; 134 A Raevuori (1661_CR48) 2007; 37 J Blanden (1661_CR37) 2007; 117 A Fraser (1661_CR34) 2013; 42 EM Tucker-Drob (1661_CR32) 2015; 27 |
References_xml | – reference: WainwrightMAWrightMJLucianoMGeffenGMMartinNGGenetic covariation among facets of openness to experience and general cognitive abilityTwin Res Hum Genet2008112758610.1375/twin.11.3.27518498206 – reference: SpeedDHemaniGJohnsonMRBaldingDJImproved heritability estimation from genome-wide SNPsAm J Hum Genet2012911011211:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XhvVWis7bP10.1016/j.ajhg.2012.10.010 – reference: BowlesSGintisHSchooling in capitalist America revisitedSociol Educ197675110.2307/3090251 – reference: O’ConnorMCPaunonenSVBig Five personality predictors of post-secondary academic performancePers Individ Dif2007439719010.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017 – reference: ScourfieldJMartinNLewisGMcGuffinPHeritability of social cognitive skills in children and adolescentsBr J Psychiatry1999175559641:STN:280:DC%2BD3c3kvFygsg%3D%3D10.1192/bjp.175.6.559 – reference: BrileyDATucker-DrobEMComparing the developmental genetics of cognition and personality over the life spanJ Pers201785516410.1111/jopy.12186 – reference: BrileyDATucker-DrobEMGenetic and environmental continuity in personality development: a meta-analysisPsychol Bull201414013033110.1037/a0037091249561224152379 – reference: RobertsBWDelVecchioWFThe rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studiesPsychol Bull20001263251:STN:280:DC%2BD3c7jtlGhtQ%3D%3D10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.310668348 – reference: YangJLeeSHGoddardMEVisscherPMGCTA: A tool for genome-wide complex trait analysisAm J Hum Genet20118876821:CAS:528:DC%2BC3MXktVejtg%3D%3D10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011 – reference: DuncanGJDowsettCJClaessensAMagnusonKHustonACKlebanovPSchool readiness and later achievementDev Psychol20074314284610.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 – reference: Von StummSHellBChamorro-PremuzicTThe hungry mind: Intellectual curiosity is the third pillar of academic performancePerspect Psychol Sci201165748810.1177/1745691611421204 – reference: SotoCJHow replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life outcomes? The life outcomes of personality replication projectPsychol Sci2019307112710.1177/0956797619831612 – reference: CheesmanRMajor Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric GenomicsCPurvesKLPingaultJBBreenGRijsdij KFExtracting stability increases the SNP heritability of emotional problems in young peopleTransl Psychiatry2018810.1038/s41398-018-0269-5 – reference: YoungAIFriggeMLGudbjartssonDFThorleifssonGBjornsdottirGSulemPRelatedness disequilibrium regression estimates heritability without environmental biasNat Genet2018501304101:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXhsFSqtbfO10.1038/s41588-018-0178-9301047646130754 – reference: FraserAMacdonald-WallisCTillingKBoydAGoldingJDavey SmithGCohort profile: The avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohortInt J Epidemiol2013429711010.1093/ije/dys066 – reference: KovasYGaron-CarrierGBoivinMPetrillSAPlominRMalykhSBWhy children differ in motivation to learn: Insights from over 13,000 twins from 6 countriesPers Individ Dif201580516310.1016/j.paid.2015.02.006260521744372262 – reference: MorrisTTDorlingDDavey SmithGHow well can we predict educational outcomes? Examining the roles of cognitive ability and social position in educational attainmentContemp Soc Sci20161111510.1080/21582041.2016.1138502 – reference: HeckmanJJKautzTHard evidence on soft skillsLabour Econ2012194516410.1016/j.labeco.2012.05.014235596943612993 – reference: HeckmanJJStixrudJUrzuaSThe effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behaviorJ Labor Econ2006244118210.1086/504455 – reference: MalanchiniMEngelhardtLEGrotzingerADHardenKPTucker-DrobEM‘Same but different’: associations between multiple aspects of self-regulation, cognition, and academic abilitiesJ Pers Soc Psychol201911711648810.1037/pspp0000224 – reference: Gutman LM, Schoon I. The impact of non-cognitive skills on outcomes for young people: literature review. Educ Endow Found London Engl. pp. 58 2013. https://educationendowmentfoundation.org.uk/education-evidence/evidence-reviews/essential-life-skills. – reference: GoodmanALampingDLPloubidisGBWhen to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ): data from british parents, teachers and childrenJ Abnorm Child Psychol20103811799110.1007/s10802-010-9434-x – reference: BlandenJGreggPMacmillanLAccounting for intergenerational income persistence: noncognitive skills, ability and educationEcon J2007117C436010.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02034.x – reference: St PourcainBHaworthCMDavisOSWangKTimpsonNJEvansDMHeritability and genome-wide analyses of problematic peer relationships during childhood and adolescenceHum Genet20151345395110.1007/s00439-014-1514-5 – reference: SchmidtFLHunterJEThe validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findingsPsychol Bull19981242627410.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262 – reference: Lemery-ChalfantKDoelgerLGoldsmithHHGenetic relations between effortful and attentional control and symptoms of psychopathology in middle childhoodInfant Child Dev Int J Res Pr2008173658510.1002/icd.581 – reference: DohmenTFalkAHuffmanDSundeUSchuppJWagnerGGIndividual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequencesJ Eur Econ Assoc201195225010.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x – reference: TaylorAEJonesHJSallisHEuesdenJStergiakouliEDaviesNMExploring the association of genetic factors with participation in the avon longitudinal study of parents and childrenInt J Epidemiol20184712071610.1093/ije/dyy060 – reference: BoydAGoldingJMacleodJLawlorDAFraserAHendersonJCohort Profile: the’children of the 90s’–the index offspring of the avon longitudinal study of parents and childrenInt J Epidemiol2013421112710.1093/ije/dys064 – reference: BorghansLDuckworthALHeckmanJJTer WeelBThe economics and psychology of personality traitsJ Hum Resour2008439721059. – reference: SmithersLGSawyerACPChittleboroughCRDaviesNMDavey SmithGLynchJWA systematic review and meta-analysis of effects of early life non-cognitive skills on academic, psychosocial, cognitive and health outcomesNat Hum Behav201828678010.1038/s41562-018-0461-x – reference: Gidziela A, Rimfeld K, Malanchini M, Allegrini AG, McMillan A, Selzam S, et al. Using DNA to predict behaviour problems from preschool to adulthood. medRxiv 2021. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.15.21251308. – reference: BorghansLGolsteynBHHHeckmanJHumphriesJEIdentification problems in personality psychologyPers Individ Dif2011513152010.1016/j.paid.2011.03.029217311703126096 – reference: Laajaj R, Macours K. Measuring Skills in Developing Countries. Policy Res Work Paper. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1596/1813-9450-8000. – reference: Kautz T, Heckman J, Diris R, ter Weel B, Borghans L. Fostering and measuring skills: improving cognitive and non-cognitive skills to promote lifetime success. 2014 https://doi.org/10.1787/19939019. – reference: HaworthSMitchellRCorbinLWadeKHDuddingTBudu-AggreyAApparent latent structure within the UK Biobank sample has implications for epidemiological analysisNat Commun20191010.1038/s41467-018-08219-1 – reference: Duckworth AL, Quinn PD. Development and validation of the short Grit Scale (Grit-S). J Pers Assess. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890802634290. – reference: BarrickMRMountMKThe big five personality dimensions and job performance: a meta-analysisPers Psychol19914412610.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x – reference: BouchardTJLoehlinJCGenes, evolution, and personalityBehav Genet2001312437310.1023/A:101229432471311699599 – reference: LucianoMWainwrightMAWrightMJMartinNGThe heritability of conscientiousness facets and their relationship to IQ and academic achievementPers Individ Dif20064011899910.1016/j.paid.2005.10.013 – reference: WillemsYEDolanCVvan BeijsterveldtCde ZeeuwELBoomsmaDIBartelsMGenetic and environmental influences on self-control: assessing self-control with the ASEBA self-control scaleBehav Genet2018481354610.1007/s10519-018-9887-1 – reference: WillemsYEBoesenNLiJFinkenauerCBartelsMThe heritability of self-control: a meta-analysisNeurosci Biobehav Rev2019100324341:STN:280:DC%2BB3cfpt1yitg%3D%3D10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.012 – reference: Cobb-ClarkDASchurerSTwo economists’ musings on the stability of locus of controlEcon J2013123F35840010.1111/ecoj.12069 – reference: KongAThorleifssonGFriggeMLVilhjalmssonBJYoungAIThorgeirssonTEThe nature of nurture: effects of parental genotypesScience (80-)201835942481:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXhsVGhsrg%3D10.1126/science.aan6877 – reference: PlominRDearyIJGenetics and intelligence differences: five special findingsMol Psychiatry201520981081:STN:280:DC%2BC2M7hsFKlsg%3D%3D10.1038/mp.2014.10525224258 – reference: Fleche S. Teacher Quality, Test Scores and Non-Cognitive Skills: Evidence from Primary School Teachers in the UK. 2017. http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/83602/. – reference: Brunello G, Schlotter M. Non Cognitive Skills and Personality Traits: Labour Market Relevance and their Development in Education & Training Systems. IZA Discussion Paper No. 5743. 2011. – reference: BenjaminiYHochbergYControlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testingJ R Stat Soc Ser B19955728930010.2307/2346101 – reference: RaevuoriADickDMKeski-RahkonenAPulkkinenLRoseRJRissanenAGenetic and environmental factors affecting self-esteem from age 14 to 17: a longitudinal study of Finnish twinsPsychol Med20073716253310.1017/S0033291707000840 – reference: Tucker-DrobEMBrileyDAEngelhardtLEMannFDHardenKPGenetically-mediated associations between measures of childhood character and academic achievementJ Pers Soc Psychol201611179081510.1037/pspp0000098 – reference: GoodmanRThe Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research noteJ child Psychol psychiatry19973858161:STN:280:DyaK2szpvFKjsA%3D%3D10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x – reference: Tucker-DrobEMHardenKPA behavioral genetic perspective on noncognitive factors and academic achievementPsychological Sci2015271384910.1177/0956797615612727 – reference: ReddenDTAllisonDBThe effect of assortative mating upon genetic association studies: Spurious associations and population substructure in the absence of admixtureBehav Genet2006366788610.1007/s10519-006-9060-016514473 – reference: Carneiro P, Crawford C, Goodman A. The impact of early cognitive and non-cognitive skills on later outcomes. Center for the Economics of Education Discussion Paper 92. 2007. – reference: McGueMRustichiniAIaconoWGCognitive, noncognitive, and family background contributions to college attainment: a behavioral genetic perspectiveJ Pers201585657810.1111/jopy.12230 – volume: 117 start-page: C43 year: 2007 ident: 1661_CR37 publication-title: Econ J doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02034.x – volume: 24 start-page: 411 year: 2006 ident: 1661_CR7 publication-title: J Labor Econ doi: 10.1086/504455 – volume: 44 start-page: 1 year: 1991 ident: 1661_CR38 publication-title: Pers Psychol doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1991.tb00688.x – volume: 50 start-page: 1304 year: 2018 ident: 1661_CR52 publication-title: Nat Genet doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0178-9 – volume: 111 start-page: 790 year: 2016 ident: 1661_CR17 publication-title: J Pers Soc Psychol doi: 10.1037/pspp0000098 – volume: 359 start-page: 424 year: 2018 ident: 1661_CR43 publication-title: Science (80-) doi: 10.1126/science.aan6877 – volume: 9 start-page: 522 year: 2011 ident: 1661_CR24 publication-title: J Eur Econ Assoc doi: 10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x – volume: 140 start-page: 1303 year: 2014 ident: 1661_CR31 publication-title: Psychol Bull doi: 10.1037/a0037091 – volume: 126 start-page: 3 year: 2000 ident: 1661_CR22 publication-title: Psychol Bull doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3 – volume: 43 start-page: 971 year: 2007 ident: 1661_CR14 publication-title: Pers Individ Dif doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017 – volume: 19 start-page: 451 year: 2012 ident: 1661_CR4 publication-title: Labour Econ doi: 10.1016/j.labeco.2012.05.014 – volume: 124 start-page: 262 year: 1998 ident: 1661_CR15 publication-title: Psychol Bull doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262 – ident: 1661_CR11 doi: 10.2139/ssrn.1858066 – volume: 2 start-page: 867 year: 2018 ident: 1661_CR20 publication-title: Nat Hum Behav doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0461-x – volume: 123 start-page: F358 year: 2013 ident: 1661_CR21 publication-title: Econ J doi: 10.1111/ecoj.12069 – volume: 17 start-page: 365 year: 2008 ident: 1661_CR27 publication-title: Infant Child Dev Int J Res Pr doi: 10.1002/icd.581 – ident: 1661_CR10 – volume: 8 year: 2018 ident: 1661_CR47 publication-title: Transl Psychiatry doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0269-5 – volume: 57 start-page: 289 year: 1995 ident: 1661_CR40 publication-title: J R Stat Soc Ser B doi: 10.2307/2346101 – ident: 1661_CR8 doi: 10.1080/00223890802634290 – volume: 75 start-page: 1 year: 1976 ident: 1661_CR3 publication-title: Sociol Educ doi: 10.2307/3090251 – volume: 85 start-page: 65 year: 2015 ident: 1661_CR9 publication-title: J Pers doi: 10.1111/jopy.12230 – volume: 38 start-page: 581 year: 1997 ident: 1661_CR35 publication-title: J child Psychol psychiatry doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x – volume: 36 start-page: 678 year: 2006 ident: 1661_CR55 publication-title: Behav Genet doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9060-0 – volume: 91 start-page: 1011 year: 2012 ident: 1661_CR53 publication-title: Am J Hum Genet doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2012.10.010 – volume: 6 start-page: 574 year: 2011 ident: 1661_CR16 publication-title: Perspect Psychol Sci doi: 10.1177/1745691611421204 – volume: 43 start-page: 1428 year: 2007 ident: 1661_CR5 publication-title: Dev Psychol doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 – ident: 1661_CR23 doi: 10.1596/1813-9450-8000 – volume: 80 start-page: 51 year: 2015 ident: 1661_CR30 publication-title: Pers Individ Dif doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.006 – volume: 134 start-page: 539 year: 2015 ident: 1661_CR46 publication-title: Hum Genet doi: 10.1007/s00439-014-1514-5 – volume: 48 start-page: 135 year: 2018 ident: 1661_CR26 publication-title: Behav Genet doi: 10.1007/s10519-018-9887-1 – volume: 31 start-page: 243 year: 2001 ident: 1661_CR25 publication-title: Behav Genet doi: 10.1023/A:1012294324713 – ident: 1661_CR1 doi: 10.1787/19939019 – volume: 85 start-page: 51 year: 2017 ident: 1661_CR51 publication-title: J Pers doi: 10.1111/jopy.12186 – volume: 42 start-page: 97 year: 2013 ident: 1661_CR34 publication-title: Int J Epidemiol doi: 10.1093/ije/dys066 – volume: 27 start-page: 138 year: 2015 ident: 1661_CR32 publication-title: Psychological Sci doi: 10.1177/0956797615612727 – ident: 1661_CR6 – volume: 51 start-page: 315 year: 2011 ident: 1661_CR13 publication-title: Pers Individ Dif doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.029 – ident: 1661_CR12 – volume: 30 start-page: 711 year: 2019 ident: 1661_CR19 publication-title: Psychol Sci doi: 10.1177/0956797619831612 – volume: 11 start-page: 275 year: 2008 ident: 1661_CR28 publication-title: Twin Res Hum Genet doi: 10.1375/twin.11.3.275 – volume: 117 start-page: 1164 year: 2019 ident: 1661_CR18 publication-title: J Pers Soc Psychol doi: 10.1037/pspp0000224 – volume: 47 start-page: 1207 year: 2018 ident: 1661_CR45 publication-title: Int J Epidemiol doi: 10.1093/ije/dyy060 – volume: 42 start-page: 111 year: 2013 ident: 1661_CR33 publication-title: Int J Epidemiol doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064 – ident: 1661_CR42 doi: 10.1101/2021.02.15.21251308 – volume: 20 start-page: 98 year: 2015 ident: 1661_CR44 publication-title: Mol Psychiatry doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.105 – volume: 100 start-page: 324 year: 2019 ident: 1661_CR50 publication-title: Neurosci Biobehav Rev doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.012 – volume: 10 year: 2019 ident: 1661_CR54 publication-title: Nat Commun doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08219-1 – volume: 88 start-page: 76 year: 2011 ident: 1661_CR41 publication-title: Am J Hum Genet doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011 – volume: 40 start-page: 1189 year: 2006 ident: 1661_CR29 publication-title: Pers Individ Dif doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.013 – volume: 175 start-page: 559 year: 1999 ident: 1661_CR49 publication-title: Br J Psychiatry doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.6.559 – volume: 43 start-page: 972 year: 2008 ident: 1661_CR2 publication-title: J Hum Resour – volume: 37 start-page: 1625 year: 2007 ident: 1661_CR48 publication-title: Psychol Med doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000840 – volume: 11 start-page: 1 year: 2016 ident: 1661_CR39 publication-title: Contemp Soc Sci doi: 10.1080/21582041.2016.1138502 – volume: 38 start-page: 1179 year: 2010 ident: 1661_CR36 publication-title: J Abnorm Child Psychol doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x |
SSID | ssj0000548171 |
Score | 2.3038952 |
Snippet | Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data and have... Abstract Noncognitive skills have been shown to associate with a range of health and socioeconomic outcomes. Many studies have relied on cross sectional data... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref springer |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 563 |
SubjectTerms | 631/208/212 631/477/2811 Adolescent Behavioral Sciences Biological Psychology Birth Cohort Child Communication Cross-Sectional Studies Educational Status Humans Locus of control Medicine Medicine & Public Health Neurosciences Personality Pharmacotherapy Psychiatry Skills United Kingdom |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Lb9QwEB5VPaBeEIUCoQUZqTeI6vixto-AqCpQOXWlHpAsO7HFilUWbbIH_j1jJ5t2gZYL18RJJvPIfM68AE4pc57LGg0pBlGK2JgS3VBVKmGaWkcfZY7oXn6ZXczFp2t5fWvUV8oJG9oDD4w7M8zxGg1SSi-EcNwzoYPxKla1DjOdy3ypobc2U0NXb6ErVY1VMpTrsw6_1qmajKXdMzqlUu94otyw_28o889kyd8iptkRnT-ChyOCJO8Gyg9hL7SP4cHlGCN_Al_zDM4uYeGfZBUJ7u-nHCHSfV8slx1xbUNyiICsx2Q40q9I2CZ74O1Xmx51MXRk0RJH5p9JmqS77o9gfv7x6sNFOc5QKGvEYn0ZXXQ6pK55QiaDY7EWNErteCUbRIvOO9lIZSofeBNYHWljdPLaFPmtUI5PYR_pDM-BRASHNCIicqn-1ihPOWcmUpXKY30IBVRbftp6bDCe5lwsbQ50c20HGViUgc0ysLqAN9M1P4b2Gveufp_ENK1MrbHzAVQYOyqM_ZfCFHCyFbId7bWzuAnkSkghVAGvp9NoaSl84tqw2uQ1-Nro0WcFPBt0YqKEC0Rm-KksQO1oyw6pu2faxbfczVtLVbEZPvftVq9uyLqbFS_-ByuO4YAlg0h_yeUJ7PfrTXiJGKv3r7I5_QKlUh9- priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – databaseName: ProQuest Central dbid: BENPR link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1bi9QwFA7rLogv4t3qKhF807Jtk0zSBxFXdlmUHUQc2AchJGmig0O7TjsP_nvPyaRdxsu-tmmb5Ny-5twIeVlUxjLhQJCC5zkPTZ2DGSpzyevGqWCDiB7d8_nsbME_XIiLPTIfc2EwrHLUiVFRN53DM_IjgOlMcsG5fHv5M8euUehdHVtomNRaoXkTS4zdIAegkhXw_cHxyfzT5-nUBQCKKmWZsmcKpo560OKYZVbhXzUYq1ztWKhYyP9f6PPvIMo_PKnRQJ3eIbcTsqTvtqxwl-z59h65eZ585_fJ19ibs0eM_It2gcJ__xQ7RPsfy9Wqp6ZtaHQd0HUKkqNDR_0YBAKv7zYD7Jfv6bKlhi4-Uuywux4ekMXpyZf3Z3nqrZA7wGhDHkwwymM1PS5QEKvgeBGEMqwUDaBIY41ohKxL61njKxeKplZozQthuQT6PiT7ME__mNAAoLEIgJQM5uXW0haMVXUoJKbNWu8zUo77qV0qPI79L1Y6OsCZ0lsaaKCBjjTQKiOvpmcut2U3rh19jGSaRmLJ7HihW3_TSQJ1XRnmQLMLWADnhtmKK19bGUqn_EzBSw5HIuskx72-4rqMvJhugwSiW8W0vtvEMbBssPSzjDza8sQ0E8YBsYEKzYjc4Zadqe7eaZffY5VvJWRZzeC7r0e-uprW_7fiyfWreEpuVcjqeC4uDsn-sN74Z4CqBvs8icpvn8MeQw priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest |
Title | Consistency of noncognitive skills and their relation to educational outcomes in a UK cohort |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.1038/s41398-021-01661-8 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34741011 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2593745447 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2594298016 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC8571267 https://doaj.org/article/92a3c11155b444a3b248e9b7f1c8e688 |
Volume | 11 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3di9QwEB_u9kB8Eb-tnksE37TY5qNJH_eWO46VO0Rd2AchpG1yLi6tbLsP_vdOsm1l9RR8KrRpO8nMZH7JfATgdUJNwUSJiuQsj7mr8hjNUBpLnlelcoUTwaN7dZ1dLvliJVZHQIdcmBC0H0pahml6iA571-Jk65PBqF_8ok2J1TGcKDR_dAIns9ni02LcWUEQolKZ9hkyCVO3vHxghUKx_tsQ5p-Bkr95S4MRurgP93r0SGZ7eh_Aka0fwp2r3j_-CL6E8zdbj4N_kMYRXNuP8UGk_bbebFpi6ooE9wDZ9oFwpGuIHQI98PPNrsNRsS1Z18SQ5XviT9Hddo9heXH-eX4Z9-cnxCXisC52xhllfcU8LryyUVfyxAllWCoqRIqmMKISMk8LyypLS5dUufIWOxEFl8jDJzBBOu0zIA6BYeIQDRmfe5vLImGM5i6RPjW2sDaCdBhPXfbFxf0ZFxsdnNxM6T0PNPJABx5oFcGb8Z3v-9Ia_2x95tk0tvRlscONZnujezHROTWsxNlbYAc4N6ygXNm8kC4tlc0UfuR0YLLudbXVuABkkgvOZQSvxseoZd51Ymrb7EIb7DZa8yyCp3uZGClhHFEZTpMRyANpOSD18Em9_hoqeSshU5rhf98OcvWLrL8PxfP_a_4C7lIv-n4vXJzCpNvu7EtEUl0xhWO5ktNegfB6dn794SPenWfzadid-An4ExsU |
linkProvider | Springer Nature |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1bb9MwFLZGJwEviDuFAUaCJ4iW-FI7DxNisKmja4XQKu0ByXMSm1WrktGkQvtz_DaOXSdTuextr4nj2D7n-Hz2uSH0OiY6ozwHQbKGRcwWaQRqKIkES4tc2sxyb9EdTwbDKft8zI830K82Fsa5VbZ7ot-oiyp3d-TbANOpYJwx8f78R-SqRjnraltCQ4fSCsWOTzEWAjtG5uInHOHqnYNPQO83hOzvHX0cRqHKQJQDWmkiq62WxuWVY9yxJLE5iy2Xmia8ADylM80LLtIkM7QwJLdxkUqn12KeMQEzhX5voE3mIlx7aHN3b_Lla3fLA4BIJiIJ0Toxlds1aA0X1UbcKR6UYyTXNKIvHPAvtPu30-YflluvEPfvojsByeIPK9a7hzZMeR_dHAdb_QP0zdcCrR0mv8CVxWVVdr5KuD6bzec11mWBvakCL4JTHm4qbFqnE-i-WjZAH1PjWYk1no6wq-i7aB6i6bWs8iPUg3GaJwhbAKmxBWSmXRxwKrKYUpLaWLgw3cyYPkra9VR5SHTu6m3MlTe4U6lWNFBAA-VpoGQfve2-OV-l-biy9a4jU9fSpej2D6rFdxUkXqVE0xw0CYcJMKZpRpg0aSZskkszkNDJVktkFfaNWl1yeR-96l6DxDszji5NtfRtYNqALAZ99HjFE91IKAOECFt2H4k1blkb6vqbcnbqs4pLLhIygP--a_nqclj_X4qnV8_iJbo1PBofqsODyegZuk0c27s7eb6Fes1iaZ4DomuyF0FsMDq5bkn9DdYZWrQ |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1bb9MwFLZGJ028IO50DDASPEHUxJfaeZgQY6s2yqoJUWkPSJ6T2FCtSkaTCu0v8qs4dp1M5bK3vSaOY_uc4_PZ54bQq5jojPIcBMkaFjFbpBGooSQSLC1yaTPLvUX3eDI8nLKPp_x0A_1qY2GcW2W7J_qNuqhyd0c-AJhOBeOMiYENbhEn-6N3Fz8iV0HKWVrbcho6lFkodn26sRDkMTaXP-E4V-8e7QPtXxMyOvjy4TAKFQeiHJBLE1lttTQuxxzjjj2JzVlsudQ04QVgK51pXnCRJpmhhSG5jYtUOh0X84wJmDX0ewttCtCSrIc29w4mJ5-7Gx8ARzIRSYjciakc1KBBXIQbcSd6UJSRXNOOvojAv5Dv3w6cf1hxvXIc3UV3AqrF71dseA9tmPI-2joOdvsH6KuvC1o7fH6JK4vLquz8lnB9PpvPa6zLAnuzBV4EBz3cVNi0DijQfbVsgFamxrMSazwdY1fdd9E8RNMbWeVHqAfjNE8QtgBYYwsoTbuY4FRkMaUktbFwIbuZMX2UtOup8pD03NXemCtvfKdSrWiggAbK00DJPnrTfXOxSvlxbes9R6aupUvX7R9Ui28qSL9KiaY5aBUOE2BM04wwadJM2CSXZiihk52WyCrsIbW64vg-etm9Bul3Jh1dmmrp28C0AWUM--jxiie6kVAGaBG27z4Sa9yyNtT1N-Xsu88wLrlIyBD--7blq6th_X8ptq-fxQu0BRKrPh1Nxk_RbeK43l3P8x3UaxZL8wzAXZM9D1KD0dlNC-pvD_Be4A |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Consistency+of+noncognitive+skills+and+their+relation+to+educational+outcomes+in+a+UK+cohort&rft.jtitle=Translational+psychiatry&rft.au=Morris%2C+Tim+T.&rft.au=Davey+Smith%2C+George&rft.au=van+den+Berg%2C+Gerard&rft.au=Davies%2C+Neil+M.&rft.date=2021-11-05&rft.pub=Nature+Publishing+Group+UK&rft.eissn=2158-3188&rft.volume=11&rft.issue=1&rft_id=info:doi/10.1038%2Fs41398-021-01661-8&rft.externalDocID=10_1038_s41398_021_01661_8 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2158-3188&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2158-3188&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2158-3188&client=summon |