The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox for MIDI interfaces, percussion pads, and sound cards

The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI–USB converter (or MIDI–PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBehavior research methods Vol. 51; no. 1; pp. 204 - 234
Main Author Schultz, Benjamin G.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published New York Springer US 15.02.2019
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1554-3528
1554-351X
1554-3528
DOI10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7

Cover

Loading…
Abstract The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI–USB converter (or MIDI–PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI–USB and MIDI–PCI interfaces. MIDI–PCI was superior to MIDI–USB, primarily because MIDI–USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion.
AbstractList The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI-USB converter (or MIDI-PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI-USB and MIDI-PCI interfaces. MIDI-PCI was superior to MIDI-USB, primarily because MIDI-USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion.
The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI–USB converter (or MIDI–PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI–USB and MIDI–PCI interfaces. MIDI–PCI was superior to MIDI–USB, primarily because MIDI–USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion.
The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI-USB converter (or MIDI-PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI-USB and MIDI-PCI interfaces. MIDI-PCI was superior to MIDI-USB, primarily because MIDI-USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion.The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI-USB converter (or MIDI-PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI-USB and MIDI-PCI interfaces. MIDI-PCI was superior to MIDI-USB, primarily because MIDI-USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion.
Author Schultz, Benjamin G.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Benjamin G.
  surname: Schultz
  fullname: Schultz, Benjamin G.
  email: ben.schultz@maastrichtuniversity.nl
  organization: Basic & Applied Neurodynamics Laboratory, Department of Neuropsychology & Psychopharmolcology, Maastricht University
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29667081$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9UU1vEzEUtFAq2gR-ABe0EhcObPHn2ntBKoFCpCAOhBMHy_FH4rKxU3sXAb8ep1tKiQQX23pvZvzmzRRMQgwWgCcInhPOxMuMCMWihkjUCFJc8wfgDDFGa8KwmNx7n4JpzlcQEoERfQhOcds0HAp0Br6strb6pLdD1_-sPizeLKrXNujtTqWvPmyqVYzdOn6vXExj14feJqe0zS-qvU16yNnHUO2VKQUVTJXjUE6tksmPwIlTXbaPb-8Z-Hz5djV_Xy8_vlvML5a1ZoT3tdOwEc7ANTFcNVRQyx0yBBkrGmKIEAgLyNs1o43m0LRM4FI0mhCjW0cdmYFXo-5-WO-s0Tb0SXVyn3yx8UNG5eXfneC3chO_yYZiSBErAs9vBVK8Hmzu5c5nbbtOBRuHLDHEHDYtbHGBPjuCXsUhhWJPYtQiwbi4QT29P9HdKL_3XgBoBOgUc07W3UEQlIds5ZitLNnKQ7aSFw4_4mjfq76sv5jy3X-ZeGTm8kvY2PRn6H-TfgHeOrcM
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_mex_2021_101427
crossref_primary_10_1063_5_0050999
crossref_primary_10_1109_TNSRE_2023_3321874
crossref_primary_10_1155_2022_5957708
crossref_primary_10_1155_2022_7569763
crossref_primary_10_1525_mp_2021_39_2_202
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_021_01653_y
crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2020_3021523
Cites_doi 10.1007/s00221-011-2616-0
10.1038/s41562-016-0007
10.1214/aos/1176346577
10.3758/s13428-013-0363-9
10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.10.002
10.1121/1.413218
10.1163/22134468-03002040
10.1080/01621459.1926.10502161
10.1080/00222899709603468
10.3758/BF03214280
10.1525/mp.2010.28.2.121
10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.006
10.1037/a0021435
10.1016/0001-6918(64)90032-0
10.1037/a0024208
10.3758/BF03206433
10.3758/PBR.16.2.225
10.3758/s13428-015-0671-3
10.2307/3679834
10.1080/09298210802322401
10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.469
10.1007/s004260100069
10.1038/249592a0
10.1037/a0037154
10.1121/1.1911293
10.1007/s004220050474
10.1177/1029864911401173
10.3758/s13428-013-0336-z
10.1016/j.humov.2009.08.002
10.3758/BF03200586
10.3758/BF03195348
10.1007/s00221-009-2115-8
10.1002/bimj.200810425
10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.205
10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2
10.5281/zenodo.16178
10.1016/j.brainres.2017.07.001
10.1145/2669485.2669514
10.1145/1517664.1517735
10.1525/mp.2009.27.2.103
10.3758/BF03203375
10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.61
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright The Author(s) 2018
Copyright Springer Nature B.V. 2019
Copyright_xml – notice: The Author(s) 2018
– notice: Copyright Springer Nature B.V. 2019
DBID C6C
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
4T-
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7
DatabaseName Springer Nature OA Free Journals
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Docstoc
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Neurosciences Abstracts
Docstoc
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE


MEDLINE - Academic
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: C6C
  name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals
  url: http://www.springeropen.com/
  sourceTypes: Publisher
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1554-3528
EndPage 234
ExternalDocumentID PMC6420415
29667081
10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Maastricht University
– fundername: ;
GroupedDBID ---
-55
-5G
-BR
-DZ
-EM
-ET
-~C
-~X
06D
0R~
0VY
199
1N0
203
23N
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
30V
4.4
406
408
40E
53G
5GY
875
8TC
8UJ
95.
96X
AABHQ
AACDK
AAHNG
AAJBT
AAJKR
AAKPC
AANZL
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYZH
AAZMS
ABAKF
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABJUD
ABKCH
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABPPZ
ABQBU
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIWK
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACPIV
ACPRK
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETCA
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFQWF
AFRAH
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHMBA
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AJRNO
AJZVZ
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
B-.
BAWUL
BENPR
BGNMA
C6C
CSCUP
DDRTE
DIK
DNIVK
DPUIP
E3Z
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMOBN
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FNLPD
FRRFC
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNWQR
GQ3
GQ6
GQ7
GUQSH
HF~
HMJXF
HRMNR
HVGLF
IAO
IHR
IKXTQ
INH
IPY
ITC
ITM
IWAJR
J-C
JBSCW
JZLTJ
KOV
LLZTM
M2O
M4Y
N9A
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O93
O9G
O9J
OK1
P2P
P9L
PF-
PT4
R9I
ROL
RPV
RSV
S16
S27
S3B
SBS
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TN5
TR2
TSG
TUC
TUS
U2A
U9L
UG4
UOJIU
UPT
UTJUX
UZXMN
VFIZW
W48
WH7
WK8
XSW
Z7R
Z7S
Z7W
Z81
Z83
Z88
Z8N
Z92
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
ZUP
0-V
2VQ
7X7
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8G5
AAAVM
AAIAL
AAPKM
AAYTO
AAYXX
ABBRH
ABDBE
ABFSG
ABQSL
ABUWG
ACBXY
ACKIV
ACSTC
ADHKG
AEBTG
AEKMD
AEZWR
AFDZB
AFFNX
AFHIU
AFKRA
AFOHR
AGJBK
AGQPQ
AHPBZ
AHSBF
AHWEU
AIXLP
AJBLW
ALSLI
ARALO
ATHPR
AYFIA
AZQEC
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C1A
CAG
CCPQU
CITATION
COF
DWQXO
FINBP
FSGXE
FYUFA
GNUQQ
H13
HMCUK
HZ~
H~9
IRVIT
M1P
M2M
M2R
MVM
N2Q
O9-
OHT
PADUT
PHGZM
PHGZT
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
S1Z
SBU
SCLPG
UKHRP
XJT
XOL
ABRTQ
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
PJZUB
PPXIY
PRQQA
4T-
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c537t-fc068fd0b3d7a6484e7f1d31de863d388128079b546c70d9582388dc33dc9f4f3
IEDL.DBID AGYKE
ISSN 1554-3528
1554-351X
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:07:13 EDT 2025
Fri Sep 05 13:24:16 EDT 2025
Sun Jul 13 03:35:09 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 05:58:27 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:05:53 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:52:58 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:37:00 EST 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 1
Keywords Motor timing
Sensorimotor synchronization
Auditory feedback
Microcontrollers
Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI)
Language English
License Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c537t-fc068fd0b3d7a6484e7f1d31de863d388128079b546c70d9582388dc33dc9f4f3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7
PMID 29667081
PQID 2191857892
PQPubID 976348
PageCount 31
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6420415
proquest_miscellaneous_2027069092
proquest_journals_2191857892
pubmed_primary_29667081
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7
crossref_citationtrail_10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7
springer_journals_10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 20190215
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2019-02-15
PublicationDate_xml – month: 2
  year: 2019
  text: 20190215
  day: 15
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace New York
PublicationPlace_xml – name: New York
– name: United States
PublicationTitle Behavior research methods
PublicationTitleAbbrev Behav Res
PublicationTitleAlternate Behav Res Methods
PublicationYear 2019
Publisher Springer US
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer US
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Collyer, Boatright-Horowitz, Hooper (CR2) 1997; 29
Maidhof, Kästner, Makkonen (CR24) 2014; 46
Schubert, D’Ausilio, Canto (CR51) 2013; 45
CR38
CR32
CR30
Thaut, Miller, Schauer (CR54) 1998; 79
Nordmark (CR31) 1968; 44
Sturges (CR53) 1926; 21
Janata, Tomic, Haberman (CR14) 2012; 141
Repp (CR40) 2005; 12
Hurley, Martens, Janata (CR11) 2014; 40
CR3
Rogers, Röhlig, Weing, Gugenheimer, Könings, Klepsch (CR48) 2014
CR6
Kaiser, Keller (CR16) 2011; 15
CR8
Aschersleben, Prinz (CR1) 1997; 29
CR44
Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, Sarkar, Core Team (CR37) 2015; 3
Repp, Su (CR47) 2013; 20
Hartigan, Hartigan (CR9) 1985; 13
Pecenka, Keller (CR33) 2011; 211
Friberg, Sundberg (CR7) 1995; 98
Repp, Knoblich (CR43) 2007; 33
Repp, Keller (CR42) 2010; 202
Sadakata, Hoppe, Brandmeyer, Timmers, Desain (CR50) 2008; 37
CR17
CR13
CR57
CR12
CR56
CR55
Jeffreys (CR15) 1961
Pfordresher, Kulpa (CR36) 2011; 37
Ravignani, Delgado, Kirby (CR39) 2016; 1
Moore (CR29) 1988; 12
Mills, van der Steen, Schultz, Keller (CR28) 2015; 3
Schultz, van Vugt (CR52) 2016; 48
Repp, London, Keller (CR45) 2012; 30
Pfordresher, Dalla Bella (CR35) 2011; 37
Repp, Marcus (CR46) 2010; 28
Marcus (CR25) 1981; 30
CR27
Hothorn, Bretz, Westfall (CR10) 2008; 50
Repp (CR41) 2010; 29
Large, Fink, Kelso (CR19) 2002; 66
Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, Iverson (CR49) 2009; 16
CR23
CR21
CR20
Pfordresher, Benitez (CR34) 2007; 26
Knörig, Wettach, Cohen (CR18) 2009
Michon (CR26) 1964; 22
Elliott, Welchman, Wing (CR4) 2009; 177
Lunney (CR22) 1974; 249
Finney (CR5) 2001; 33
A Friberg (1042_CR7) 1995; 98
M Sadakata (1042_CR50) 2008; 37
BG Schultz (1042_CR52) 2016; 48
HMW Lunney (1042_CR22) 1974; 249
JN Rouder (1042_CR49) 2009; 16
1042_CR44
A Ravignani (1042_CR39) 2016; 1
BH Repp (1042_CR42) 2010; 202
EW Large (1042_CR19) 2002; 66
T Hothorn (1042_CR10) 2008; 50
BH Repp (1042_CR46) 2010; 28
1042_CR6
BH Repp (1042_CR40) 2005; 12
1042_CR8
PQ Pfordresher (1042_CR35) 2011; 37
PF Mills (1042_CR28) 2015; 3
1042_CR3
BK Hurley (1042_CR11) 2014; 40
1042_CR12
1042_CR56
1042_CR13
1042_CR57
1042_CR55
1042_CR17
PQ Pfordresher (1042_CR36) 2011; 37
C Maidhof (1042_CR24) 2014; 46
MT Elliott (1042_CR4) 2009; 177
JO Nordmark (1042_CR31) 1968; 44
H Jeffreys (1042_CR15) 1961
BH Repp (1042_CR45) 2012; 30
1042_CR20
CE Collyer (1042_CR2) 1997; 29
JA Michon (1042_CR26) 1964; 22
MH Thaut (1042_CR54) 1998; 79
1042_CR23
PQ Pfordresher (1042_CR34) 2007; 26
1042_CR21
BH Repp (1042_CR47) 2013; 20
1042_CR27
P Janata (1042_CR14) 2012; 141
BH Repp (1042_CR41) 2010; 29
G Aschersleben (1042_CR1) 1997; 29
SM Marcus (1042_CR25) 1981; 30
N Pecenka (1042_CR33) 2011; 211
BH Repp (1042_CR43) 2007; 33
K Rogers (1042_CR48) 2014
TW Schubert (1042_CR51) 2013; 45
1042_CR30
R Kaiser (1042_CR16) 2011; 15
1042_CR32
SA Finney (1042_CR5) 2001; 33
JA Hartigan (1042_CR9) 1985; 13
FR Moore (1042_CR29) 1988; 12
1042_CR38
J Pinheiro (1042_CR37) 2015; 3
A Knörig (1042_CR18) 2009
HA Sturges (1042_CR53) 1926; 21
References_xml – volume: 211
  start-page: 505
  year: 2011
  end-page: 515
  ident: CR33
  article-title: The role of temporal prediction abilities in interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2616-0
– volume: 1
  start-page: 0007
  year: 2016
  ident: CR39
  article-title: Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals
  publication-title: Nature Human Behaviour
  doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0007
– volume: 13
  start-page: 70
  year: 1985
  end-page: 84
  ident: CR9
  article-title: The dip test of unimodality
  publication-title: Annals of Statistics
  doi: 10.1214/aos/1176346577
– volume: 46
  start-page: 185
  year: 2014
  end-page: 195
  ident: CR24
  article-title: Combining EEG, MIDI, and motion capture techniques for investigating musical performance
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0363-9
– ident: CR12
– volume: 177
  start-page: 250
  year: 2009
  end-page: 257
  ident: CR4
  article-title: MatTAP: A MATLAB toolbox for the control and analysis of movement synchronisation experiments
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.10.002
– volume: 98
  start-page: 2524
  year: 1995
  end-page: 2531
  ident: CR7
  article-title: Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronous sequence
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.413218
– ident: CR8
– volume: 3
  start-page: 13
  year: 2015
  end-page: 31
  ident: CR28
  article-title: Individual differences in temporal anticipation and adaptation during sensorimotor synchronization
  publication-title: Timing & Time Perception
  doi: 10.1163/22134468-03002040
– volume: 21
  start-page: 65
  year: 1926
  end-page: 66
  ident: CR53
  article-title: The choice of a class interval
  publication-title: Journal of the American Statistical Association
  doi: 10.1080/01621459.1926.10502161
– volume: 29
  start-page: 35
  year: 1997
  end-page: 46
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization
  publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior
  doi: 10.1080/00222899709603468
– ident: CR21
– volume: 30
  start-page: 247
  year: 1981
  end-page: 256
  ident: CR25
  article-title: Acoustic determinants of perceptual center (P-center) location
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03214280
– volume: 28
  start-page: 121
  year: 2010
  end-page: 134
  ident: CR46
  article-title: No sustained sound illusion in rhythmic sequences
  publication-title: Music Perception
  doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.2.121
– start-page: 149
  year: 2014
  end-page: 158
  ident: CR48
  article-title: Piano: Faster piano learning with interactive projection
– start-page: 351
  year: 2009
  end-page: 358
  ident: CR18
  article-title: Fritzing: A tool for advancing electronic prototyping for designers
– volume: 26
  start-page: 742
  year: 2007
  end-page: 756
  ident: CR34
  article-title: Temporal coordination between actions and sound during sequence production
  publication-title: Human Movement Science
  doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.006
– volume: 37
  start-page: 949
  year: 2011
  end-page: 967
  ident: CR36
  article-title: The dynamics of disruption from altered auditory feedback: Further evidence for a dissociation of sequencing and timing
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0021435
– volume: 22
  start-page: 441
  year: 1964
  end-page: 450
  ident: CR26
  article-title: Studies on subjective duration 1: Differential sensitivity on the perception of repeated temporal intervals
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(64)90032-0
– ident: CR57
– ident: CR32
– volume: 141
  start-page: 54
  year: 2012
  end-page: 75
  ident: CR14
  article-title: Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/a0024208
– year: 1961
  ident: CR15
– volume: 12
  start-page: 969
  year: 2005
  end-page: 992
  ident: CR40
  article-title: Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206433
– volume: 16
  start-page: 225
  year: 2009
  end-page: 237
  ident: CR49
  article-title: Bayesian tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225
– volume: 48
  start-page: 1591
  year: 2016
  end-page: 1607
  ident: CR52
  article-title: Tap Arduino: An Arduino microcontroller for low-latency auditory feedback in sensorimotor synchronization experiments
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0671-3
– volume: 12
  start-page: 19
  year: 1988
  end-page: 28
  ident: CR29
  article-title: The dysfunctions of MIDI
  publication-title: Computer Music Journal
  doi: 10.2307/3679834
– volume: 37
  start-page: 207
  year: 2008
  end-page: 220
  ident: CR50
  article-title: Real-time visual feedback for learning to perform short rhythms with expressive variations in timing and loudness
  publication-title: Journal of New Music Research
  doi: 10.1080/09298210802322401
– volume: 33
  start-page: 469
  year: 2007
  end-page: 482
  ident: CR43
  article-title: Toward a psychophysics of agency: Detecting gain and loss of control over auditory action effects
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.469
– volume: 66
  start-page: 3
  year: 2002
  end-page: 17
  ident: CR19
  article-title: Tracking simple and complex sequences
  publication-title: Psychological Research
  doi: 10.1007/s004260100069
– volume: 249
  start-page: 592
  year: 1974
  ident: CR22
  article-title: Time as heard in speech and music
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/249592a0
– ident: CR30
– volume: 3
  start-page: 1
  year: 2015
  end-page: 120
  ident: CR37
  article-title: nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models
  publication-title: R package version
– ident: CR6
– volume: 40
  start-page: 1679
  year: 2014
  end-page: 1696
  ident: CR11
  article-title: Spontaneous sensorimotor coupling with multipart music
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0037154
– volume: 44
  start-page: 1533
  year: 1968
  end-page: 1540
  ident: CR31
  article-title: Mechanisms of frequency discrimination
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.1911293
– ident: CR56
– ident: CR27
– ident: CR23
– volume: 79
  start-page: 241
  year: 1998
  end-page: 250
  ident: CR54
  article-title: Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: Phase vs. period correction
  publication-title: Biological Cybernetics
  doi: 10.1007/s004220050474
– ident: CR44
– ident: CR3
– ident: CR38
– volume: 15
  start-page: 270
  year: 2011
  end-page: 287
  ident: CR16
  article-title: Music’s impact on the visual perception of emotional dyadic interactions
  publication-title: Musicae Scientiae
  doi: 10.1177/1029864911401173
– ident: CR17
– ident: CR13
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1332
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1346
  ident: CR51
  article-title: Using Arduino microcontroller boards to measure response latencies
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0336-z
– volume: 29
  start-page: 200
  year: 2010
  end-page: 213
  ident: CR41
  article-title: Sensorimotor synchronization and perception of timing: Effects of music training and task experience
  publication-title: Human Movement Science
  doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.08.002
– volume: 29
  start-page: 346
  year: 1997
  end-page: 352
  ident: CR2
  article-title: A motor timing experiment implemented using a musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) approach
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers
  doi: 10.3758/BF03200586
– ident: CR55
– volume: 33
  start-page: 65
  year: 2001
  end-page: 72
  ident: CR5
  article-title: FTAP: A Linux-based program for tapping and music experiments
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers
  doi: 10.3758/BF03195348
– volume: 202
  start-page: 101
  year: 2010
  end-page: 110
  ident: CR42
  article-title: Self versus other in piano performance: Detectability of timing perturbations depends on personal playing style
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2115-8
– volume: 50
  start-page: 346
  year: 2008
  end-page: 363
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Simultaneous inference in general parametric models
  publication-title: Biometrical Journal
  doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425
– volume: 30
  start-page: 205
  year: 2012
  end-page: 223
  ident: CR45
  article-title: Distortions in reproduction of two-interval rhythms: When the “attractor ratio” is not exactly 1:2
  publication-title: Music Perception
  doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.205
– ident: CR20
– volume: 20
  start-page: 403
  year: 2013
  end-page: 452
  ident: CR47
  article-title: Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012)
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2
– volume: 37
  start-page: 566
  year: 2011
  end-page: 579
  ident: CR35
  article-title: Delayed auditory feedback and movement
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– ident: 1042_CR56
  doi: 10.5281/zenodo.16178
– volume: 15
  start-page: 270
  year: 2011
  ident: 1042_CR16
  publication-title: Musicae Scientiae
  doi: 10.1177/1029864911401173
– volume: 20
  start-page: 403
  year: 2013
  ident: 1042_CR47
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2
– volume: 12
  start-page: 969
  year: 2005
  ident: 1042_CR40
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206433
– volume: 66
  start-page: 3
  year: 2002
  ident: 1042_CR19
  publication-title: Psychological Research
  doi: 10.1007/s004260100069
– ident: 1042_CR32
– volume: 21
  start-page: 65
  year: 1926
  ident: 1042_CR53
  publication-title: Journal of the American Statistical Association
  doi: 10.1080/01621459.1926.10502161
– ident: 1042_CR55
– ident: 1042_CR57
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.07.001
– ident: 1042_CR13
– start-page: 149
  volume-title: Proceedings of the Ninth ACM International Conference on Interactive Tabletops and Surfaces
  year: 2014
  ident: 1042_CR48
  doi: 10.1145/2669485.2669514
– volume: 28
  start-page: 121
  year: 2010
  ident: 1042_CR46
  publication-title: Music Perception
  doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.2.121
– volume: 50
  start-page: 346
  year: 2008
  ident: 1042_CR10
  publication-title: Biometrical Journal
  doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425
– volume: 30
  start-page: 247
  year: 1981
  ident: 1042_CR25
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03214280
– volume: 33
  start-page: 469
  year: 2007
  ident: 1042_CR43
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.469
– ident: 1042_CR8
– volume: 46
  start-page: 185
  year: 2014
  ident: 1042_CR24
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0363-9
– volume: 3
  start-page: 1
  year: 2015
  ident: 1042_CR37
  publication-title: R package version
– ident: 1042_CR38
– volume: 48
  start-page: 1591
  year: 2016
  ident: 1042_CR52
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0671-3
– start-page: 351
  volume-title: Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Tangible and Embedded Interaction
  year: 2009
  ident: 1042_CR18
  doi: 10.1145/1517664.1517735
– volume: 37
  start-page: 207
  year: 2008
  ident: 1042_CR50
  publication-title: Journal of New Music Research
  doi: 10.1080/09298210802322401
– volume: 249
  start-page: 592
  year: 1974
  ident: 1042_CR22
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/249592a0
– volume: 211
  start-page: 505
  year: 2011
  ident: 1042_CR33
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2616-0
– volume: 16
  start-page: 225
  year: 2009
  ident: 1042_CR49
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225
– volume: 202
  start-page: 101
  year: 2010
  ident: 1042_CR42
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2115-8
– ident: 1042_CR21
  doi: 10.1525/mp.2009.27.2.103
– ident: 1042_CR3
– volume: 30
  start-page: 205
  year: 2012
  ident: 1042_CR45
  publication-title: Music Perception
  doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.205
– volume: 79
  start-page: 241
  year: 1998
  ident: 1042_CR54
  publication-title: Biological Cybernetics
  doi: 10.1007/s004220050474
– volume: 98
  start-page: 2524
  year: 1995
  ident: 1042_CR7
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.413218
– ident: 1042_CR20
– volume: 37
  start-page: 566
  year: 2011
  ident: 1042_CR35
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 29
  start-page: 346
  year: 1997
  ident: 1042_CR2
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers
  doi: 10.3758/BF03200586
– ident: 1042_CR6
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1332
  year: 2013
  ident: 1042_CR51
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0336-z
– volume: 177
  start-page: 250
  year: 2009
  ident: 1042_CR4
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.10.002
– ident: 1042_CR30
– volume: 37
  start-page: 949
  year: 2011
  ident: 1042_CR36
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0021435
– ident: 1042_CR27
– volume: 141
  start-page: 54
  year: 2012
  ident: 1042_CR14
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/a0024208
– ident: 1042_CR17
  doi: 10.3758/BF03203375
– volume: 13
  start-page: 70
  year: 1985
  ident: 1042_CR9
  publication-title: Annals of Statistics
  doi: 10.1214/aos/1176346577
– ident: 1042_CR23
– volume: 29
  start-page: 35
  year: 1997
  ident: 1042_CR1
  publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior
  doi: 10.1080/00222899709603468
– ident: 1042_CR44
  doi: 10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.61
– volume: 44
  start-page: 1533
  year: 1968
  ident: 1042_CR31
  publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
  doi: 10.1121/1.1911293
– volume: 26
  start-page: 742
  year: 2007
  ident: 1042_CR34
  publication-title: Human Movement Science
  doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.006
– volume: 12
  start-page: 19
  year: 1988
  ident: 1042_CR29
  publication-title: Computer Music Journal
  doi: 10.2307/3679834
– volume: 40
  start-page: 1679
  year: 2014
  ident: 1042_CR11
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  doi: 10.1037/a0037154
– volume: 29
  start-page: 200
  year: 2010
  ident: 1042_CR41
  publication-title: Human Movement Science
  doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.08.002
– volume: 3
  start-page: 13
  year: 2015
  ident: 1042_CR28
  publication-title: Timing & Time Perception
  doi: 10.1163/22134468-03002040
– ident: 1042_CR12
– volume-title: Theory of probability
  year: 1961
  ident: 1042_CR15
– volume: 33
  start-page: 65
  year: 2001
  ident: 1042_CR5
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers
  doi: 10.3758/BF03195348
– volume: 22
  start-page: 441
  year: 1964
  ident: 1042_CR26
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(64)90032-0
– volume: 1
  start-page: 0007
  year: 2016
  ident: 1042_CR39
  publication-title: Nature Human Behaviour
  doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0007
SSID ssj0038214
Score 2.329208
Snippet The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
springer
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 204
SubjectTerms Auditory Perception
Auditory stimuli
Behavioral Research - instrumentation
Behavioral Science and Psychology
Benchmarking
Cognitive Psychology
Experiments
Feedback
Feedback, Sensory
Fingers
Humans
Interfaces
Latency
Musical instruments
Percussion
Psychology
Sensorimotor system
Sound
Sound boards
Synchronization
Title The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox for MIDI interfaces, percussion pads, and sound cards
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29667081
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2191857892
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2027069092
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6420415
Volume 51
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Lb9QwEB7R7aUX3tBAqYzECUhJYid2jsvS0oLKhV2piEPkpxaxJKvdRIL-esabB9oWkHrJIR4r8Xhsf-MZfwZ4kUQyk8qyUKG9hCx1PFQ5VWFk0QXS0mWJ8VsD55-y0xn7cJFedOe41322ex-S3MzU3q9EUPtmHVPm2ZRjT0fKEBbuwG4ai1yMYHf8_svH434CpiKJWRvA_HvF7SXoGq68nh55JUa6WXpO7sC0_-k24-T7UVOrI315hc_xhq26C7c7KErGre3cg1u2vA97w4z46wF8RSMin_W8WdSX5Pzs3Rl5i0Y9_yE3G-xkWlULVf0kiHvbUs89sXI-yes1WdqVbnyKbUmW0uALWRqy9pc4Ee0Pej2E2cnxdHIadrcxhDqlvA6djjLhTKSo4TJjglnuYkNjY0VGDRWIFETEc5WyTPPI5KlANCCMptTo3DFHH8GorEq7D4Qzl0QWgZQR6F9miZI0QZDPDeNcaBkHEPUdVOiOqtzfmLEo0GXxaitatRWotsKrreABvByqLFuejv8JH_S9XnRDdl3g1O15sUSeBPB8KMbB5iMosrRVgzLoxHtqZy_zuDWS4WsJOo4cAVYAfMt8BgFP5L1dUn6bbwi90Qf0TAkBvOpt5M9v_bMRT24k_RT2EOrlPt88Tg9gVK8a-wzhVK0Ou-FzCDuTbILPWTL-DRg6GGs
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Lb9QwELagHOil4k1KASNxAiKS2ImdYwtUu9DthV2pEgfLTy3SNlntQyr8-s7khZYCEtd4rCQzY_sbz_gzIa-zRBfaeB4b8JeY50HEpmQmTjyEQFaHInO4NTA5L0Yz_vkiv-jOca_7avc-JdnM1BhXAqh9v04ZRzblFOlIOcDC2-QOYAGJ1xbMsuN--oUHKW_Tl3_utrsA3UCVN4sjf8uQNgvP6T1y0CFGetya-D655asHZH-YuH48JN_A1vSrnW8Xm590Mv44pifge_NL3eyD02ldL0x9RQGetq1IEbEKWIv1ji79ym6xEraiS-3gga4cXeNdS9TieaxHZHb6afphFHeXJsQ2Z2ITB5sUMrjEMCd0wSX3IqSOpc7LgjkmYUGXiShNzgsrElfmEnQonWXM2TLwwB6Tvaqu_FNCBQ9Z4gHvOAlhYJEZzTLA4sJxIaTVaUSSXpPKdozieLHFQkFkgcpXrfIVKF-h8pWIyJuhy7Kl0_iX8FFvHtWNrLWCGRbpq2SZReTV0AxjAhMduvL1FmQg1kYGZpR50lpzeFsG8Z0AHBQRsWPnQQD5tndbqu_zhncbQjUkNIjI294jfn3WX3_i8L-kX5K7o-nkTJ2Nz788I_uAzkosEU_zI7K3WW39c0BAG_Oi8fhr48r8pA
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3di9QwEA96gtyL-G3PUyP4pJZrmzRJH_XO5Va9Q_AODnwI-WSFtV12u3D61zuz_ZD1VPC1mdB2Jsn8JjP5hZAXRWaEsYGnFsZLyssoU1sxm2YBQiBnoig8bg2cnIrjc_7-orzo7zldDdXuQ0qyO9OALE11e7DwEac4A4B7sMoZR2blHKlJOUDE6-QGR8-H2VpxOCzFTBU571KZf-627YyuIMyrhZK_ZUs3Tmhym9zq0SN905n7DrkW6rtkd1zEvt8jX8Du9LObreftD3oyPZrStzAOZ9_MZk-cnjXN3DaXFKBq14p0EcuIdVmv6SIs3RqrYmu6MB4emNrTFd67RB2ezbpPzifvzg6P0_4ChdSVTLZpdJlQ0WeWeWkEVzzImHuW-6AE80yBc1eZrGzJhZOZr0oFDlx5x5h3VeSRPSA7dVOHR4RKHossAPbxCkJCUVjDCsDl0nMplTN5QrJBk9r17OJ4ycVcQ5SByted8jUoX6PytUzIy7HLoqPW-Jfw_mAe3c-ylYbVFqmsVFUk5PnYDPMDkx6mDs0aZCDuRjZmlHnYWXN8WwGxngRMlBC5ZedRALm3t1vqr7MNBzeEbUhukJBXw4j49Vl__Ym9_5J-Rm5-Oproj9PTD4_JLgC1CqvF83Kf7LTLdXgCYKi1TzcD_icq_ADZ
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+Schultz+MIDI+Benchmarking+Toolbox+for+MIDI+interfaces%2C+percussion+pads%2C+and+sound+cards&rft.jtitle=Behavior+research+methods&rft.au=Schultz%2C+Benjamin+G&rft.date=2019-02-15&rft.issn=1554-3528&rft.eissn=1554-3528&rft.volume=51&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=204&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13428-018-1042-7&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon