The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox for MIDI interfaces, percussion pads, and sound cards
The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI–USB converter (or MIDI–PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and...
Saved in:
Published in | Behavior research methods Vol. 51; no. 1; pp. 204 - 234 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
New York
Springer US
15.02.2019
Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1554-3528 1554-351X 1554-3528 |
DOI | 10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI–USB converter (or MIDI–PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment
1
showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment
2
benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI–USB and MIDI–PCI interfaces. MIDI–PCI was superior to MIDI–USB, primarily because MIDI–USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment
3
tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment
4
benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion. |
---|---|
AbstractList | The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI-USB converter (or MIDI-PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI-USB and MIDI-PCI interfaces. MIDI-PCI was superior to MIDI-USB, primarily because MIDI-USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion. The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI–USB converter (or MIDI–PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI–USB and MIDI–PCI interfaces. MIDI–PCI was superior to MIDI–USB, primarily because MIDI–USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion. The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI-USB converter (or MIDI-PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI-USB and MIDI-PCI interfaces. MIDI-PCI was superior to MIDI-USB, primarily because MIDI-USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion.The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI percussion pads to collect responses, a MIDI-USB converter (or MIDI-PCI interface) to record responses on a PC and manipulate feedback, and an external MIDI sound module to generate auditory feedback. Previous studies have suggested that auditory feedback latencies can be introduced by these devices. The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox (SMIDIBT) is an open-source, Arduino-based package designed to measure the point-to-point latencies incurred by several devices used in the generation of response-triggered auditory feedback. Experiment 1 showed that MIDI messages are sent and received within 1 ms (on average) in the absence of any external MIDI device. Latencies decreased when the baud rate increased above the MIDI protocol default (31,250 bps). Experiment 2 benchmarked the latencies introduced by different MIDI-USB and MIDI-PCI interfaces. MIDI-PCI was superior to MIDI-USB, primarily because MIDI-USB is subject to USB polling. Experiment 3 tested three MIDI percussion pads. Both the audio and MIDI message latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms for all devices, and there were significant differences between percussion pads and instrument patches. Experiment 4 benchmarked four MIDI sound modules. Audio latencies were significantly greater than 1 ms, and there were significant differences between sound modules and instrument patches. These experiments suggest that millisecond accuracy might not be achievable with MIDI devices. The SMIDIBT can be used to benchmark a range of MIDI devices, thus allowing researchers to make informed decisions when choosing testing materials and to arrive at an acceptable latency at their discretion. |
Author | Schultz, Benjamin G. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Benjamin G. surname: Schultz fullname: Schultz, Benjamin G. email: ben.schultz@maastrichtuniversity.nl organization: Basic & Applied Neurodynamics Laboratory, Department of Neuropsychology & Psychopharmolcology, Maastricht University |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29667081$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9UU1vEzEUtFAq2gR-ABe0EhcObPHn2ntBKoFCpCAOhBMHy_FH4rKxU3sXAb8ep1tKiQQX23pvZvzmzRRMQgwWgCcInhPOxMuMCMWihkjUCFJc8wfgDDFGa8KwmNx7n4JpzlcQEoERfQhOcds0HAp0Br6strb6pLdD1_-sPizeLKrXNujtTqWvPmyqVYzdOn6vXExj14feJqe0zS-qvU16yNnHUO2VKQUVTJXjUE6tksmPwIlTXbaPb-8Z-Hz5djV_Xy8_vlvML5a1ZoT3tdOwEc7ANTFcNVRQyx0yBBkrGmKIEAgLyNs1o43m0LRM4FI0mhCjW0cdmYFXo-5-WO-s0Tb0SXVyn3yx8UNG5eXfneC3chO_yYZiSBErAs9vBVK8Hmzu5c5nbbtOBRuHLDHEHDYtbHGBPjuCXsUhhWJPYtQiwbi4QT29P9HdKL_3XgBoBOgUc07W3UEQlIds5ZitLNnKQ7aSFw4_4mjfq76sv5jy3X-ZeGTm8kvY2PRn6H-TfgHeOrcM |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_mex_2021_101427 crossref_primary_10_1063_5_0050999 crossref_primary_10_1109_TNSRE_2023_3321874 crossref_primary_10_1155_2022_5957708 crossref_primary_10_1155_2022_7569763 crossref_primary_10_1525_mp_2021_39_2_202 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_021_01653_y crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2020_3021523 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/s00221-011-2616-0 10.1038/s41562-016-0007 10.1214/aos/1176346577 10.3758/s13428-013-0363-9 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.10.002 10.1121/1.413218 10.1163/22134468-03002040 10.1080/01621459.1926.10502161 10.1080/00222899709603468 10.3758/BF03214280 10.1525/mp.2010.28.2.121 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.006 10.1037/a0021435 10.1016/0001-6918(64)90032-0 10.1037/a0024208 10.3758/BF03206433 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225 10.3758/s13428-015-0671-3 10.2307/3679834 10.1080/09298210802322401 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.469 10.1007/s004260100069 10.1038/249592a0 10.1037/a0037154 10.1121/1.1911293 10.1007/s004220050474 10.1177/1029864911401173 10.3758/s13428-013-0336-z 10.1016/j.humov.2009.08.002 10.3758/BF03200586 10.3758/BF03195348 10.1007/s00221-009-2115-8 10.1002/bimj.200810425 10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.205 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2 10.5281/zenodo.16178 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.07.001 10.1145/2669485.2669514 10.1145/1517664.1517735 10.1525/mp.2009.27.2.103 10.3758/BF03203375 10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.61 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author(s) 2018 Copyright Springer Nature B.V. 2019 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author(s) 2018 – notice: Copyright Springer Nature B.V. 2019 |
DBID | C6C AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 4T- 7TK K9. 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7 |
DatabaseName | Springer Nature OA Free Journals CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Docstoc Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts Docstoc MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: C6C name: Springer Nature OA Free Journals url: http://www.springeropen.com/ sourceTypes: Publisher – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1554-3528 |
EndPage | 234 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC6420415 29667081 10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Maastricht University – fundername: ; |
GroupedDBID | --- -55 -5G -BR -DZ -EM -ET -~C -~X 06D 0R~ 0VY 199 1N0 203 23N 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 30V 4.4 406 408 40E 53G 5GY 875 8TC 8UJ 95. 96X AABHQ AACDK AAHNG AAJBT AAJKR AAKPC AANZL AARTL AASML AATNV AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYQN AAYZH AAZMS ABAKF ABDZT ABECU ABFTV ABHLI ABIVO ABJNI ABJOX ABJUD ABKCH ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABPPZ ABQBU ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTMW ABXPI ACAOD ACDTI ACGFS ACHQT ACHSB ACHXU ACIWK ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACNCT ACOKC ACPIV ACPRK ACZOJ ADBBV ADHHG ADHIR ADINQ ADKNI ADKPE ADURQ ADYFF ADZKW AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEMSY AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETCA AEVLU AEXYK AFBBN AFQWF AFRAH AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGRTI AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHAVH AHBYD AHMBA AHYZX AIAKS AIGIU AIIXL AILAN AITGF AJRNO AJZVZ ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AMYQR AOCGG ARMRJ ASPBG AVWKF AXYYD AYQZM AZFZN B-. BAWUL BENPR BGNMA C6C CSCUP DDRTE DIK DNIVK DPUIP E3Z EBD EBLON EBS EIOEI EJD EMOBN ESBYG F5P FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FNLPD FRRFC GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNWQR GQ3 GQ6 GQ7 GUQSH HF~ HMJXF HRMNR HVGLF IAO IHR IKXTQ INH IPY ITC ITM IWAJR J-C JBSCW JZLTJ KOV LLZTM M2O M4Y N9A NB0 NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O93 O9G O9J OK1 P2P P9L PF- PT4 R9I ROL RPV RSV S16 S27 S3B SBS SHX SISQX SJYHP SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SV3 SZN T13 TN5 TR2 TSG TUC TUS U2A U9L UG4 UOJIU UPT UTJUX UZXMN VFIZW W48 WH7 WK8 XSW Z7R Z7S Z7W Z81 Z83 Z88 Z8N Z92 ZMTXR ZOVNA ZUP 0-V 2VQ 7X7 88E 8AO 8FI 8FJ 8G5 AAAVM AAIAL AAPKM AAYTO AAYXX ABBRH ABDBE ABFSG ABQSL ABUWG ACBXY ACKIV ACSTC ADHKG AEBTG AEKMD AEZWR AFDZB AFFNX AFHIU AFKRA AFOHR AGJBK AGQPQ AHPBZ AHSBF AHWEU AIXLP AJBLW ALSLI ARALO ATHPR AYFIA AZQEC BPHCQ BVXVI C1A CAG CCPQU CITATION COF DWQXO FINBP FSGXE FYUFA GNUQQ H13 HMCUK HZ~ H~9 IRVIT M1P M2M M2R MVM N2Q O9- OHT PADUT PHGZM PHGZT PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ S1Z SBU SCLPG UKHRP XJT XOL ABRTQ CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PJZUB PPXIY PRQQA 4T- 7TK K9. 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c537t-fc068fd0b3d7a6484e7f1d31de863d388128079b546c70d9582388dc33dc9f4f3 |
IEDL.DBID | AGYKE |
ISSN | 1554-3528 1554-351X |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:07:13 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 13:24:16 EDT 2025 Sun Jul 13 03:35:09 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:58:27 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:05:53 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:52:58 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 21 02:37:00 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | Motor timing Sensorimotor synchronization Auditory feedback Microcontrollers Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) |
Language | English |
License | Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c537t-fc068fd0b3d7a6484e7f1d31de863d388128079b546c70d9582388dc33dc9f4f3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7 |
PMID | 29667081 |
PQID | 2191857892 |
PQPubID | 976348 |
PageCount | 31 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6420415 proquest_miscellaneous_2027069092 proquest_journals_2191857892 pubmed_primary_29667081 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7 crossref_citationtrail_10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7 springer_journals_10_3758_s13428_018_1042_7 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20190215 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2019-02-15 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 2 year: 2019 text: 20190215 day: 15 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | New York |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: New York – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Behavior research methods |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Behav Res |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Behav Res Methods |
PublicationYear | 2019 |
Publisher | Springer US Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer US – name: Springer Nature B.V |
References | Collyer, Boatright-Horowitz, Hooper (CR2) 1997; 29 Maidhof, Kästner, Makkonen (CR24) 2014; 46 Schubert, D’Ausilio, Canto (CR51) 2013; 45 CR38 CR32 CR30 Thaut, Miller, Schauer (CR54) 1998; 79 Nordmark (CR31) 1968; 44 Sturges (CR53) 1926; 21 Janata, Tomic, Haberman (CR14) 2012; 141 Repp (CR40) 2005; 12 Hurley, Martens, Janata (CR11) 2014; 40 CR3 Rogers, Röhlig, Weing, Gugenheimer, Könings, Klepsch (CR48) 2014 CR6 Kaiser, Keller (CR16) 2011; 15 CR8 Aschersleben, Prinz (CR1) 1997; 29 CR44 Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, Sarkar, Core Team (CR37) 2015; 3 Repp, Su (CR47) 2013; 20 Hartigan, Hartigan (CR9) 1985; 13 Pecenka, Keller (CR33) 2011; 211 Friberg, Sundberg (CR7) 1995; 98 Repp, Knoblich (CR43) 2007; 33 Repp, Keller (CR42) 2010; 202 Sadakata, Hoppe, Brandmeyer, Timmers, Desain (CR50) 2008; 37 CR17 CR13 CR57 CR12 CR56 CR55 Jeffreys (CR15) 1961 Pfordresher, Kulpa (CR36) 2011; 37 Ravignani, Delgado, Kirby (CR39) 2016; 1 Moore (CR29) 1988; 12 Mills, van der Steen, Schultz, Keller (CR28) 2015; 3 Schultz, van Vugt (CR52) 2016; 48 Repp, London, Keller (CR45) 2012; 30 Pfordresher, Dalla Bella (CR35) 2011; 37 Repp, Marcus (CR46) 2010; 28 Marcus (CR25) 1981; 30 CR27 Hothorn, Bretz, Westfall (CR10) 2008; 50 Repp (CR41) 2010; 29 Large, Fink, Kelso (CR19) 2002; 66 Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, Iverson (CR49) 2009; 16 CR23 CR21 CR20 Pfordresher, Benitez (CR34) 2007; 26 Knörig, Wettach, Cohen (CR18) 2009 Michon (CR26) 1964; 22 Elliott, Welchman, Wing (CR4) 2009; 177 Lunney (CR22) 1974; 249 Finney (CR5) 2001; 33 A Friberg (1042_CR7) 1995; 98 M Sadakata (1042_CR50) 2008; 37 BG Schultz (1042_CR52) 2016; 48 HMW Lunney (1042_CR22) 1974; 249 JN Rouder (1042_CR49) 2009; 16 1042_CR44 A Ravignani (1042_CR39) 2016; 1 BH Repp (1042_CR42) 2010; 202 EW Large (1042_CR19) 2002; 66 T Hothorn (1042_CR10) 2008; 50 BH Repp (1042_CR46) 2010; 28 1042_CR6 BH Repp (1042_CR40) 2005; 12 1042_CR8 PQ Pfordresher (1042_CR35) 2011; 37 PF Mills (1042_CR28) 2015; 3 1042_CR3 BK Hurley (1042_CR11) 2014; 40 1042_CR12 1042_CR56 1042_CR13 1042_CR57 1042_CR55 1042_CR17 PQ Pfordresher (1042_CR36) 2011; 37 C Maidhof (1042_CR24) 2014; 46 MT Elliott (1042_CR4) 2009; 177 JO Nordmark (1042_CR31) 1968; 44 H Jeffreys (1042_CR15) 1961 BH Repp (1042_CR45) 2012; 30 1042_CR20 CE Collyer (1042_CR2) 1997; 29 JA Michon (1042_CR26) 1964; 22 MH Thaut (1042_CR54) 1998; 79 1042_CR23 PQ Pfordresher (1042_CR34) 2007; 26 1042_CR21 BH Repp (1042_CR47) 2013; 20 1042_CR27 P Janata (1042_CR14) 2012; 141 BH Repp (1042_CR41) 2010; 29 G Aschersleben (1042_CR1) 1997; 29 SM Marcus (1042_CR25) 1981; 30 N Pecenka (1042_CR33) 2011; 211 BH Repp (1042_CR43) 2007; 33 K Rogers (1042_CR48) 2014 TW Schubert (1042_CR51) 2013; 45 1042_CR30 R Kaiser (1042_CR16) 2011; 15 1042_CR32 SA Finney (1042_CR5) 2001; 33 JA Hartigan (1042_CR9) 1985; 13 FR Moore (1042_CR29) 1988; 12 1042_CR38 J Pinheiro (1042_CR37) 2015; 3 A Knörig (1042_CR18) 2009 HA Sturges (1042_CR53) 1926; 21 |
References_xml | – volume: 211 start-page: 505 year: 2011 end-page: 515 ident: CR33 article-title: The role of temporal prediction abilities in interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2616-0 – volume: 1 start-page: 0007 year: 2016 ident: CR39 article-title: Musical evolution in the lab exhibits rhythmic universals publication-title: Nature Human Behaviour doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0007 – volume: 13 start-page: 70 year: 1985 end-page: 84 ident: CR9 article-title: The dip test of unimodality publication-title: Annals of Statistics doi: 10.1214/aos/1176346577 – volume: 46 start-page: 185 year: 2014 end-page: 195 ident: CR24 article-title: Combining EEG, MIDI, and motion capture techniques for investigating musical performance publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0363-9 – ident: CR12 – volume: 177 start-page: 250 year: 2009 end-page: 257 ident: CR4 article-title: MatTAP: A MATLAB toolbox for the control and analysis of movement synchronisation experiments publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.10.002 – volume: 98 start-page: 2524 year: 1995 end-page: 2531 ident: CR7 article-title: Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronous sequence publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America doi: 10.1121/1.413218 – ident: CR8 – volume: 3 start-page: 13 year: 2015 end-page: 31 ident: CR28 article-title: Individual differences in temporal anticipation and adaptation during sensorimotor synchronization publication-title: Timing & Time Perception doi: 10.1163/22134468-03002040 – volume: 21 start-page: 65 year: 1926 end-page: 66 ident: CR53 article-title: The choice of a class interval publication-title: Journal of the American Statistical Association doi: 10.1080/01621459.1926.10502161 – volume: 29 start-page: 35 year: 1997 end-page: 46 ident: CR1 article-title: Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222899709603468 – ident: CR21 – volume: 30 start-page: 247 year: 1981 end-page: 256 ident: CR25 article-title: Acoustic determinants of perceptual center (P-center) location publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03214280 – volume: 28 start-page: 121 year: 2010 end-page: 134 ident: CR46 article-title: No sustained sound illusion in rhythmic sequences publication-title: Music Perception doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.2.121 – start-page: 149 year: 2014 end-page: 158 ident: CR48 article-title: Piano: Faster piano learning with interactive projection – start-page: 351 year: 2009 end-page: 358 ident: CR18 article-title: Fritzing: A tool for advancing electronic prototyping for designers – volume: 26 start-page: 742 year: 2007 end-page: 756 ident: CR34 article-title: Temporal coordination between actions and sound during sequence production publication-title: Human Movement Science doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.006 – volume: 37 start-page: 949 year: 2011 end-page: 967 ident: CR36 article-title: The dynamics of disruption from altered auditory feedback: Further evidence for a dissociation of sequencing and timing publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/a0021435 – volume: 22 start-page: 441 year: 1964 end-page: 450 ident: CR26 article-title: Studies on subjective duration 1: Differential sensitivity on the perception of repeated temporal intervals publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(64)90032-0 – ident: CR57 – ident: CR32 – volume: 141 start-page: 54 year: 2012 end-page: 75 ident: CR14 article-title: Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/a0024208 – year: 1961 ident: CR15 – volume: 12 start-page: 969 year: 2005 end-page: 992 ident: CR40 article-title: Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03206433 – volume: 16 start-page: 225 year: 2009 end-page: 237 ident: CR49 article-title: Bayesian tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225 – volume: 48 start-page: 1591 year: 2016 end-page: 1607 ident: CR52 article-title: Tap Arduino: An Arduino microcontroller for low-latency auditory feedback in sensorimotor synchronization experiments publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0671-3 – volume: 12 start-page: 19 year: 1988 end-page: 28 ident: CR29 article-title: The dysfunctions of MIDI publication-title: Computer Music Journal doi: 10.2307/3679834 – volume: 37 start-page: 207 year: 2008 end-page: 220 ident: CR50 article-title: Real-time visual feedback for learning to perform short rhythms with expressive variations in timing and loudness publication-title: Journal of New Music Research doi: 10.1080/09298210802322401 – volume: 33 start-page: 469 year: 2007 end-page: 482 ident: CR43 article-title: Toward a psychophysics of agency: Detecting gain and loss of control over auditory action effects publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.469 – volume: 66 start-page: 3 year: 2002 end-page: 17 ident: CR19 article-title: Tracking simple and complex sequences publication-title: Psychological Research doi: 10.1007/s004260100069 – volume: 249 start-page: 592 year: 1974 ident: CR22 article-title: Time as heard in speech and music publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/249592a0 – ident: CR30 – volume: 3 start-page: 1 year: 2015 end-page: 120 ident: CR37 article-title: nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models publication-title: R package version – ident: CR6 – volume: 40 start-page: 1679 year: 2014 end-page: 1696 ident: CR11 article-title: Spontaneous sensorimotor coupling with multipart music publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/a0037154 – volume: 44 start-page: 1533 year: 1968 end-page: 1540 ident: CR31 article-title: Mechanisms of frequency discrimination publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America doi: 10.1121/1.1911293 – ident: CR56 – ident: CR27 – ident: CR23 – volume: 79 start-page: 241 year: 1998 end-page: 250 ident: CR54 article-title: Multiple synchronization strategies in rhythmic sensorimotor tasks: Phase vs. period correction publication-title: Biological Cybernetics doi: 10.1007/s004220050474 – ident: CR44 – ident: CR3 – ident: CR38 – volume: 15 start-page: 270 year: 2011 end-page: 287 ident: CR16 article-title: Music’s impact on the visual perception of emotional dyadic interactions publication-title: Musicae Scientiae doi: 10.1177/1029864911401173 – ident: CR17 – ident: CR13 – volume: 45 start-page: 1332 year: 2013 end-page: 1346 ident: CR51 article-title: Using Arduino microcontroller boards to measure response latencies publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0336-z – volume: 29 start-page: 200 year: 2010 end-page: 213 ident: CR41 article-title: Sensorimotor synchronization and perception of timing: Effects of music training and task experience publication-title: Human Movement Science doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.08.002 – volume: 29 start-page: 346 year: 1997 end-page: 352 ident: CR2 article-title: A motor timing experiment implemented using a musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) approach publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03200586 – ident: CR55 – volume: 33 start-page: 65 year: 2001 end-page: 72 ident: CR5 article-title: FTAP: A Linux-based program for tapping and music experiments publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03195348 – volume: 202 start-page: 101 year: 2010 end-page: 110 ident: CR42 article-title: Self versus other in piano performance: Detectability of timing perturbations depends on personal playing style publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2115-8 – volume: 50 start-page: 346 year: 2008 end-page: 363 ident: CR10 article-title: Simultaneous inference in general parametric models publication-title: Biometrical Journal doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425 – volume: 30 start-page: 205 year: 2012 end-page: 223 ident: CR45 article-title: Distortions in reproduction of two-interval rhythms: When the “attractor ratio” is not exactly 1:2 publication-title: Music Perception doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.205 – ident: CR20 – volume: 20 start-page: 403 year: 2013 end-page: 452 ident: CR47 article-title: Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012) publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2 – volume: 37 start-page: 566 year: 2011 end-page: 579 ident: CR35 article-title: Delayed auditory feedback and movement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – ident: 1042_CR56 doi: 10.5281/zenodo.16178 – volume: 15 start-page: 270 year: 2011 ident: 1042_CR16 publication-title: Musicae Scientiae doi: 10.1177/1029864911401173 – volume: 20 start-page: 403 year: 2013 ident: 1042_CR47 publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2 – volume: 12 start-page: 969 year: 2005 ident: 1042_CR40 publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03206433 – volume: 66 start-page: 3 year: 2002 ident: 1042_CR19 publication-title: Psychological Research doi: 10.1007/s004260100069 – ident: 1042_CR32 – volume: 21 start-page: 65 year: 1926 ident: 1042_CR53 publication-title: Journal of the American Statistical Association doi: 10.1080/01621459.1926.10502161 – ident: 1042_CR55 – ident: 1042_CR57 doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.07.001 – ident: 1042_CR13 – start-page: 149 volume-title: Proceedings of the Ninth ACM International Conference on Interactive Tabletops and Surfaces year: 2014 ident: 1042_CR48 doi: 10.1145/2669485.2669514 – volume: 28 start-page: 121 year: 2010 ident: 1042_CR46 publication-title: Music Perception doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.28.2.121 – volume: 50 start-page: 346 year: 2008 ident: 1042_CR10 publication-title: Biometrical Journal doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425 – volume: 30 start-page: 247 year: 1981 ident: 1042_CR25 publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03214280 – volume: 33 start-page: 469 year: 2007 ident: 1042_CR43 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.469 – ident: 1042_CR8 – volume: 46 start-page: 185 year: 2014 ident: 1042_CR24 publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0363-9 – volume: 3 start-page: 1 year: 2015 ident: 1042_CR37 publication-title: R package version – ident: 1042_CR38 – volume: 48 start-page: 1591 year: 2016 ident: 1042_CR52 publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0671-3 – start-page: 351 volume-title: Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Tangible and Embedded Interaction year: 2009 ident: 1042_CR18 doi: 10.1145/1517664.1517735 – volume: 37 start-page: 207 year: 2008 ident: 1042_CR50 publication-title: Journal of New Music Research doi: 10.1080/09298210802322401 – volume: 249 start-page: 592 year: 1974 ident: 1042_CR22 publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/249592a0 – volume: 211 start-page: 505 year: 2011 ident: 1042_CR33 publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2616-0 – volume: 16 start-page: 225 year: 2009 ident: 1042_CR49 publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225 – volume: 202 start-page: 101 year: 2010 ident: 1042_CR42 publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2115-8 – ident: 1042_CR21 doi: 10.1525/mp.2009.27.2.103 – ident: 1042_CR3 – volume: 30 start-page: 205 year: 2012 ident: 1042_CR45 publication-title: Music Perception doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.205 – volume: 79 start-page: 241 year: 1998 ident: 1042_CR54 publication-title: Biological Cybernetics doi: 10.1007/s004220050474 – volume: 98 start-page: 2524 year: 1995 ident: 1042_CR7 publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America doi: 10.1121/1.413218 – ident: 1042_CR20 – volume: 37 start-page: 566 year: 2011 ident: 1042_CR35 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 29 start-page: 346 year: 1997 ident: 1042_CR2 publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03200586 – ident: 1042_CR6 – volume: 45 start-page: 1332 year: 2013 ident: 1042_CR51 publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0336-z – volume: 177 start-page: 250 year: 2009 ident: 1042_CR4 publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.10.002 – ident: 1042_CR30 – volume: 37 start-page: 949 year: 2011 ident: 1042_CR36 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/a0021435 – ident: 1042_CR27 – volume: 141 start-page: 54 year: 2012 ident: 1042_CR14 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/a0024208 – ident: 1042_CR17 doi: 10.3758/BF03203375 – volume: 13 start-page: 70 year: 1985 ident: 1042_CR9 publication-title: Annals of Statistics doi: 10.1214/aos/1176346577 – ident: 1042_CR23 – volume: 29 start-page: 35 year: 1997 ident: 1042_CR1 publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222899709603468 – ident: 1042_CR44 doi: 10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.61 – volume: 44 start-page: 1533 year: 1968 ident: 1042_CR31 publication-title: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America doi: 10.1121/1.1911293 – volume: 26 start-page: 742 year: 2007 ident: 1042_CR34 publication-title: Human Movement Science doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.006 – volume: 12 start-page: 19 year: 1988 ident: 1042_CR29 publication-title: Computer Music Journal doi: 10.2307/3679834 – volume: 40 start-page: 1679 year: 2014 ident: 1042_CR11 publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance doi: 10.1037/a0037154 – volume: 29 start-page: 200 year: 2010 ident: 1042_CR41 publication-title: Human Movement Science doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.08.002 – volume: 3 start-page: 13 year: 2015 ident: 1042_CR28 publication-title: Timing & Time Perception doi: 10.1163/22134468-03002040 – ident: 1042_CR12 – volume-title: Theory of probability year: 1961 ident: 1042_CR15 – volume: 33 start-page: 65 year: 2001 ident: 1042_CR5 publication-title: Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers doi: 10.3758/BF03195348 – volume: 22 start-page: 441 year: 1964 ident: 1042_CR26 publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(64)90032-0 – volume: 1 start-page: 0007 year: 2016 ident: 1042_CR39 publication-title: Nature Human Behaviour doi: 10.1038/s41562-016-0007 |
SSID | ssj0038214 |
Score | 2.329208 |
Snippet | The Musical Instrument Digital Interface (MIDI) was readily adopted for auditory sensorimotor synchronization experiments. These experiments typically use MIDI... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref springer |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 204 |
SubjectTerms | Auditory Perception Auditory stimuli Behavioral Research - instrumentation Behavioral Science and Psychology Benchmarking Cognitive Psychology Experiments Feedback Feedback, Sensory Fingers Humans Interfaces Latency Musical instruments Percussion Psychology Sensorimotor system Sound Sound boards Synchronization |
Title | The Schultz MIDI Benchmarking Toolbox for MIDI interfaces, percussion pads, and sound cards |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13428-018-1042-7 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29667081 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2191857892 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2027069092 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6420415 |
Volume | 51 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Lb9QwEB7R7aUX3tBAqYzECUhJYid2jsvS0oLKhV2piEPkpxaxJKvdRIL-esabB9oWkHrJIR4r8Xhsf-MZfwZ4kUQyk8qyUKG9hCx1PFQ5VWFk0QXS0mWJ8VsD55-y0xn7cJFedOe41322ex-S3MzU3q9EUPtmHVPm2ZRjT0fKEBbuwG4ai1yMYHf8_svH434CpiKJWRvA_HvF7SXoGq68nh55JUa6WXpO7sC0_-k24-T7UVOrI315hc_xhq26C7c7KErGre3cg1u2vA97w4z46wF8RSMin_W8WdSX5Pzs3Rl5i0Y9_yE3G-xkWlULVf0kiHvbUs89sXI-yes1WdqVbnyKbUmW0uALWRqy9pc4Ee0Pej2E2cnxdHIadrcxhDqlvA6djjLhTKSo4TJjglnuYkNjY0VGDRWIFETEc5WyTPPI5KlANCCMptTo3DFHH8GorEq7D4Qzl0QWgZQR6F9miZI0QZDPDeNcaBkHEPUdVOiOqtzfmLEo0GXxaitatRWotsKrreABvByqLFuejv8JH_S9XnRDdl3g1O15sUSeBPB8KMbB5iMosrRVgzLoxHtqZy_zuDWS4WsJOo4cAVYAfMt8BgFP5L1dUn6bbwi90Qf0TAkBvOpt5M9v_bMRT24k_RT2EOrlPt88Tg9gVK8a-wzhVK0Ou-FzCDuTbILPWTL-DRg6GGs |
linkProvider | Springer Nature |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1Lb9QwELagHOil4k1KASNxAiKS2ImdYwtUu9DthV2pEgfLTy3SNlntQyr8-s7khZYCEtd4rCQzY_sbz_gzIa-zRBfaeB4b8JeY50HEpmQmTjyEQFaHInO4NTA5L0Yz_vkiv-jOca_7avc-JdnM1BhXAqh9v04ZRzblFOlIOcDC2-QOYAGJ1xbMsuN--oUHKW_Tl3_utrsA3UCVN4sjf8uQNgvP6T1y0CFGetya-D655asHZH-YuH48JN_A1vSrnW8Xm590Mv44pifge_NL3eyD02ldL0x9RQGetq1IEbEKWIv1ji79ym6xEraiS-3gga4cXeNdS9TieaxHZHb6afphFHeXJsQ2Z2ITB5sUMrjEMCd0wSX3IqSOpc7LgjkmYUGXiShNzgsrElfmEnQonWXM2TLwwB6Tvaqu_FNCBQ9Z4gHvOAlhYJEZzTLA4sJxIaTVaUSSXpPKdozieLHFQkFkgcpXrfIVKF-h8pWIyJuhy7Kl0_iX8FFvHtWNrLWCGRbpq2SZReTV0AxjAhMduvL1FmQg1kYGZpR50lpzeFsG8Z0AHBQRsWPnQQD5tndbqu_zhncbQjUkNIjI294jfn3WX3_i8L-kX5K7o-nkTJ2Nz788I_uAzkosEU_zI7K3WW39c0BAG_Oi8fhr48r8pA |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3di9QwEA96gtyL-G3PUyP4pJZrmzRJH_XO5Va9Q_AODnwI-WSFtV12u3D61zuz_ZD1VPC1mdB2Jsn8JjP5hZAXRWaEsYGnFsZLyssoU1sxm2YBQiBnoig8bg2cnIrjc_7-orzo7zldDdXuQ0qyO9OALE11e7DwEac4A4B7sMoZR2blHKlJOUDE6-QGR8-H2VpxOCzFTBU571KZf-627YyuIMyrhZK_ZUs3Tmhym9zq0SN905n7DrkW6rtkd1zEvt8jX8Du9LObreftD3oyPZrStzAOZ9_MZk-cnjXN3DaXFKBq14p0EcuIdVmv6SIs3RqrYmu6MB4emNrTFd67RB2ezbpPzifvzg6P0_4ChdSVTLZpdJlQ0WeWeWkEVzzImHuW-6AE80yBc1eZrGzJhZOZr0oFDlx5x5h3VeSRPSA7dVOHR4RKHossAPbxCkJCUVjDCsDl0nMplTN5QrJBk9r17OJ4ycVcQ5SByted8jUoX6PytUzIy7HLoqPW-Jfw_mAe3c-ylYbVFqmsVFUk5PnYDPMDkx6mDs0aZCDuRjZmlHnYWXN8WwGxngRMlBC5ZedRALm3t1vqr7MNBzeEbUhukJBXw4j49Vl__Ym9_5J-Rm5-Oproj9PTD4_JLgC1CqvF83Kf7LTLdXgCYKi1TzcD_icq_ADZ |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+Schultz+MIDI+Benchmarking+Toolbox+for+MIDI+interfaces%2C+percussion+pads%2C+and+sound+cards&rft.jtitle=Behavior+research+methods&rft.au=Schultz%2C+Benjamin+G&rft.date=2019-02-15&rft.issn=1554-3528&rft.eissn=1554-3528&rft.volume=51&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=204&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13428-018-1042-7&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon |