Contribution of motor representations to action verb processing

•fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study, hand- and foot-related verbs were judged in a dual-task context.•Results evidenced a verb meaning – response effector compatibility effect.•In...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inCognition Vol. 134; no. Jan; pp. 174 - 184
Main Authors Andres, Michael, Finocchiaro, Chiara, Buiatti, Marco, Piazza, Manuela
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Netherlands Elsevier B.V 01.01.2015
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0010-0277
1873-7838
1873-7838
DOI10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004

Cover

Loading…
Abstract •fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study, hand- and foot-related verbs were judged in a dual-task context.•Results evidenced a verb meaning – response effector compatibility effect.•Interference of the dual task challenges post-conceptual accounts. Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection.
AbstractList Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection.Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection.
Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. All rights reserved, Elsevier
Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection.
•fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study, hand- and foot-related verbs were judged in a dual-task context.•Results evidenced a verb meaning – response effector compatibility effect.•Interference of the dual task challenges post-conceptual accounts. Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection.
Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. [Copyright Elsevier B.V.]
Author Buiatti, Marco
Piazza, Manuela
Finocchiaro, Chiara
Andres, Michael
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Michael
  surname: Andres
  fullname: Andres, Michael
  email: andres.meh@gmail.com
  organization: Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Chiara
  surname: Finocchiaro
  fullname: Finocchiaro, Chiara
  organization: Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Marco
  surname: Buiatti
  fullname: Buiatti, Marco
  organization: Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Manuela
  surname: Piazza
  fullname: Piazza, Manuela
  organization: Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25460390$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqNkU9PGzEUxC0EgkD7Fdo99rLp859d24eqQlFpKyFxgbPl9b5FjhI7tR2kfnu8DXDoJZwsjX8zT5q5JKchBiTkM4UlBdp_XS9dfAy--BiWDKio6hJAnJAFVZK3UnF1ShYAFFpgUl6Qy5zXUAkm1Tm5YJ3ogWtYkO-rGEryw36OauLUbGOJqUm4S5gxFDvruSmxse4f8oRpaHYpOszZh8cP5Gyym4wfX94r8nDz4371q729-_l7dX3buo7q0oqeSmGBaqqYAzZ1SijewaSYHSynvB_cMLhuGihjSo9cI1B02iqnxio6fkW-HHLr6T97zMVsfXa42diAcZ8NlVwJyakSx9G-B2BaafkOVEiAHlT3DpRp3dXeeUU_vaD7YYuj2SW_temveW29At8OgEsx54STcf7QdEnWbwwFM69s1uZtZTOvPH_UDatf_ud_PXHceX1wYt3qyWMy2XkMDkef0BUzRn804xn_Z8WA
CODEN CGTNAU
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4706_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_11_021
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2017_01_010
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_newideapsych_2019_100777
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2022_10_022
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0270352
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2024_12_018
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_022_02918_1
crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2017_1322093
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00072
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11406_023_00693_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_09_022
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_17326_w
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00223
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10670_021_00395_9
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2023_e13426
crossref_primary_10_17711_SM_0185_3325_2019_039
crossref_primary_10_1111_papq_12243
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2015_07_003
crossref_primary_10_1027_1618_3169_a000507
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_016_0776_z
crossref_primary_10_1080_02643294_2020_1732319
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2018_02_003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_plrev_2018_04_005
crossref_primary_10_1017_langcog_2023_22
crossref_primary_10_1177_09593543231170854
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_015_0870_z
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_52426_9
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11245_020_09723_0
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2017_04_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2021_103428
Cites_doi 10.1007/s00221-009-1864-8
10.1073/pnas.0805903105
10.1006/nimg.2001.0835
10.1037/h0073262
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x
10.1007/s00426-011-0371-2
10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060
10.1080/02643294.2011.570326
10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00022-4
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.017
10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90415-6
10.1038/nrn1706
10.1177/0956797609354072
10.1371/journal.pone.0035403
10.1162/0898929053124965
10.3758/BF03210951
10.1002/hbm.10157
10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.282
10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.015
10.1006/nimg.2001.0832
10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9
10.1093/cercor/bhn217
10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.015
10.1080/17470210701625667
10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220
10.1162/0898929054021102
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.123
10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607
10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90110-X
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.007
10.1073/pnas.1000674107
10.1371/journal.pone.0004508
10.3758/BF03194745
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.011
10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03126.x
10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.013
10.1006/brln.2000.2390
10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06013.x
10.1162/0898929054021111
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.006
10.1016/j.bandl.2007.03.004
10.1016/S0165-0270(97)02242-5
10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00179-3
10.1371/journal.pbio.0030037
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2014 Elsevier B.V.
Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Copyright_xml – notice: 2014 Elsevier B.V.
– notice: Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
7TK
8BJ
FQK
JBE
7T9
DOI 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
MEDLINE
Neurosciences Abstracts

Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1873-7838
EndPage 184
ExternalDocumentID 25460390
10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004
S0010027714001966
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
--K
--M
--Z
-DZ
-~X
.~1
0R~
186
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29F
3EH
4.4
41~
457
4G.
53G
5GY
5VS
6J9
6PF
7-5
71M
8P~
9JM
9JO
AABNK
AACTN
AADFP
AADPK
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFJI
AAGJA
AAGUQ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAWTL
AAXLA
AAXUO
ABCQJ
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABLJU
ABMAC
ABMMH
ABOYX
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACIUM
ACKIV
ACNCT
ACPRK
ACRLP
ACXNI
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADIYS
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AETEA
AFFNX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AFXIZ
AFYLN
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGWIK
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
AKYCK
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
AOMHK
ASPBG
AVARZ
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
CS3
DU5
EBS
EFJIC
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FD6
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
G8K
GBLVA
HF~
HMQ
HMW
HVGLF
HZ~
IHE
J1W
K-O
KOM
LPU
M2V
M3V
M41
MO0
MOBAO
MVM
N9A
NHB
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OHT
OKEIE
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
PRBVW
Q38
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SCC
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SNS
SPCBC
SPS
SSB
SSN
SSO
SSY
SSZ
T5K
TN5
UBW
UPT
UQL
WH7
WUQ
XFK
XIH
XJT
XKC
XOL
XPP
YYP
YZZ
ZA5
ZKB
ZMT
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABDPE
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADMHG
ADVLN
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
SSH
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
7TK
8BJ
FQK
JBE
7T9
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c519t-46174a019182c02f5848350f82aba3136bcbbc5fb12289d39e01ec9a8c8dfb1c3
IEDL.DBID .~1
ISSN 0010-0277
1873-7838
IngestDate Thu Jul 10 19:16:35 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 23:39:41 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 09:52:31 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 09:06:41 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 07:00:58 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:29:11 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:00:53 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:18:56 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue Jan
Keywords Concept
Embodied cognition
Language
Body
Dual task
Sensorimotor
Language English
License Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c519t-46174a019182c02f5848350f82aba3136bcbbc5fb12289d39e01ec9a8c8dfb1c3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink http://hdl.handle.net/2078.1/156434
PMID 25460390
PQID 1629958383
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 11
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_1738473184
proquest_miscellaneous_1660029897
proquest_miscellaneous_1647006085
proquest_miscellaneous_1629958383
pubmed_primary_25460390
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate January 2015
2015-01-00
2015-Jan
20150101
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2015-01-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 01
  year: 2015
  text: January 2015
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace Netherlands
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Netherlands
PublicationTitle Cognition
PublicationTitleAlternate Cognition
PublicationYear 2015
Publisher Elsevier B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier B.V
References Sato, Mengarelli, Riggio, Gallese, Buccino (b0210) 2008; 105
Nazir, Boulenger, Roy, Silber, Jeannerod, Paulignan (b0135) 2008; 61
de Groot (b0055) 1989; 15
Meister, Boroojerdi, Foltys, Sparing, Huber, Töpper (b0125) 2003; 41
Dalla Volta, Gianelli, Campione, Gentilucci (b0050) 2009; 196
Boulenger, Roy, Paulignan, Deprez, Jeannerod, Nazir (b0035) 2006; 18
Binder, Westbury, McKiernan, Possing, Medler (b0020) 2005; 17
Telford (b0225) 1931; 14
Hauk, Shtyrov, Pulvermüller (b0080) 2008; 102
Pashler (b0160) 1994; 116
Hauk, Pulvermüller (b0075) 2004; 21
Marino, Gough, Gallese, Riggio, Buccino (b0115) 2013; 77
Kroll, Merves (b0100) 1986; 12
Papeo, Vallesi, Isaja, Rumiati (b0155) 2009; 4
Rizzolatti, Luppino, Matelli (b0195) 1998; 106
Postle, McMahon, Ashton, Meredith, de Zubicaray (b0170) 2008; 43
Loftus, Masson (b0110) 1994; 1
Whaley (b0245) 1978; 17
Boulenger, Silber, Roy, Paulignan, Jeannerod, Nazir (b0045) 2008; 102
Boulenger, Shtyrov, Pulvermüller (b0040) 2011; 59
Papeo, Negri, Zadini, Rumiati (b0150) 2010; 27
Willems, Hagoort, Casasanto (b0255) 2010; 21
Aziz-Zadeh, Wilson, Rizzolatti, Iacoboni (b0010) 2006; 16
Pulvermüller (b0175) 2005; 6
Boulenger, Mechtouff, Thobois, Broussolle, Jeannerod, Nazir (b0030) 2008; 46
James (b0090) 1975; 104
van Elk, van Schie, Zwaan, Bekkering (b0240) 2010; 50
Gaskell, Quinlan, Tamminen, Cleland (b0065) 2008; 137
Hauk, Johnsrude, Pulvermüller (b0070) 2004; 41
Pulvermüller, Hauk, Nikulin, Ilmoniemi (b0185) 2005; 21
Jeannerod (b0095) 2001; 14
Matelli, Luppino (b0120) 2001; 14
Sigman, Dehaene (b0220) 2005; 3
Arévalo, Baldo, Dronkers (b0005) 2010
Willems, Hagoort (b0250) 2007; 101
Perea, Rosa (b0165) 2002; 64
Rothwell (b0200) 1997; 74
Pulvermüller, Härle, Hummel (b0180) 2001; 78
Tomasino, Fink, Sparing, Dafotakis, Weiss (b0235) 2008; 46
Rubin (b0205) 1980; 19
Mirabella, Iaconelli, Spadacenta, Federico, Gallese (b0130) 2012; 7
Boulenger, Hauk, Pulvermüller (b0025) 2009; 19
Forstmann, Dutilh, Brown, Neumann, von Cramon, Ridderinkhof (b0060) 2008; 105
Shtyrov, Hauk, Pulvermüller (b0215) 2004; 19
Laudanna, Thornton, Brown, Burani, Marconi (b0105) 1995; Vol. I
Bedny, Caramazza (b0015) 2011; 1224
Paivio (b0145) 1986
Tettamanti, Buccino, Saccuman, Gallese, Danna, Scifo (b0230) 2005; 17
Pulvermüller, Shtyrov, Ilmoniemi (b0190) 2005; 17
Hesselman, Flandin, Dehaene (b0085) 2011; 56
Neubert, Mars, Buch, Olivier, Rushworth (b0140) 2010; 107
Loftus (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0110) 1994; 1
Sigman (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0220) 2005; 3
Hauk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0080) 2008; 102
Pashler (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0160) 1994; 116
Laudanna (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0105) 1995; Vol. I
Meister (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0125) 2003; 41
Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0045) 2008; 102
Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0175) 2005; 6
Neubert (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0140) 2010; 107
Sato (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0210) 2008; 105
Binder (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0020) 2005; 17
Hesselman (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0085) 2011; 56
Marino (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0115) 2013; 77
Paivio (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0145) 1986
Hauk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0070) 2004; 41
Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0030) 2008; 46
Papeo (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0155) 2009; 4
Tomasino (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0235) 2008; 46
Perea (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0165) 2002; 64
Shtyrov (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0215) 2004; 19
Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0040) 2011; 59
Rothwell (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0200) 1997; 74
van Elk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0240) 2010; 50
Hauk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0075) 2004; 21
Postle (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0170) 2008; 43
Willems (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0250) 2007; 101
Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0185) 2005; 21
Arévalo (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0005) 2010
Whaley (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0245) 1978; 17
Forstmann (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0060) 2008; 105
Dalla Volta (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0050) 2009; 196
Papeo (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0150) 2010; 27
Matelli (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0120) 2001; 14
Nazir (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0135) 2008; 61
Jeannerod (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0095) 2001; 14
de Groot (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0055) 1989; 15
Gaskell (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0065) 2008; 137
Rizzolatti (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0195) 1998; 106
James (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0090) 1975; 104
Kroll (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0100) 1986; 12
Aziz-Zadeh (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0010) 2006; 16
Mirabella (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0130) 2012; 7
Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0190) 2005; 17
Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0180) 2001; 78
Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0035) 2006; 18
Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0025) 2009; 19
Bedny (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0015) 2011; 1224
Tettamanti (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0230) 2005; 17
Rubin (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0205) 1980; 19
Willems (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0255) 2010; 21
Telford (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0225) 1931; 14
References_xml – year: 1986
  ident: b0145
  article-title: Mental representations – A dual coding approach
– volume: 21
  start-page: 191
  year: 2004
  end-page: 201
  ident: b0075
  article-title: Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto-central cortex
  publication-title: Human Brain Mapping
– volume: 19
  start-page: 736
  year: 1980
  end-page: 755
  ident: b0205
  article-title: 51 properties of 125 words: A unit analysis of verbal behaviour
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
– volume: 16
  start-page: 1818
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1823
  ident: b0010
  article-title: Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions
  publication-title: Current Biology
– volume: 1224
  start-page: 81
  year: 2011
  end-page: 95
  ident: b0015
  article-title: Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: The case from action verbs
  publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
– volume: 17
  start-page: 884
  year: 2005
  end-page: 892
  ident: b0190
  article-title: Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 105
  start-page: 83
  year: 2008
  end-page: 90
  ident: b0210
  article-title: Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing
  publication-title: Brain and Language
– volume: 77
  start-page: 64
  year: 2013
  end-page: 73
  ident: b0115
  article-title: How the motor system handles nouns: A behavioral study
  publication-title: Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung
– volume: 78
  start-page: 143
  year: 2001
  end-page: 168
  ident: b0180
  article-title: Walking or talking? Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing
  publication-title: Brain and Language
– volume: 61
  start-page: 933
  year: 2008
  end-page: 943
  ident: b0135
  article-title: Language-induced motor perturbations during the execution of a reaching movement
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove)
– volume: 106
  start-page: 283
  year: 1998
  end-page: 296
  ident: b0195
  article-title: The organization of the cortical motor system: New concepts
  publication-title: Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 102
  start-page: 50
  year: 2008
  end-page: 58
  ident: b0080
  article-title: The time course of action and action-word comprehension in the human brain as revealed by neurophysiology
  publication-title: Journal of Physiology, Paris
– volume: 56
  start-page: 1608
  year: 2011
  end-page: 1621
  ident: b0085
  article-title: Probing the cortical network underlying the psychological refractory period: A combined EEG-fMRI study
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 17
  start-page: 143
  year: 1978
  end-page: 154
  ident: b0245
  article-title: Word-nonword classification times
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
– volume: 6
  start-page: 576
  year: 2005
  end-page: 582
  ident: b0175
  article-title: Brain mechanisms linking language and action
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
– year: 2010
  ident: b0005
  article-title: What do brain lesions tell us about theories of embodied semantics and the human mirror neuron system?
  publication-title: Cortex
– volume: 105
  start-page: 17538
  year: 2008
  end-page: 17542
  ident: b0060
  article-title: Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
– volume: 104
  start-page: 130
  year: 1975
  end-page: 136
  ident: b0090
  article-title: The role of semantic information in lexical decisions
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 17
  start-page: 905
  year: 2005
  end-page: 917
  ident: b0020
  article-title: Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract words
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 14
  start-page: S103
  year: 2001
  end-page: 109
  ident: b0095
  article-title: Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 1
  start-page: 476
  year: 1994
  end-page: 490
  ident: b0110
  article-title: Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1083
  year: 2004
  end-page: 1092
  ident: b0215
  article-title: Distributed neuronal networks for encoding category-specific semantic information: The mismatch negativity to action words
  publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 107
  start-page: 13240
  year: 2010
  end-page: 13245
  ident: b0140
  article-title: Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
– volume: 46
  start-page: 743
  year: 2008
  end-page: 756
  ident: b0030
  article-title: Word processing in Parkinson’s disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 101
  start-page: 278
  year: 2007
  end-page: 289
  ident: b0250
  article-title: Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review
  publication-title: Brain and Language
– volume: 12
  start-page: 92
  year: 1986
  end-page: 107
  ident: b0100
  article-title: Lexical access for concrete and abstract words
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
– volume: Vol. I
  start-page: 103
  year: 1995
  end-page: 109
  ident: b0105
  article-title: Un corpus dell’italiano scritto contemporaneo dalla parte del ricevente
  publication-title: III Giornate internazionali di Analisi Statistica dei Dati Testuali
– volume: 116
  start-page: 220
  year: 1994
  end-page: 244
  ident: b0160
  article-title: Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
– volume: 14
  start-page: 1
  year: 1931
  end-page: 36
  ident: b0225
  article-title: The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
– volume: 41
  start-page: 301
  year: 2004
  end-page: 307
  ident: b0070
  article-title: Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex
  publication-title: Neuron
– volume: 46
  start-page: 1915
  year: 2008
  end-page: 1926
  ident: b0235
  article-title: Action verbs and the primary motor cortex: A comparative TMS study of silent reading, frequency judgments, and motor imagery
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 15
  start-page: 824
  year: 1989
  end-page: 845
  ident: b0055
  article-title: Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
– volume: 21
  start-page: 793
  year: 2005
  end-page: 797
  ident: b0185
  article-title: Functional links between motor and language systems
  publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 3
  start-page: e37
  year: 2005
  ident: b0220
  article-title: Parsing a cognitive task: A characterization of the mind’s bottleneck
  publication-title: PLoS Biology
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1905
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1914
  ident: b0025
  article-title: Grasping ideas with the motor system: Semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension
  publication-title: Cerebral Cortex
– volume: 137
  start-page: 282
  year: 2008
  end-page: 302
  ident: b0065
  article-title: The nature of phoneme representation in spoken word recognition
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
– volume: 4
  start-page: e4508
  year: 2009
  ident: b0155
  article-title: Effects of TMS on different stages of motor and non-motor verb processing in the primary motor cortex
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 21
  start-page: 67
  year: 2010
  end-page: 74
  ident: b0255
  article-title: Body-specific representations of action verbs: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 14
  start-page: S27
  year: 2001
  end-page: 32
  ident: b0120
  article-title: Parietofrontal circuits for action and space perception in the macaque monkey
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 102
  start-page: 130
  year: 2008
  end-page: 136
  ident: b0045
  article-title: Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: A combined EEG and kinematic study
  publication-title: Journal of Physiology Paris
– volume: 50
  start-page: 665
  year: 2010
  end-page: 677
  ident: b0240
  article-title: The functional role of motor activation in language processing: Motor cortical oscillations support lexical-semantic retrieval
  publication-title: Neuroimage
– volume: 74
  start-page: 113
  year: 1997
  end-page: 122
  ident: b0200
  article-title: Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human motor cortex
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods
– volume: 64
  start-page: 785
  year: 2002
  end-page: 794
  ident: b0165
  article-title: Does “whole-word shape” play a role in visual word recognition?
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
– volume: 41
  start-page: 401
  year: 2003
  end-page: 406
  ident: b0125
  article-title: Motor cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 7
  start-page: e35403
  year: 2012
  ident: b0130
  article-title: Processing of hand-related verbs specifically affects the planning and exécution of arm reaching movements
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 43
  start-page: 634
  year: 2008
  end-page: 644
  ident: b0170
  article-title: Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 196
  start-page: 403
  year: 2009
  end-page: 412
  ident: b0050
  article-title: Action word understanding and overt motor behavior
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1607
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1615
  ident: b0035
  article-title: Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 59
  start-page: 3502
  year: 2011
  end-page: 3513
  ident: b0040
  article-title: When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 27
  start-page: 428
  year: 2010
  end-page: 461
  ident: b0150
  article-title: Action performance and action-word understanding: Evidence of double dissociations in left-damaged patients
  publication-title: Cognitive Neuropsychology
– volume: 17
  start-page: 273
  year: 2005
  end-page: 281
  ident: b0230
  article-title: Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 196
  start-page: 403
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0050
  article-title: Action word understanding and overt motor behavior
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1864-8
– volume: 105
  start-page: 17538
  issue: 45
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0060
  article-title: Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805903105
– volume: 14
  start-page: S27
  issue: 1 Pt 2
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0120
  article-title: Parietofrontal circuits for action and space perception in the macaque monkey
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0835
– volume: 14
  start-page: 1
  year: 1931
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0225
  article-title: The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/h0073262
– volume: 21
  start-page: 793
  issue: 3
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0185
  article-title: Functional links between motor and language systems
  publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x
– year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0005
  article-title: What do brain lesions tell us about theories of embodied semantics and the human mirror neuron system?
  publication-title: Cortex
– volume: 77
  start-page: 64
  issue: 1
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0115
  article-title: How the motor system handles nouns: A behavioral study
  publication-title: Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0371-2
– volume: 104
  start-page: 130
  year: 1975
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0090
  article-title: The role of semantic information in lexical decisions
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
– volume: 16
  start-page: 1818
  issue: 18
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0010
  article-title: Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060
– volume: 27
  start-page: 428
  issue: 5
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0150
  article-title: Action performance and action-word understanding: Evidence of double dissociations in left-damaged patients
  publication-title: Cognitive Neuropsychology
  doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.570326
– volume: Vol. I
  start-page: 103
  year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0105
  article-title: Un corpus dell’italiano scritto contemporaneo dalla parte del ricevente
– volume: 106
  start-page: 283
  issue: 4
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0195
  article-title: The organization of the cortical motor system: New concepts
  publication-title: Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology
  doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00022-4
– volume: 15
  start-page: 824
  year: 1989
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0055
  article-title: Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
– volume: 56
  start-page: 1608
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0085
  article-title: Probing the cortical network underlying the psychological refractory period: A combined EEG-fMRI study
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.017
– volume: 19
  start-page: 736
  year: 1980
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0205
  article-title: 51 properties of 125 words: A unit analysis of verbal behaviour
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90415-6
– volume: 6
  start-page: 576
  issue: 7
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0175
  article-title: Brain mechanisms linking language and action
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1038/nrn1706
– volume: 21
  start-page: 67
  issue: 1
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0255
  article-title: Body-specific representations of action verbs: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797609354072
– volume: 7
  start-page: e35403
  issue: 4
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0130
  article-title: Processing of hand-related verbs specifically affects the planning and exécution of arm reaching movements
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035403
– volume: 17
  start-page: 273
  issue: 2
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0230
  article-title: Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/0898929053124965
– volume: 1
  start-page: 476
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0110
  article-title: Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03210951
– volume: 21
  start-page: 191
  issue: 3
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0075
  article-title: Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto-central cortex
  publication-title: Human Brain Mapping
  doi: 10.1002/hbm.10157
– volume: 137
  start-page: 282
  issue: 2
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0065
  article-title: The nature of phoneme representation in spoken word recognition
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.282
– volume: 102
  start-page: 130
  issue: 1–3
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0045
  article-title: Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: A combined EEG and kinematic study
  publication-title: Journal of Physiology Paris
  doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.015
– volume: 14
  start-page: S103
  issue: 1 Pt 2
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0095
  article-title: Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition
  publication-title: NeuroImage
  doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832
– volume: 41
  start-page: 301
  issue: 2
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0070
  article-title: Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex
  publication-title: Neuron
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1905
  issue: 8
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0025
  article-title: Grasping ideas with the motor system: Semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension
  publication-title: Cerebral Cortex
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn217
– volume: 105
  start-page: 83
  issue: 2
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0210
  article-title: Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing
  publication-title: Brain and Language
  doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001
– volume: 46
  start-page: 1915
  issue: 7
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0235
  article-title: Action verbs and the primary motor cortex: A comparative TMS study of silent reading, frequency judgments, and motor imagery
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.015
– volume: 61
  start-page: 933
  issue: 6
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0135
  article-title: Language-induced motor perturbations during the execution of a reaching movement
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove)
  doi: 10.1080/17470210701625667
– volume: 116
  start-page: 220
  issue: 2
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0160
  article-title: Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
  doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220
– volume: 17
  start-page: 905
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0020
  article-title: Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract words
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/0898929054021102
– volume: 50
  start-page: 665
  issue: 2
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0240
  article-title: The functional role of motor activation in language processing: Motor cortical oscillations support lexical-semantic retrieval
  publication-title: Neuroimage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.123
– volume: 18
  start-page: 1607
  issue: 10
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0035
  article-title: Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200msec of processing
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607
– volume: 17
  start-page: 143
  year: 1978
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0245
  article-title: Word-nonword classification times
  publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90110-X
– year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0145
– volume: 46
  start-page: 743
  issue: 2
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0030
  article-title: Word processing in Parkinson’s disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.007
– volume: 107
  start-page: 13240
  issue: 30
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0140
  article-title: Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000674107
– volume: 12
  start-page: 92
  year: 1986
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0100
  article-title: Lexical access for concrete and abstract words
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
– volume: 4
  start-page: e4508
  issue: 2
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0155
  article-title: Effects of TMS on different stages of motor and non-motor verb processing in the primary motor cortex
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004508
– volume: 64
  start-page: 785
  issue: 5
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0165
  article-title: Does “whole-word shape” play a role in visual word recognition?
  publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/BF03194745
– volume: 59
  start-page: 3502
  issue: 4
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0040
  article-title: When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding
  publication-title: NeuroImage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.011
– volume: 19
  start-page: 1083
  issue: 4
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0215
  article-title: Distributed neuronal networks for encoding category-specific semantic information: The mismatch negativity to action words
  publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03126.x
– volume: 102
  start-page: 50
  issue: 1–3
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0080
  article-title: The time course of action and action-word comprehension in the human brain as revealed by neurophysiology
  publication-title: Journal of Physiology, Paris
  doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.013
– volume: 78
  start-page: 143
  issue: 2
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0180
  article-title: Walking or talking? Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing
  publication-title: Brain and Language
  doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2390
– volume: 1224
  start-page: 81
  issue: 1
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0015
  article-title: Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: The case from action verbs
  publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
  doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06013.x
– volume: 17
  start-page: 884
  issue: 6
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0190
  article-title: Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
  doi: 10.1162/0898929054021111
– volume: 43
  start-page: 634
  issue: 3
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0170
  article-title: Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices
  publication-title: NeuroImage
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.006
– volume: 101
  start-page: 278
  issue: 3
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0250
  article-title: Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review
  publication-title: Brain and Language
  doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.03.004
– volume: 74
  start-page: 113
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0200
  article-title: Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human motor cortex
  publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods
  doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(97)02242-5
– volume: 41
  start-page: 401
  issue: 4
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0125
  article-title: Motor cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
  doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00179-3
– volume: 3
  start-page: e37
  issue: 2
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0220
  article-title: Parsing a cognitive task: A characterization of the mind’s bottleneck
  publication-title: PLoS Biology
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030037
SSID ssj0004278
Score 2.314165
Snippet •fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study,...
Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 174
SubjectTerms Adult
Auditory Perception - physiology
Body
Cognition
Cognitive Processes
Concept
Decision making
Dual task
Embodied cognition
Executive Function - physiology
Female
Foot
Hand
Humans
Imageability
Imagery
Judgement
Language
Male
Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology
Perception
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Sensorimotor
Verbs
Young Adult
Title Contribution of motor representations to action verb processing
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25460390
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1629958383
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1647006085
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1660029897
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1738473184
Volume 134
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV07T8MwELZQWVgQb8qjMhJr2iR-JSwIIaoCggmkbpbtJlIRSqo2DCz8du7ipIiBdmCMY0f22b77HN99R8glgtAsliJQljs4oCjQg9xFgQyVUSI2aRpjNPLTsxy98oexGG-Q2zYWBt0qG93vdXqtrZuSQSPNwWw6xRhfpA9VyDiHJC9Iu825wlXe__px88BUEl4bh_V95S8fr8ZFp0Qi1Ij3azcv_peF-guB1pZouEO2GwhJb3wvd8lGVuyRraUm-9wn18g51WayomVOYT7KOa0JLNtgo2JBq5L6sAYK69nSmY8ZAFt2QF6Hdy-3o6DJlBA4QGBVwAGHcANSgNOCC-McUAUgqzBPYmMNi5i0zlonchvFcMCasDQLo8ylJnHJBAodOySdoiyyY0IjYRVscsNzJrnJYyOEY6m0eQLfi5XrEtlKR7uGRhyzWbzr1l_sTS_FqlGs-ALE2iXhsuHMM2msb3LVil__WhQa9P36xhfthGnYMngPYoqs_FjoSIINxutitqoOV8hVk4hVdaRnsFcr6igG9h8UJ_TnyK-a5egxFUHI0vDkP8M8JVvwJPxfoTPSqeYf2TngpMr26o3QI5s394-j529t_hIT
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV25TsQwEB3BUkCDuG8wEm0g8ZnQIIRAy7UVSHSWbRIJhJLVshT8PTM5FlGwFLQ-IntszzzHM28AjgiE5lyryHgZ8IJiUA_KkEQ6Ns4o7rKMUzTy_UD3H-XNk3qagYsuFobcKlvd3-j0Wlu3JSetNE-GLy8U40v0oYYY54jkRc_CHLFTqR7MnV_f9gff4ZG8U8hx_WT5w82r9dKpiAs1kce1p5f8zUj9BkJrY3S1BIstimTnzUCXYSYvV2Bhosw-V-GMaKe6ZFasKhguSTViNYdlF29UvrNxxZrIBoZb2rNhEzaA5mwNHq8uHy76UZssIQoIwsaRRCgiHQoCLwwh5gUCCwRXcZFy551IhPbB-6AKn3C8Yz2LLI-TPGQuDekzFgaxDr2yKvNNYInyBs-5k4XQ0hXcKRVEpn2R4ve4CVugO-nY0DKJU0KLN9u5jL3aiVgtiZUqUKxbEE86Dhsyjb-7nHbitz_2hUWV_3fnw27BLJ4aegpxZV59vNtEoxmmF2MxrY00RFeTqmltdENib6a0MQIhAOpOHM9Gs2sms6dsBLHI4u3_TPMA5vsP93f27npwuwMLWKOan0S70BuPPvI9hE1jv98eiy_B9BTE
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Contribution+of+motor+representations+to+action+verb+processing&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Andres%2C+Michael&rft.au=Finocchiaro%2C+Chiara&rft.au=Buiatti%2C+Marco&rft.au=Piazza%2C+Manuela&rft.date=2015-01-01&rft.issn=1873-7838&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft.volume=134&rft.spage=174&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.cognition.2014.10.004&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon