Contribution of motor representations to action verb processing
•fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study, hand- and foot-related verbs were judged in a dual-task context.•Results evidenced a verb meaning – response effector compatibility effect.•In...
Saved in:
Published in | Cognition Vol. 134; no. Jan; pp. 174 - 184 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Netherlands
Elsevier B.V
01.01.2015
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0010-0277 1873-7838 1873-7838 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | •fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study, hand- and foot-related verbs were judged in a dual-task context.•Results evidenced a verb meaning – response effector compatibility effect.•Interference of the dual task challenges post-conceptual accounts.
Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection.Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. All rights reserved, Elsevier Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. •fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study, hand- and foot-related verbs were judged in a dual-task context.•Results evidenced a verb meaning – response effector compatibility effect.•Interference of the dual task challenges post-conceptual accounts. Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor activation has been taken as an indication that the meaning of action verbs is embedded in motor representations. However, discrepancies in the literature led to the alternative hypothesis that motor representations are activated during the course of a mental imagery process emerging only after the meaning of the action has been accessed. In order to address this issue, we asked participants to decide whether a visually presented verb was concrete or abstract by pressing a button or a pedal (primary task) and then to provide a distinct vocal response to low and high sounds played soon after the verb display (secondary task). Manipulations of the visual display (lower vs. uppercase), verb imageability (concrete vs. abstract), verb meaning (hand vs. foot-related), and response effector (hand vs. foot) allowed us to trace the perceptual, semantic and response stages of verb processing. We capitalized on the psychological refractory period (PRP), which implies that the initiation of the secondary task should be delayed only by those factors that slow down the central decision process in the primary task. In line with this prediction, our results showed that the time cost resulting from the processing of abstract verbs, when compared to concrete verbs, was still observed in the subsequent response to the sounds, whereas the overall advantage of hand over foot responses did not influence sound judgments. Crucially, we also observed a verb-effector compatibility effect (i.e., foot-related verbs are responded faster with the foot and hand-related verbs with the hand) that contaminated the performance of the secondary task, providing clear evidence that motor interference from verb meaning occurred during the central decision stage. These results cannot be explained by a mental imagery process that would deploy only during the execution of the response to verb judgments. They rather indicate that the motor activation induced by action verbs accompanies the lexico-semantic processes leading to response selection. [Copyright Elsevier B.V.] |
Author | Buiatti, Marco Piazza, Manuela Finocchiaro, Chiara Andres, Michael |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Michael surname: Andres fullname: Andres, Michael email: andres.meh@gmail.com organization: Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain, Place Cardinal Mercier 10, 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium – sequence: 2 givenname: Chiara surname: Finocchiaro fullname: Finocchiaro, Chiara organization: Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy – sequence: 3 givenname: Marco surname: Buiatti fullname: Buiatti, Marco organization: Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy – sequence: 4 givenname: Manuela surname: Piazza fullname: Piazza, Manuela organization: Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Corso Bettini 31, 38068 Rovereto, Italy |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25460390$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkU9PGzEUxC0EgkD7Fdo99rLp859d24eqQlFpKyFxgbPl9b5FjhI7tR2kfnu8DXDoJZwsjX8zT5q5JKchBiTkM4UlBdp_XS9dfAy--BiWDKio6hJAnJAFVZK3UnF1ShYAFFpgUl6Qy5zXUAkm1Tm5YJ3ogWtYkO-rGEryw36OauLUbGOJqUm4S5gxFDvruSmxse4f8oRpaHYpOszZh8cP5Gyym4wfX94r8nDz4371q729-_l7dX3buo7q0oqeSmGBaqqYAzZ1SijewaSYHSynvB_cMLhuGihjSo9cI1B02iqnxio6fkW-HHLr6T97zMVsfXa42diAcZ8NlVwJyakSx9G-B2BaafkOVEiAHlT3DpRp3dXeeUU_vaD7YYuj2SW_temveW29At8OgEsx54STcf7QdEnWbwwFM69s1uZtZTOvPH_UDatf_ud_PXHceX1wYt3qyWMy2XkMDkef0BUzRn804xn_Z8WA |
CODEN | CGTNAU |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4706_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_11_021 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2017_01_010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_newideapsych_2019_100777 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2022_10_022 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0270352 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2024_12_018 crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_022_02918_1 crossref_primary_10_1080_20445911_2017_1322093 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00072 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11406_023_00693_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_09_022 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_17326_w crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00223 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10670_021_00395_9 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2023_e13426 crossref_primary_10_17711_SM_0185_3325_2019_039 crossref_primary_10_1111_papq_12243 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneuroling_2015_07_003 crossref_primary_10_1027_1618_3169_a000507 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_016_0776_z crossref_primary_10_1080_02643294_2020_1732319 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2018_02_003 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_plrev_2018_04_005 crossref_primary_10_1017_langcog_2023_22 crossref_primary_10_1177_09593543231170854 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_015_0870_z crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_52426_9 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11245_020_09723_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2017_04_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2021_103428 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/s00221-009-1864-8 10.1073/pnas.0805903105 10.1006/nimg.2001.0835 10.1037/h0073262 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x 10.1007/s00426-011-0371-2 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060 10.1080/02643294.2011.570326 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00022-4 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.017 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90415-6 10.1038/nrn1706 10.1177/0956797609354072 10.1371/journal.pone.0035403 10.1162/0898929053124965 10.3758/BF03210951 10.1002/hbm.10157 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.282 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.015 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9 10.1093/cercor/bhn217 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.015 10.1080/17470210701625667 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220 10.1162/0898929054021102 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.123 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607 10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90110-X 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.007 10.1073/pnas.1000674107 10.1371/journal.pone.0004508 10.3758/BF03194745 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.011 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03126.x 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.013 10.1006/brln.2000.2390 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06013.x 10.1162/0898929054021111 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.006 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.03.004 10.1016/S0165-0270(97)02242-5 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00179-3 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030037 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2014 Elsevier B.V. Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2014 Elsevier B.V. – notice: Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 7TK 8BJ FQK JBE 7T9 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) International Bibliography of the Social Sciences International Bibliography of the Social Sciences Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) MEDLINE Neurosciences Abstracts Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1873-7838 |
EndPage | 184 |
ExternalDocumentID | 25460390 10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004 S0010027714001966 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M --Z -DZ -~X .~1 0R~ 186 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29F 3EH 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5VS 6J9 6PF 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFJI AAGJA AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAWTL AAXLA AAXUO ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABLJU ABMAC ABMMH ABOYX ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACIUM ACKIV ACNCT ACPRK ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AETEA AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV AKYCK ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ AOMHK ASPBG AVARZ AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CS3 DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FD6 FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q G8K GBLVA HF~ HMQ HMW HVGLF HZ~ IHE J1W K-O KOM LPU M2V M3V M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A NHB O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OKEIE OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. PRBVW Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSO SSY SSZ T5K TN5 UBW UPT UQL WH7 WUQ XFK XIH XJT XKC XOL XPP YYP YZZ ZA5 ZKB ZMT ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABDPE ACVFH ADCNI ADMHG ADVLN AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 7TK 8BJ FQK JBE 7T9 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c519t-46174a019182c02f5848350f82aba3136bcbbc5fb12289d39e01ec9a8c8dfb1c3 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0010-0277 1873-7838 |
IngestDate | Thu Jul 10 19:16:35 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 23:39:41 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 09:52:31 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 09:06:41 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:00:58 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:29:11 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:00:53 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:18:56 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | Jan |
Keywords | Concept Embodied cognition Language Body Dual task Sensorimotor |
Language | English |
License | Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c519t-46174a019182c02f5848350f82aba3136bcbbc5fb12289d39e01ec9a8c8dfb1c3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | http://hdl.handle.net/2078.1/156434 |
PMID | 25460390 |
PQID | 1629958383 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 11 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_1738473184 proquest_miscellaneous_1660029897 proquest_miscellaneous_1647006085 proquest_miscellaneous_1629958383 pubmed_primary_25460390 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_10_004 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | January 2015 2015-01-00 2015-Jan 20150101 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2015-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2015 text: January 2015 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Netherlands |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Netherlands |
PublicationTitle | Cognition |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Cognition |
PublicationYear | 2015 |
Publisher | Elsevier B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier B.V |
References | Sato, Mengarelli, Riggio, Gallese, Buccino (b0210) 2008; 105 Nazir, Boulenger, Roy, Silber, Jeannerod, Paulignan (b0135) 2008; 61 de Groot (b0055) 1989; 15 Meister, Boroojerdi, Foltys, Sparing, Huber, Töpper (b0125) 2003; 41 Dalla Volta, Gianelli, Campione, Gentilucci (b0050) 2009; 196 Boulenger, Roy, Paulignan, Deprez, Jeannerod, Nazir (b0035) 2006; 18 Binder, Westbury, McKiernan, Possing, Medler (b0020) 2005; 17 Telford (b0225) 1931; 14 Hauk, Shtyrov, Pulvermüller (b0080) 2008; 102 Pashler (b0160) 1994; 116 Hauk, Pulvermüller (b0075) 2004; 21 Marino, Gough, Gallese, Riggio, Buccino (b0115) 2013; 77 Kroll, Merves (b0100) 1986; 12 Papeo, Vallesi, Isaja, Rumiati (b0155) 2009; 4 Rizzolatti, Luppino, Matelli (b0195) 1998; 106 Postle, McMahon, Ashton, Meredith, de Zubicaray (b0170) 2008; 43 Loftus, Masson (b0110) 1994; 1 Whaley (b0245) 1978; 17 Boulenger, Silber, Roy, Paulignan, Jeannerod, Nazir (b0045) 2008; 102 Boulenger, Shtyrov, Pulvermüller (b0040) 2011; 59 Papeo, Negri, Zadini, Rumiati (b0150) 2010; 27 Willems, Hagoort, Casasanto (b0255) 2010; 21 Aziz-Zadeh, Wilson, Rizzolatti, Iacoboni (b0010) 2006; 16 Pulvermüller (b0175) 2005; 6 Boulenger, Mechtouff, Thobois, Broussolle, Jeannerod, Nazir (b0030) 2008; 46 James (b0090) 1975; 104 van Elk, van Schie, Zwaan, Bekkering (b0240) 2010; 50 Gaskell, Quinlan, Tamminen, Cleland (b0065) 2008; 137 Hauk, Johnsrude, Pulvermüller (b0070) 2004; 41 Pulvermüller, Hauk, Nikulin, Ilmoniemi (b0185) 2005; 21 Jeannerod (b0095) 2001; 14 Matelli, Luppino (b0120) 2001; 14 Sigman, Dehaene (b0220) 2005; 3 Arévalo, Baldo, Dronkers (b0005) 2010 Willems, Hagoort (b0250) 2007; 101 Perea, Rosa (b0165) 2002; 64 Rothwell (b0200) 1997; 74 Pulvermüller, Härle, Hummel (b0180) 2001; 78 Tomasino, Fink, Sparing, Dafotakis, Weiss (b0235) 2008; 46 Rubin (b0205) 1980; 19 Mirabella, Iaconelli, Spadacenta, Federico, Gallese (b0130) 2012; 7 Boulenger, Hauk, Pulvermüller (b0025) 2009; 19 Forstmann, Dutilh, Brown, Neumann, von Cramon, Ridderinkhof (b0060) 2008; 105 Shtyrov, Hauk, Pulvermüller (b0215) 2004; 19 Laudanna, Thornton, Brown, Burani, Marconi (b0105) 1995; Vol. I Bedny, Caramazza (b0015) 2011; 1224 Paivio (b0145) 1986 Tettamanti, Buccino, Saccuman, Gallese, Danna, Scifo (b0230) 2005; 17 Pulvermüller, Shtyrov, Ilmoniemi (b0190) 2005; 17 Hesselman, Flandin, Dehaene (b0085) 2011; 56 Neubert, Mars, Buch, Olivier, Rushworth (b0140) 2010; 107 Loftus (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0110) 1994; 1 Sigman (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0220) 2005; 3 Hauk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0080) 2008; 102 Pashler (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0160) 1994; 116 Laudanna (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0105) 1995; Vol. I Meister (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0125) 2003; 41 Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0045) 2008; 102 Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0175) 2005; 6 Neubert (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0140) 2010; 107 Sato (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0210) 2008; 105 Binder (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0020) 2005; 17 Hesselman (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0085) 2011; 56 Marino (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0115) 2013; 77 Paivio (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0145) 1986 Hauk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0070) 2004; 41 Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0030) 2008; 46 Papeo (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0155) 2009; 4 Tomasino (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0235) 2008; 46 Perea (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0165) 2002; 64 Shtyrov (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0215) 2004; 19 Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0040) 2011; 59 Rothwell (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0200) 1997; 74 van Elk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0240) 2010; 50 Hauk (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0075) 2004; 21 Postle (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0170) 2008; 43 Willems (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0250) 2007; 101 Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0185) 2005; 21 Arévalo (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0005) 2010 Whaley (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0245) 1978; 17 Forstmann (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0060) 2008; 105 Dalla Volta (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0050) 2009; 196 Papeo (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0150) 2010; 27 Matelli (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0120) 2001; 14 Nazir (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0135) 2008; 61 Jeannerod (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0095) 2001; 14 de Groot (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0055) 1989; 15 Gaskell (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0065) 2008; 137 Rizzolatti (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0195) 1998; 106 James (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0090) 1975; 104 Kroll (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0100) 1986; 12 Aziz-Zadeh (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0010) 2006; 16 Mirabella (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0130) 2012; 7 Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0190) 2005; 17 Pulvermüller (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0180) 2001; 78 Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0035) 2006; 18 Boulenger (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0025) 2009; 19 Bedny (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0015) 2011; 1224 Tettamanti (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0230) 2005; 17 Rubin (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0205) 1980; 19 Willems (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0255) 2010; 21 Telford (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0225) 1931; 14 |
References_xml | – year: 1986 ident: b0145 article-title: Mental representations – A dual coding approach – volume: 21 start-page: 191 year: 2004 end-page: 201 ident: b0075 article-title: Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto-central cortex publication-title: Human Brain Mapping – volume: 19 start-page: 736 year: 1980 end-page: 755 ident: b0205 article-title: 51 properties of 125 words: A unit analysis of verbal behaviour publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior – volume: 16 start-page: 1818 year: 2006 end-page: 1823 ident: b0010 article-title: Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 1224 start-page: 81 year: 2011 end-page: 95 ident: b0015 article-title: Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: The case from action verbs publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences – volume: 17 start-page: 884 year: 2005 end-page: 892 ident: b0190 article-title: Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 105 start-page: 83 year: 2008 end-page: 90 ident: b0210 article-title: Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing publication-title: Brain and Language – volume: 77 start-page: 64 year: 2013 end-page: 73 ident: b0115 article-title: How the motor system handles nouns: A behavioral study publication-title: Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung – volume: 78 start-page: 143 year: 2001 end-page: 168 ident: b0180 article-title: Walking or talking? Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing publication-title: Brain and Language – volume: 61 start-page: 933 year: 2008 end-page: 943 ident: b0135 article-title: Language-induced motor perturbations during the execution of a reaching movement publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove) – volume: 106 start-page: 283 year: 1998 end-page: 296 ident: b0195 article-title: The organization of the cortical motor system: New concepts publication-title: Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology – volume: 102 start-page: 50 year: 2008 end-page: 58 ident: b0080 article-title: The time course of action and action-word comprehension in the human brain as revealed by neurophysiology publication-title: Journal of Physiology, Paris – volume: 56 start-page: 1608 year: 2011 end-page: 1621 ident: b0085 article-title: Probing the cortical network underlying the psychological refractory period: A combined EEG-fMRI study publication-title: Neuroimage – volume: 17 start-page: 143 year: 1978 end-page: 154 ident: b0245 article-title: Word-nonword classification times publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior – volume: 6 start-page: 576 year: 2005 end-page: 582 ident: b0175 article-title: Brain mechanisms linking language and action publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience – year: 2010 ident: b0005 article-title: What do brain lesions tell us about theories of embodied semantics and the human mirror neuron system? publication-title: Cortex – volume: 105 start-page: 17538 year: 2008 end-page: 17542 ident: b0060 article-title: Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America – volume: 104 start-page: 130 year: 1975 end-page: 136 ident: b0090 article-title: The role of semantic information in lexical decisions publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 17 start-page: 905 year: 2005 end-page: 917 ident: b0020 article-title: Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract words publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 14 start-page: S103 year: 2001 end-page: 109 ident: b0095 article-title: Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition publication-title: NeuroImage – volume: 1 start-page: 476 year: 1994 end-page: 490 ident: b0110 article-title: Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 19 start-page: 1083 year: 2004 end-page: 1092 ident: b0215 article-title: Distributed neuronal networks for encoding category-specific semantic information: The mismatch negativity to action words publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 107 start-page: 13240 year: 2010 end-page: 13245 ident: b0140 article-title: Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America – volume: 46 start-page: 743 year: 2008 end-page: 756 ident: b0030 article-title: Word processing in Parkinson’s disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 101 start-page: 278 year: 2007 end-page: 289 ident: b0250 article-title: Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review publication-title: Brain and Language – volume: 12 start-page: 92 year: 1986 end-page: 107 ident: b0100 article-title: Lexical access for concrete and abstract words publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition – volume: Vol. I start-page: 103 year: 1995 end-page: 109 ident: b0105 article-title: Un corpus dell’italiano scritto contemporaneo dalla parte del ricevente publication-title: III Giornate internazionali di Analisi Statistica dei Dati Testuali – volume: 116 start-page: 220 year: 1994 end-page: 244 ident: b0160 article-title: Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 14 start-page: 1 year: 1931 end-page: 36 ident: b0225 article-title: The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 41 start-page: 301 year: 2004 end-page: 307 ident: b0070 article-title: Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex publication-title: Neuron – volume: 46 start-page: 1915 year: 2008 end-page: 1926 ident: b0235 article-title: Action verbs and the primary motor cortex: A comparative TMS study of silent reading, frequency judgments, and motor imagery publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 15 start-page: 824 year: 1989 end-page: 845 ident: b0055 article-title: Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 21 start-page: 793 year: 2005 end-page: 797 ident: b0185 article-title: Functional links between motor and language systems publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 3 start-page: e37 year: 2005 ident: b0220 article-title: Parsing a cognitive task: A characterization of the mind’s bottleneck publication-title: PLoS Biology – volume: 19 start-page: 1905 year: 2009 end-page: 1914 ident: b0025 article-title: Grasping ideas with the motor system: Semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – volume: 137 start-page: 282 year: 2008 end-page: 302 ident: b0065 article-title: The nature of phoneme representation in spoken word recognition publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 4 start-page: e4508 year: 2009 ident: b0155 article-title: Effects of TMS on different stages of motor and non-motor verb processing in the primary motor cortex publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 21 start-page: 67 year: 2010 end-page: 74 ident: b0255 article-title: Body-specific representations of action verbs: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 14 start-page: S27 year: 2001 end-page: 32 ident: b0120 article-title: Parietofrontal circuits for action and space perception in the macaque monkey publication-title: Neuroimage – volume: 102 start-page: 130 year: 2008 end-page: 136 ident: b0045 article-title: Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: A combined EEG and kinematic study publication-title: Journal of Physiology Paris – volume: 50 start-page: 665 year: 2010 end-page: 677 ident: b0240 article-title: The functional role of motor activation in language processing: Motor cortical oscillations support lexical-semantic retrieval publication-title: Neuroimage – volume: 74 start-page: 113 year: 1997 end-page: 122 ident: b0200 article-title: Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human motor cortex publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods – volume: 64 start-page: 785 year: 2002 end-page: 794 ident: b0165 article-title: Does “whole-word shape” play a role in visual word recognition? publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics – volume: 41 start-page: 401 year: 2003 end-page: 406 ident: b0125 article-title: Motor cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 7 start-page: e35403 year: 2012 ident: b0130 article-title: Processing of hand-related verbs specifically affects the planning and exécution of arm reaching movements publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 43 start-page: 634 year: 2008 end-page: 644 ident: b0170 article-title: Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices publication-title: NeuroImage – volume: 196 start-page: 403 year: 2009 end-page: 412 ident: b0050 article-title: Action word understanding and overt motor behavior publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 18 start-page: 1607 year: 2006 end-page: 1615 ident: b0035 article-title: Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 59 start-page: 3502 year: 2011 end-page: 3513 ident: b0040 article-title: When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding publication-title: NeuroImage – volume: 27 start-page: 428 year: 2010 end-page: 461 ident: b0150 article-title: Action performance and action-word understanding: Evidence of double dissociations in left-damaged patients publication-title: Cognitive Neuropsychology – volume: 17 start-page: 273 year: 2005 end-page: 281 ident: b0230 article-title: Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 196 start-page: 403 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0050 article-title: Action word understanding and overt motor behavior publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1864-8 – volume: 105 start-page: 17538 issue: 45 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0060 article-title: Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805903105 – volume: 14 start-page: S27 issue: 1 Pt 2 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0120 article-title: Parietofrontal circuits for action and space perception in the macaque monkey publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0835 – volume: 14 start-page: 1 year: 1931 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0225 article-title: The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1037/h0073262 – volume: 21 start-page: 793 issue: 3 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0185 article-title: Functional links between motor and language systems publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x – year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0005 article-title: What do brain lesions tell us about theories of embodied semantics and the human mirror neuron system? publication-title: Cortex – volume: 77 start-page: 64 issue: 1 year: 2013 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0115 article-title: How the motor system handles nouns: A behavioral study publication-title: Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0371-2 – volume: 104 start-page: 130 year: 1975 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0090 article-title: The role of semantic information in lexical decisions publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 16 start-page: 1818 issue: 18 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0010 article-title: Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060 – volume: 27 start-page: 428 issue: 5 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0150 article-title: Action performance and action-word understanding: Evidence of double dissociations in left-damaged patients publication-title: Cognitive Neuropsychology doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.570326 – volume: Vol. I start-page: 103 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0105 article-title: Un corpus dell’italiano scritto contemporaneo dalla parte del ricevente – volume: 106 start-page: 283 issue: 4 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0195 article-title: The organization of the cortical motor system: New concepts publication-title: Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00022-4 – volume: 15 start-page: 824 year: 1989 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0055 article-title: Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition – volume: 56 start-page: 1608 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0085 article-title: Probing the cortical network underlying the psychological refractory period: A combined EEG-fMRI study publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.017 – volume: 19 start-page: 736 year: 1980 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0205 article-title: 51 properties of 125 words: A unit analysis of verbal behaviour publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90415-6 – volume: 6 start-page: 576 issue: 7 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0175 article-title: Brain mechanisms linking language and action publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience doi: 10.1038/nrn1706 – volume: 21 start-page: 67 issue: 1 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0255 article-title: Body-specific representations of action verbs: Neural evidence from right- and left-handers publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797609354072 – volume: 7 start-page: e35403 issue: 4 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0130 article-title: Processing of hand-related verbs specifically affects the planning and exécution of arm reaching movements publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035403 – volume: 17 start-page: 273 issue: 2 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0230 article-title: Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/0898929053124965 – volume: 1 start-page: 476 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0110 article-title: Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03210951 – volume: 21 start-page: 191 issue: 3 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0075 article-title: Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto-central cortex publication-title: Human Brain Mapping doi: 10.1002/hbm.10157 – volume: 137 start-page: 282 issue: 2 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0065 article-title: The nature of phoneme representation in spoken word recognition publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.282 – volume: 102 start-page: 130 issue: 1–3 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0045 article-title: Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: A combined EEG and kinematic study publication-title: Journal of Physiology Paris doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.015 – volume: 14 start-page: S103 issue: 1 Pt 2 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0095 article-title: Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition publication-title: NeuroImage doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832 – volume: 41 start-page: 301 issue: 2 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0070 article-title: Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex publication-title: Neuron doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9 – volume: 19 start-page: 1905 issue: 8 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0025 article-title: Grasping ideas with the motor system: Semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension publication-title: Cerebral Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn217 – volume: 105 start-page: 83 issue: 2 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0210 article-title: Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing publication-title: Brain and Language doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001 – volume: 46 start-page: 1915 issue: 7 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0235 article-title: Action verbs and the primary motor cortex: A comparative TMS study of silent reading, frequency judgments, and motor imagery publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.015 – volume: 61 start-page: 933 issue: 6 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0135 article-title: Language-induced motor perturbations during the execution of a reaching movement publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove) doi: 10.1080/17470210701625667 – volume: 116 start-page: 220 issue: 2 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0160 article-title: Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory publication-title: Psychological Bulletin doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220 – volume: 17 start-page: 905 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0020 article-title: Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract words publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/0898929054021102 – volume: 50 start-page: 665 issue: 2 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0240 article-title: The functional role of motor activation in language processing: Motor cortical oscillations support lexical-semantic retrieval publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.123 – volume: 18 start-page: 1607 issue: 10 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0035 article-title: Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200msec of processing publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607 – volume: 17 start-page: 143 year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0245 article-title: Word-nonword classification times publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90110-X – year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0145 – volume: 46 start-page: 743 issue: 2 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0030 article-title: Word processing in Parkinson’s disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.007 – volume: 107 start-page: 13240 issue: 30 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0140 article-title: Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000674107 – volume: 12 start-page: 92 year: 1986 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0100 article-title: Lexical access for concrete and abstract words publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition – volume: 4 start-page: e4508 issue: 2 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0155 article-title: Effects of TMS on different stages of motor and non-motor verb processing in the primary motor cortex publication-title: PLoS ONE doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004508 – volume: 64 start-page: 785 issue: 5 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0165 article-title: Does “whole-word shape” play a role in visual word recognition? publication-title: Perception & Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03194745 – volume: 59 start-page: 3502 issue: 4 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0040 article-title: When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding publication-title: NeuroImage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.011 – volume: 19 start-page: 1083 issue: 4 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0215 article-title: Distributed neuronal networks for encoding category-specific semantic information: The mismatch negativity to action words publication-title: The European Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03126.x – volume: 102 start-page: 50 issue: 1–3 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0080 article-title: The time course of action and action-word comprehension in the human brain as revealed by neurophysiology publication-title: Journal of Physiology, Paris doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.013 – volume: 78 start-page: 143 issue: 2 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0180 article-title: Walking or talking? Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of action verb processing publication-title: Brain and Language doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2390 – volume: 1224 start-page: 81 issue: 1 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0015 article-title: Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: The case from action verbs publication-title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06013.x – volume: 17 start-page: 884 issue: 6 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0190 article-title: Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/0898929054021111 – volume: 43 start-page: 634 issue: 3 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0170 article-title: Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices publication-title: NeuroImage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.006 – volume: 101 start-page: 278 issue: 3 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0250 article-title: Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review publication-title: Brain and Language doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.03.004 – volume: 74 start-page: 113 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0200 article-title: Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human motor cortex publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience Methods doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(97)02242-5 – volume: 41 start-page: 401 issue: 4 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0125 article-title: Motor cortex hand area and speech: Implications for the development of language publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00179-3 – volume: 3 start-page: e37 issue: 2 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004_b0220 article-title: Parsing a cognitive task: A characterization of the mind’s bottleneck publication-title: PLoS Biology doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030037 |
SSID | ssj0004278 |
Score | 2.314165 |
Snippet | •fMRI studies showed somatotopic activation while reading action verbs.•Such activation could reflect conceptual or post-conceptual processes.•In this study,... Electrophysiological and brain imaging studies show a somatotopic activation of the premotor cortex while subjects process action verbs. This somatotopic motor... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 174 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Auditory Perception - physiology Body Cognition Cognitive Processes Concept Decision making Dual task Embodied cognition Executive Function - physiology Female Foot Hand Humans Imageability Imagery Judgement Language Male Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology Perception Psychomotor Performance - physiology Sensorimotor Verbs Young Adult |
Title | Contribution of motor representations to action verb processing |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.10.004 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25460390 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1629958383 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1647006085 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1660029897 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1738473184 |
Volume | 134 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV07T8MwELZQWVgQb8qjMhJr2iR-JSwIIaoCggmkbpbtJlIRSqo2DCz8du7ipIiBdmCMY0f22b77HN99R8glgtAsliJQljs4oCjQg9xFgQyVUSI2aRpjNPLTsxy98oexGG-Q2zYWBt0qG93vdXqtrZuSQSPNwWw6xRhfpA9VyDiHJC9Iu825wlXe__px88BUEl4bh_V95S8fr8ZFp0Qi1Ij3azcv_peF-guB1pZouEO2GwhJb3wvd8lGVuyRraUm-9wn18g51WayomVOYT7KOa0JLNtgo2JBq5L6sAYK69nSmY8ZAFt2QF6Hdy-3o6DJlBA4QGBVwAGHcANSgNOCC-McUAUgqzBPYmMNi5i0zlonchvFcMCasDQLo8ylJnHJBAodOySdoiyyY0IjYRVscsNzJrnJYyOEY6m0eQLfi5XrEtlKR7uGRhyzWbzr1l_sTS_FqlGs-ALE2iXhsuHMM2msb3LVil__WhQa9P36xhfthGnYMngPYoqs_FjoSIINxutitqoOV8hVk4hVdaRnsFcr6igG9h8UJ_TnyK-a5egxFUHI0vDkP8M8JVvwJPxfoTPSqeYf2TngpMr26o3QI5s394-j529t_hIT |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV25TsQwEB3BUkCDuG8wEm0g8ZnQIIRAy7UVSHSWbRIJhJLVshT8PTM5FlGwFLQ-IntszzzHM28AjgiE5lyryHgZ8IJiUA_KkEQ6Ns4o7rKMUzTy_UD3H-XNk3qagYsuFobcKlvd3-j0Wlu3JSetNE-GLy8U40v0oYYY54jkRc_CHLFTqR7MnV_f9gff4ZG8U8hx_WT5w82r9dKpiAs1kce1p5f8zUj9BkJrY3S1BIstimTnzUCXYSYvV2Bhosw-V-GMaKe6ZFasKhguSTViNYdlF29UvrNxxZrIBoZb2rNhEzaA5mwNHq8uHy76UZssIQoIwsaRRCgiHQoCLwwh5gUCCwRXcZFy551IhPbB-6AKn3C8Yz2LLI-TPGQuDekzFgaxDr2yKvNNYInyBs-5k4XQ0hXcKRVEpn2R4ve4CVugO-nY0DKJU0KLN9u5jL3aiVgtiZUqUKxbEE86Dhsyjb-7nHbitz_2hUWV_3fnw27BLJ4aegpxZV59vNtEoxmmF2MxrY00RFeTqmltdENib6a0MQIhAOpOHM9Gs2sms6dsBLHI4u3_TPMA5vsP93f27npwuwMLWKOan0S70BuPPvI9hE1jv98eiy_B9BTE |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Contribution+of+motor+representations+to+action+verb+processing&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Andres%2C+Michael&rft.au=Finocchiaro%2C+Chiara&rft.au=Buiatti%2C+Marco&rft.au=Piazza%2C+Manuela&rft.date=2015-01-01&rft.issn=1873-7838&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft.volume=134&rft.spage=174&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.cognition.2014.10.004&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |