Neural Measures Reveal a Fixed Item Limit in Subitizing

For centuries, it has been known that humans can rapidly and accurately enumerate small sets of items, a process referred to as subitizing. However, there is still active debate regarding the mechanisms that mediate this ability. For example, some have argued that subitizing reflects the operation o...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inThe Journal of neuroscience Vol. 32; no. 21; pp. 7169 - 7177
Main Authors Ester, Edward F., Drew, Trafton, Klee, Daniel, Vogel, Edward K., Awh, Edward
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Society for Neuroscience 23.05.2012
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract For centuries, it has been known that humans can rapidly and accurately enumerate small sets of items, a process referred to as subitizing. However, there is still active debate regarding the mechanisms that mediate this ability. For example, some have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a fixed-capacity individuation mechanism that enables concurrent access to a small number of items. However, others have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a continuous numerical estimation mechanism whose precision varies with numerosity in a manner consistent with Weber's law. Critically, quantitative models based on either of these predictions can provide a reasonable description of subitizing performance, making it difficult to discriminate between these alternatives solely on the basis of subjects' behavioral performance. Here, we attempted to discriminate between fixed-capacity and continuous estimation models of subitizing using neural measures. In two experiments, we recorded EEGs while subjects performed a demanding subitizing task and examined set-size-dependent changes in a neurophysiological marker of visual selection (the N2pc event-related potential component) evoked by an array of to-be-enumerated items. In both experiments, N2pc amplitudes increased monotonically within the subitizing range before reaching an asymptotic limit at approximately three items. Moreover, inter-participant differences in the location of this asymptote were strongly predictive of behavioral estimates of subitizing span derived from a fixed-capacity model. Thus, neural activity linked with subitizing ability shows evidence of an early and discrete limit in the number of items that can be concurrently apprehended, supporting a fixed-capacity model of this process.
AbstractList For centuries, it has been known that humans can rapidly and accurately enumerate small sets of items, a process referred to as subitizing. However, there is still active debate regarding the mechanisms that mediate this ability. For example, some have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a fixed-capacity individuation mechanism that enables concurrent access to a small number of items. However, others have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a continuous numerical estimation mechanism whose precision varies with numerosity in a manner consistent with Weber's law. Critically, quantitative models based on either of these predictions can provide a reasonable description of subitizing performance, making it difficult to discriminate between these alternatives solely on the basis of subjects' behavioral performance. Here, we attempted to discriminate between fixed-capacity and continuous estimation models of subitizing using neural measures. In two experiments, we recorded EEGs while subjects performed a demanding subitizing task and examined set-size-dependent changes in a neurophysiological marker of visual selection (the N2pc event-related potential component) evoked by an array of to-be-enumerated items. In both experiments, N2pc amplitudes increased monotonically within the subitizing range before reaching an asymptotic limit at approximately three items. Moreover, inter-participant differences in the location of this asymptote were strongly predictive of behavioral estimates of subitizing span derived from a fixed-capacity model. Thus, neural activity linked with subitizing ability shows evidence of an early and discrete limit in the number of items that can be concurrently apprehended, supporting a fixed-capacity model of this process.
For centuries, it has been known that humans can rapidly and accurately enumerate small sets of items, a process referred to as subitizing. However, there is still active debate regarding the mechanisms that mediate this ability. For example, some have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a fixed-capacity individuation mechanism that enables concurrent access to a small number of items. However, others have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a continuous numerical estimation mechanism whose precision varies with numerosity in a manner consistent with Weber's law. Critically, quantitative models based on either of these predictions can provide a reasonable description of subitizing performance, making it difficult to discriminate between these alternatives solely on the basis of subjects' behavioral performance. Here, we attempted to discriminate between fixed-capacity and continuous estimation models of subitizing using neural measures. In two experiments, we recorded EEGs while subjects performed a demanding subitizing task and examined set-size-dependent changes in a neurophysiological marker of visual selection (the N2pc event-related potential component) evoked by an array of to-be-enumerated items. In both experiments, N2pc amplitudes increased monotonically within the subitizing range before reaching an asymptotic limit at approximately three items. Moreover, inter-participant differences in the location of this asymptote were strongly predictive of behavioral estimates of subitizing span derived from a fixed-capacity model. Thus, neural activity linked with subitizing ability shows evidence of an early and discrete limit in the number of items that can be concurrently apprehended, supporting a fixed-capacity model of this process.For centuries, it has been known that humans can rapidly and accurately enumerate small sets of items, a process referred to as subitizing. However, there is still active debate regarding the mechanisms that mediate this ability. For example, some have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a fixed-capacity individuation mechanism that enables concurrent access to a small number of items. However, others have argued that subitizing reflects the operation of a continuous numerical estimation mechanism whose precision varies with numerosity in a manner consistent with Weber's law. Critically, quantitative models based on either of these predictions can provide a reasonable description of subitizing performance, making it difficult to discriminate between these alternatives solely on the basis of subjects' behavioral performance. Here, we attempted to discriminate between fixed-capacity and continuous estimation models of subitizing using neural measures. In two experiments, we recorded EEGs while subjects performed a demanding subitizing task and examined set-size-dependent changes in a neurophysiological marker of visual selection (the N2pc event-related potential component) evoked by an array of to-be-enumerated items. In both experiments, N2pc amplitudes increased monotonically within the subitizing range before reaching an asymptotic limit at approximately three items. Moreover, inter-participant differences in the location of this asymptote were strongly predictive of behavioral estimates of subitizing span derived from a fixed-capacity model. Thus, neural activity linked with subitizing ability shows evidence of an early and discrete limit in the number of items that can be concurrently apprehended, supporting a fixed-capacity model of this process.
Author Vogel, Edward K.
Klee, Daniel
Ester, Edward F.
Awh, Edward
Drew, Trafton
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Edward F.
  surname: Ester
  fullname: Ester, Edward F.
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Trafton
  surname: Drew
  fullname: Drew, Trafton
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Daniel
  surname: Klee
  fullname: Klee, Daniel
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Edward K.
  surname: Vogel
  fullname: Vogel, Edward K.
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Edward
  surname: Awh
  fullname: Awh, Edward
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22623661$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkVtLxDAQhYMoul7-gvTRl66ZaZu0IIIsq66sCl6eQ9JONNKLNq2ov94WL6gvviSEOeebQ84mW62bmhjbBT6FBKP9s4v57dXl9WwxBYQ0BJwiB1xhk2GahRhzWGUTjpKHIpbxBtv0_oFzLjnIdbaBKDASAiZMXlDf6jI4J-37lnxwRc80vHVw7F6oCBYdVcHSVa4LXB1c98Z17s3Vd9tszerS087nvcVujuc3s9NweXmymB0twzyBqAu1zbUodIwmLYTFrEAuwWCcFdYSCWPQ8pxQREBJTDYtCpNJbaRFwS2YaIsdfmAfe1NRkVPdDWnVY-sq3b6qRjv1e1K7e3XXPKsokjxNswGw9wlom6eefKcq53MqS11T03sFSQIC4yiJ_5dyEEKChJG6-zPWd56vfx0EBx-CvG28b8mq3HW6c82Y0pUDS401qu8a1VjjcKixxsEu_ti_NvxjfAeXsqKU
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsycho_2021_108091
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_12703
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnagi_2016_00046
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_02371
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2023_1190317
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurobiolaging_2016_01_013
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00082_2016
crossref_primary_10_1177_0301006615594263
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15352
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep38434
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2017_02_012
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2860_17_2017
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01890_6
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02585_1
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2018_03_008
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2024_1335857
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_013_0203_5
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_024_06796_w
crossref_primary_10_1080_02643294_2017_1331212
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2014_01_003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2021_105118
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_013_0222_2
crossref_primary_10_6115_fer_2017_036
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0050862
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2013_02_004
crossref_primary_10_1080_00220671_2018_1486281
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_13485
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_75297_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_clinph_2022_02_026
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2014_03_006
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2019_102870
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2018_00460
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2022_104753
crossref_primary_10_1007_s13394_020_00349_4
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00649
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_750582
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1064_0
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_12948
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0131063
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2013_11_025
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_13219
crossref_primary_10_1111_tops_12248
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_020_20567_5
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_00522
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_12589
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyt_2020_544540
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2017_03_023
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_13375
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1152_1
crossref_primary_10_5964_jnc_v4i2_161
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2018_12_008
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2018_01_022
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_022_02580_6
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_10_013
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2962_19_2020
crossref_primary_10_3233_JAD_161274
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pediatrneurol_2016_02_007
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2015_12_047
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_00349
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_68788_4
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2015_00162
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2019_08_012
crossref_primary_10_1002_aur_1498
Cites_doi 10.1037/h0093759
10.1016/0010-0277(92)90050-R
10.1371/journal.pone.0020779
10.1038/23698
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02130.x
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01746.x
10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.121
10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.390
10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.011
10.1016/j.cognition.2005.10.004
10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.007
10.3758/PP.70.5.743
10.1163/156856897X00357
10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70447-7
10.1038/nature02447
10.1080/13506285.2011.603709
10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.80
10.1162/jocn.2009.21194
10.1038/003281a0
10.1162/jocn.2011.21602
10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.008
10.1093/cercor/bhm205
10.1007/BF00937136
10.1162/jocn.2009.21039
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4125-10.2011
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0556-08.2008
10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1000
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01949.x
10.3758/BF03207540
10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.005
10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01263-6
10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9
10.1371/journal.pone.0017453
10.1163/156856897X00366
10.2307/1418556
10.1037/0096-3445.111.1.1
10.1038/nature06860
10.1037/0096-1523.19.2.331
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2012 the authors 0270-6474/12/327169-09$15.00/0 2012
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2012 the authors 0270-6474/12/327169-09$15.00/0 2012
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
7TK
5PM
DOI 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1218-12.2012
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
DatabaseTitleList CrossRef
Neurosciences Abstracts

MEDLINE
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1529-2401
EndPage 7177
ExternalDocumentID PMC3370889
22623661
10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1218_12_2012
Genre Journal Article
Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIMH NIH HHS
  grantid: R01-MH087214
– fundername: NIMH NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 MH087214
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-~X
.55
18M
2WC
34G
39C
3O-
53G
5GY
5RE
5VS
AAFWJ
AAJMC
AAYXX
ABBAR
ABIVO
ACGUR
ACNCT
ADBBV
ADCOW
ADHGD
AENEX
AETEA
AFCFT
AFOSN
AFSQR
AHWXS
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
BAWUL
BTFSW
CITATION
CS3
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EBS
EJD
F5P
GX1
H13
HYE
H~9
KQ8
L7B
MVM
OK1
P0W
P2P
QZG
R.V
RHI
RPM
TFN
TR2
W8F
WH7
WOQ
X7M
XJT
YBU
YHG
YKV
YNH
YSK
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7X8
7TK
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c513t-afca6da42b8d6f29d2071b249dffee6bb2f0ce2631e54ef8ddb97ab7f260f1b3
ISSN 0270-6474
1529-2401
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:09:18 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 10:35:48 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 22:18:31 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 07:01:41 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 03:46:49 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:06:19 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 21
Language English
License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c513t-afca6da42b8d6f29d2071b249dffee6bb2f0ce2631e54ef8ddb97ab7f260f1b3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
Author contributions: E.F.E., T.D., E.K.V., and E.A. designed research; E.F.E. and D.K. performed research; E.F.E. and D.K. analyzed data; E.F.E., T.D., E.K.V., and E.A. wrote the paper.
OpenAccessLink https://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneuro/32/21/7169.full.pdf
PMID 22623661
PQID 1016671719
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 9
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3370889
proquest_miscellaneous_1551624354
proquest_miscellaneous_1016671719
pubmed_primary_22623661
crossref_citationtrail_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1218_12_2012
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1218_12_2012
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2012-05-23
2012-May-23
20120523
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2012-05-23
PublicationDate_xml – month: 05
  year: 2012
  text: 2012-05-23
  day: 23
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
PublicationTitle The Journal of neuroscience
PublicationTitleAlternate J Neurosci
PublicationYear 2012
Publisher Society for Neuroscience
Publisher_xml – name: Society for Neuroscience
References 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.17
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.39
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.18
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.19
Sperling (2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.30) 1960; 74
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.1
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.4
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.24
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.5
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.25
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.2
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.26
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.27
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.8
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.20
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.9
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.21
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.6
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.22
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.7
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.23
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.28
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.29
Trick (2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.32) 2008; 70
Balakrishnan (2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.3) 1991; 50
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.13
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.35
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.14
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.36
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.15
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.37
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.16
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.38
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.31
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.10
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.11
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.33
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.12
2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.34
7351125 - Cogn Psychol. 1980 Jan;12(1):97-136
17334209 - Cortex. 2007 Jan;43(1):77-94
15392567 - Am J Psychol. 1949 Oct;62(4):498-525
18042643 - Cereb Cortex. 2008 Aug;18(8):1788-98
8862112 - Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996 Sep;99(3):225-34
6460833 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 1982 Mar;111(1):1-22
22053147 - Vis cogn. 2011 Aug;19(7):956-972
18385672 - Nature. 2008 May 8;453(7192):233-5
21387002 - PLoS One. 2011;6(2):e17453
1780203 - Percept Psychophys. 1991 Dec;50(6):555-64
18578852 - Psychol Sci. 2008 Jun;19(6):607-14
1620801 - Psychol Res. 1992;54(2):80-90
21679934 - Cognition. 2011 Oct;121(1):147-53
15085132 - Nature. 2004 Apr 15;428(6984):748-51
1511586 - Cognition. 1992 Aug;44(1-2):43-74
7964526 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1994 Oct;20(5):1000-14
19269882 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Apr;13(4):167-74
18417697 - J Neurosci. 2008 Apr 16;28(16):4183-91
8121961 - Psychol Rev. 1994 Jan;101(1):80-102
20362427 - Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010 Apr;20(2):177-82
12669752 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2003 Feb;29(1):121-38
21695212 - PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e20779
21248137 - J Neurosci. 2011 Jan 19;31(3):1128-38
18613623 - Percept Psychophys. 2008 Jul;70(5):743-60
17614871 - Psychol Sci. 2007 Jul;18(7):622-8
9176953 - Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):437-42
9176952 - Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):433-6
23964915 - J Cogn Neurosci. 1993 Fall;5(4):390-407
19199425 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 Jan;22(1):32-47
16359652 - Cognition. 2006 Aug;101(1):217-45
18564048 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2009 Apr;21(4):760-75
9720604 - Trends Neurosci. 1998 Aug;21(8):355-61
21254796 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2011 Oct;23(10):2650-64
8473843 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1993 Apr;19(2):331-51
16866741 - Psychol Sci. 2006 Jul;17(7):572-6
20647016 - Vision Res. 2010 Sep 15;50(19):2000-7
10476964 - Nature. 1999 Aug 26;400(6747):867-9
References_xml – volume: 74
  start-page: 1
  year: 1960
  ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.30
  article-title: The information available in brief visual presentations
  publication-title: Psychol Monogr
  doi: 10.1037/h0093759
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.13
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90050-R
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.25
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020779
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.36
  doi: 10.1038/23698
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.28
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02130.x
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.15
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01746.x
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.37
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.121
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.8
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.390
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.24
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.011
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.14
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.10.004
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.27
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.007
– volume: 70
  start-page: 743
  year: 2008
  ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.32
  article-title: More than superstition: differential effects of featural heterogeneity and change on subitizing and counting
  publication-title: Percept Psychophys
  doi: 10.3758/PP.70.5.743
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.5
  doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.22
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70447-7
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.35
  doi: 10.1038/nature02447
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.29
  doi: 10.1080/13506285.2011.603709
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.31
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.34
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.80
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.7
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21194
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.17
  doi: 10.1038/003281a0
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.6
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21602
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.38
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.008
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.23
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm205
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.4
  doi: 10.1007/BF00937136
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.16
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21039
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.1
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4125-10.2011
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.10
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0556-08.2008
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.19
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1000
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.2
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01949.x
– volume: 50
  start-page: 555
  year: 1991
  ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.3
  article-title: Is subitizing a unique numerical ability?
  publication-title: Percept Psychophys
  doi: 10.3758/BF03207540
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.12
  doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.005
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.9
  doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01263-6
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.11
  doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.21
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017453
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.26
  doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.18
  doi: 10.2307/1418556
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.20
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.111.1.1
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.39
  doi: 10.1038/nature06860
– ident: 2023041303563277000_32.21.7169.33
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.2.331
– reference: 15085132 - Nature. 2004 Apr 15;428(6984):748-51
– reference: 17614871 - Psychol Sci. 2007 Jul;18(7):622-8
– reference: 21695212 - PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e20779
– reference: 16866741 - Psychol Sci. 2006 Jul;17(7):572-6
– reference: 9176953 - Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):437-42
– reference: 10476964 - Nature. 1999 Aug 26;400(6747):867-9
– reference: 18417697 - J Neurosci. 2008 Apr 16;28(16):4183-91
– reference: 18578852 - Psychol Sci. 2008 Jun;19(6):607-14
– reference: 1620801 - Psychol Res. 1992;54(2):80-90
– reference: 21254796 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2011 Oct;23(10):2650-64
– reference: 20362427 - Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010 Apr;20(2):177-82
– reference: 8121961 - Psychol Rev. 1994 Jan;101(1):80-102
– reference: 23964915 - J Cogn Neurosci. 1993 Fall;5(4):390-407
– reference: 1780203 - Percept Psychophys. 1991 Dec;50(6):555-64
– reference: 20647016 - Vision Res. 2010 Sep 15;50(19):2000-7
– reference: 9720604 - Trends Neurosci. 1998 Aug;21(8):355-61
– reference: 18042643 - Cereb Cortex. 2008 Aug;18(8):1788-98
– reference: 21679934 - Cognition. 2011 Oct;121(1):147-53
– reference: 12669752 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2003 Feb;29(1):121-38
– reference: 21248137 - J Neurosci. 2011 Jan 19;31(3):1128-38
– reference: 19199425 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 Jan;22(1):32-47
– reference: 9176952 - Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):433-6
– reference: 7351125 - Cogn Psychol. 1980 Jan;12(1):97-136
– reference: 21387002 - PLoS One. 2011;6(2):e17453
– reference: 22053147 - Vis cogn. 2011 Aug;19(7):956-972
– reference: 8473843 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1993 Apr;19(2):331-51
– reference: 17334209 - Cortex. 2007 Jan;43(1):77-94
– reference: 18385672 - Nature. 2008 May 8;453(7192):233-5
– reference: 6460833 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 1982 Mar;111(1):1-22
– reference: 8862112 - Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996 Sep;99(3):225-34
– reference: 15392567 - Am J Psychol. 1949 Oct;62(4):498-525
– reference: 19269882 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Apr;13(4):167-74
– reference: 16359652 - Cognition. 2006 Aug;101(1):217-45
– reference: 7964526 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1994 Oct;20(5):1000-14
– reference: 1511586 - Cognition. 1992 Aug;44(1-2):43-74
– reference: 18564048 - J Cogn Neurosci. 2009 Apr;21(4):760-75
– reference: 18613623 - Percept Psychophys. 2008 Jul;70(5):743-60
SSID ssj0007017
Score 2.3520908
Snippet For centuries, it has been known that humans can rapidly and accurately enumerate small sets of items, a process referred to as subitizing. However, there is...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
StartPage 7169
SubjectTerms Adult
Evoked Potentials, Visual - physiology
Female
Humans
Male
Photic Stimulation - methods
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Reaction Time
Visual Perception - physiology
Title Neural Measures Reveal a Fixed Item Limit in Subitizing
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22623661
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1016671719
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1551624354
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC3370889
Volume 32
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3db9MwELdgvPCCgPFRvmQkxAsKS5zYbh6n0WpsowiRob5FdmJDEKRopAj213Nn56tQweAlqtwkrnzX8_18d78j5IkKDZg8boN4qmWQpFoEKS8xp0EB3lCp4AaB4quFODxNjpZ8ObS5c9UljX5enG-tK_kfqcIYyBWrZP9Bsv1LYQA-g3zhChKG64VkjMwarrbWnfNhxcc35AlWz-bVd3Ak8RjeVzDhqQaYiKqpzrut6uOgJiOndERv2Ut81jXv8A2eh1zgF2fGBYZgv7PNKJzfku_76vVu9N3qvU8IaN9yPD5vwMQNHviS4M5EMheTicY2dDijXHclz61FRDae0e4K6FFutdzcMUgcLTCB8e3BS2S9AHiLpXJtmvUGVfbidT4_PTnJs9kyu0yuMMAI2L7i-M1AFS9D1265_7lteTjMs7d9lk3P5De48WvW7MgNya6Ta62o6L5Xhhvkkqlvkt39WjWrzz_oU-oyel2oZJdIrx-00w_q9YMq6vSDon5Qpx-0qumgH7dINp9lB4dB2ygjKHgUN4GyhRKlSpielsKytGTgOGoA1qW1xgitmQ0Lw0QcGZ4YOy1LnUqlpQUwayMd3yY79ao2dwlVsP9pwNhlFGKJsdRCmUJHhZ3GSWiknRDerVFetCTy2MvkU45gEtY279c2x7WFS45rOyF7_XNfPI3KX5943IkgB4uHYSxVm9X6q0tKFKBHUfqHezD-ywAKJBNyx4utnxcAB4vBLZ0QuSHQ_gZkXN_8pq4-OOb1OJaYFnjvAvPeJ1eHf88DstOcrc1D8F8b_cjp6U-zW5iG
linkProvider Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Neural+Measures+Reveal+a+Fixed+Item+Limit+in+Subitizing&rft.jtitle=The+Journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Ester%2C+Edward+F&rft.au=Drew%2C+Trafton&rft.au=Klee%2C+Daniel&rft.au=Vogel%2C+Edward+K&rft.date=2012-05-23&rft.issn=1529-2401&rft.volume=32&rft.issue=21&rft.spage=7169&rft.epage=7177&rft_id=info:doi/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.1218-12.2012&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon