Baseline cortisol levels and social behavior differ as a function of handedness in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)
Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual's preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left‐...
Saved in:
Published in | American journal of primatology Vol. 81; no. 9; pp. e23057 - n/a |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
01.09.2019
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual's preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left‐handed individuals tend to be more fearful than their right‐handed counterparts. Based on these findings, we tested the hypotheses that left‐handed marmosets are (a) more reactive to a social stressor and (b) are slower than right‐handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. We examined the hand preference of 27 male and female marmosets (ages of 4–7 years old) previously tested in a social separation task and a reversal learning task. Hand preference was determined via a simple reaching task. In the social separation task, monkeys were separated from their partner and the colony for a single 7‐hr session. Urinary cortisol levels and behavior were assessed at baseline, during the separation and 24 hr postseparation. Hand preferences were equally distributed between left (n = 10), right‐handed (n = 10), and ambidextrous (n = 7) individuals. The separation phase was associated with an increase in cortisol levels and behavioral changes that were similar across handedness groups. However, cortisol levels at baseline were positively correlated with right‐handedness, and this relationship was stronger in females than in males. In addition, the occurrence of social behaviors (pre‐ and postseparation) was positively correlated with right‐handedness in both sexes. Baseline cortisol levels did not correlate significantly with social behavior. Acquisition of the reversals was poorer in females than males but did not differ as a function of handedness. We conclude that (a) both stress reactivity and cognitive flexibility are similar across handedness groups and (b) left‐handers exhibit less social behavior and have lower basal cortisol levels than ambidextrous and right‐handed subjects. The underlying causes for these differences remain to be established.
Figure 4. Basal cortisol correlates with right‐handedness in female marmosets.
Research Highlights
We tested the hypothesis that left‐handed marmosets are more reactive to a social stressor and are slower than right‐handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task.
Neither cortisol levels during the social stressor or cognitive performance differed as a function of handedness.
Basal cortisol was positively correlated with right‐handedness in females.
Social interactions with a partner increased with right‐handedness in both sexes.
Higher basal cortisol and female sex independently predicted poorer cognitive performance. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual's preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left-handed individuals tend to be more fearful than their right-handed counterparts. Based on these findings, we tested the hypotheses that left-handed marmosets are (a) more reactive to a social stressor and (b) are slower than right-handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. We examined the hand preference of 27 male and female marmosets (ages of 4-7 years old) previously tested in a social separation task and a reversal learning task. Hand preference was determined via a simple reaching task. In the social separation task, monkeys were separated from their partner and the colony for a single 7-hr session. Urinary cortisol levels and behavior were assessed at baseline, during the separation and 24 hr postseparation. Hand preferences were equally distributed between left (n = 10), right-handed (n = 10), and ambidextrous (n = 7) individuals. The separation phase was associated with an increase in cortisol levels and behavioral changes that were similar across handedness groups. However, cortisol levels at baseline were positively correlated with right-handedness, and this relationship was stronger in females than in males. In addition, the occurrence of social behaviors (pre- and postseparation) was positively correlated with right-handedness in both sexes. Baseline cortisol levels did not correlate significantly with social behavior. Acquisition of the reversals was poorer in females than males but did not differ as a function of handedness. We conclude that (a) both stress reactivity and cognitive flexibility are similar across handedness groups and (b) left-handers exhibit less social behavior and have lower basal cortisol levels than ambidextrous and right-handed subjects. The underlying causes for these differences remain to be established. Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual’s preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left-handed individuals tend to be more fearful than their right-handed counterparts. Based on these findings, we tested the hypotheses that left-handed marmosets are (1) more reactive to a social stressor and (2) are slower than right-handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. We examined the hand preference of 27 male and female marmosets (ages of 4–7 years old) previously tested in a social separation task and a reversal learning task. Hand preference was determined via a simple reaching task. In the social separation task, monkeys were separated from their partner and the colony for a single 7-hour session. Urinary cortisol levels and behavior were assessed at baseline, during the separation and 24h post-separation. Hand preferences were equally distributed between left (n=10), right-handed (n=10) and ambidextrous (n=7) individuals. The separation phase was associated with an increase in cortisol levels and behavioral changes that were similar across handedness groups. However, cortisol levels at baseline were positively correlated with right-handedness, and this relationship was stronger in females than in males. In addition, the occurrence of social behaviors (pre- and post- separation) was positively correlated with right-handedness in both sexes. Baseline cortisol levels did not correlate significantly with social behavior. Acquisition of the reversals was poorer in females than males but did not differ as a function of handedness. We conclude that (1) both stress reactivity and cognitive flexibility are similar across handedness groups; (2) left-handers exhibit less social behavior and have lower basal cortisol levels than ambidextrous and right-handed subjects. The underlying causes for these differences remain to be established. Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual's preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left‐handed individuals tend to be more fearful than their right‐handed counterparts. Based on these findings, we tested the hypotheses that left‐handed marmosets are (a) more reactive to a social stressor and (b) are slower than right‐handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. We examined the hand preference of 27 male and female marmosets (ages of 4–7 years old) previously tested in a social separation task and a reversal learning task. Hand preference was determined via a simple reaching task. In the social separation task, monkeys were separated from their partner and the colony for a single 7‐hr session. Urinary cortisol levels and behavior were assessed at baseline, during the separation and 24 hr postseparation. Hand preferences were equally distributed between left ( n = 10), right‐handed ( n = 10), and ambidextrous ( n = 7) individuals. The separation phase was associated with an increase in cortisol levels and behavioral changes that were similar across handedness groups. However, cortisol levels at baseline were positively correlated with right‐handedness, and this relationship was stronger in females than in males. In addition, the occurrence of social behaviors (pre‐ and postseparation) was positively correlated with right‐handedness in both sexes. Baseline cortisol levels did not correlate significantly with social behavior. Acquisition of the reversals was poorer in females than males but did not differ as a function of handedness. We conclude that (a) both stress reactivity and cognitive flexibility are similar across handedness groups and (b) left‐handers exhibit less social behavior and have lower basal cortisol levels than ambidextrous and right‐handed subjects. The underlying causes for these differences remain to be established. We tested the hypothesis that left‐handed marmosets are more reactive to a social stressor and are slower than right‐handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. Neither cortisol levels during the social stressor or cognitive performance differed as a function of handedness. Basal cortisol was positively correlated with right‐handedness in females. Social interactions with a partner increased with right‐handedness in both sexes. Higher basal cortisol and female sex independently predicted poorer cognitive performance. Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual's preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left-handed individuals tend to be more fearful than their right-handed counterparts. Based on these findings, we tested the hypotheses that left-handed marmosets are (a) more reactive to a social stressor and (b) are slower than right-handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. We examined the hand preference of 27 male and female marmosets (ages of 4-7 years old) previously tested in a social separation task and a reversal learning task. Hand preference was determined via a simple reaching task. In the social separation task, monkeys were separated from their partner and the colony for a single 7-hr session. Urinary cortisol levels and behavior were assessed at baseline, during the separation and 24 hr postseparation. Hand preferences were equally distributed between left (n = 10), right-handed (n = 10), and ambidextrous (n = 7) individuals. The separation phase was associated with an increase in cortisol levels and behavioral changes that were similar across handedness groups. However, cortisol levels at baseline were positively correlated with right-handedness, and this relationship was stronger in females than in males. In addition, the occurrence of social behaviors (pre- and postseparation) was positively correlated with right-handedness in both sexes. Baseline cortisol levels did not correlate significantly with social behavior. Acquisition of the reversals was poorer in females than males but did not differ as a function of handedness. We conclude that (a) both stress reactivity and cognitive flexibility are similar across handedness groups and (b) left-handers exhibit less social behavior and have lower basal cortisol levels than ambidextrous and right-handed subjects. The underlying causes for these differences remain to be established.Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual's preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left-handed individuals tend to be more fearful than their right-handed counterparts. Based on these findings, we tested the hypotheses that left-handed marmosets are (a) more reactive to a social stressor and (b) are slower than right-handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. We examined the hand preference of 27 male and female marmosets (ages of 4-7 years old) previously tested in a social separation task and a reversal learning task. Hand preference was determined via a simple reaching task. In the social separation task, monkeys were separated from their partner and the colony for a single 7-hr session. Urinary cortisol levels and behavior were assessed at baseline, during the separation and 24 hr postseparation. Hand preferences were equally distributed between left (n = 10), right-handed (n = 10), and ambidextrous (n = 7) individuals. The separation phase was associated with an increase in cortisol levels and behavioral changes that were similar across handedness groups. However, cortisol levels at baseline were positively correlated with right-handedness, and this relationship was stronger in females than in males. In addition, the occurrence of social behaviors (pre- and postseparation) was positively correlated with right-handedness in both sexes. Baseline cortisol levels did not correlate significantly with social behavior. Acquisition of the reversals was poorer in females than males but did not differ as a function of handedness. We conclude that (a) both stress reactivity and cognitive flexibility are similar across handedness groups and (b) left-handers exhibit less social behavior and have lower basal cortisol levels than ambidextrous and right-handed subjects. The underlying causes for these differences remain to be established. Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an individual's preference to use a particular hemisphere when engaged in a specific task. Previous findings in marmosets have indicated that left‐handed individuals tend to be more fearful than their right‐handed counterparts. Based on these findings, we tested the hypotheses that left‐handed marmosets are (a) more reactive to a social stressor and (b) are slower than right‐handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. We examined the hand preference of 27 male and female marmosets (ages of 4–7 years old) previously tested in a social separation task and a reversal learning task. Hand preference was determined via a simple reaching task. In the social separation task, monkeys were separated from their partner and the colony for a single 7‐hr session. Urinary cortisol levels and behavior were assessed at baseline, during the separation and 24 hr postseparation. Hand preferences were equally distributed between left (n = 10), right‐handed (n = 10), and ambidextrous (n = 7) individuals. The separation phase was associated with an increase in cortisol levels and behavioral changes that were similar across handedness groups. However, cortisol levels at baseline were positively correlated with right‐handedness, and this relationship was stronger in females than in males. In addition, the occurrence of social behaviors (pre‐ and postseparation) was positively correlated with right‐handedness in both sexes. Baseline cortisol levels did not correlate significantly with social behavior. Acquisition of the reversals was poorer in females than males but did not differ as a function of handedness. We conclude that (a) both stress reactivity and cognitive flexibility are similar across handedness groups and (b) left‐handers exhibit less social behavior and have lower basal cortisol levels than ambidextrous and right‐handed subjects. The underlying causes for these differences remain to be established. Figure 4. Basal cortisol correlates with right‐handedness in female marmosets. Research Highlights We tested the hypothesis that left‐handed marmosets are more reactive to a social stressor and are slower than right‐handed marmosets in acquiring a reversal learning task. Neither cortisol levels during the social stressor or cognitive performance differed as a function of handedness. Basal cortisol was positively correlated with right‐handedness in females. Social interactions with a partner increased with right‐handedness in both sexes. Higher basal cortisol and female sex independently predicted poorer cognitive performance. |
Author | Workman, Kathryn P. Casey, Michaela Vaughan, Emma Le, Annie Lacreuse, Agnès |
AuthorAffiliation | 2 Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst MA, USA 1 Department of Veterinary and Animal Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst MA, USA 3 Center for Neuroendocrine Studies, University of Massachusetts, Amherst MA, USA |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 2 Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst MA, USA – name: 3 Center for Neuroendocrine Studies, University of Massachusetts, Amherst MA, USA – name: 1 Department of Veterinary and Animal Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst MA, USA |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Emma surname: Vaughan fullname: Vaughan, Emma organization: University of Massachusetts – sequence: 2 givenname: Annie surname: Le fullname: Le, Annie organization: University of Massachusetts – sequence: 3 givenname: Michaela surname: Casey fullname: Casey, Michaela organization: University of Massachusetts – sequence: 4 givenname: Kathryn P. surname: Workman fullname: Workman, Kathryn P. organization: University of Massachusetts – sequence: 5 givenname: Agnès orcidid: 0000-0003-2890-9649 surname: Lacreuse fullname: Lacreuse, Agnès email: alacreuse@psych.umass.edu organization: University of Massachusetts |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31566763$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kU9v1DAQxS1URLeFA18AWeLSHtLaScbZXJDKir-qBAc4WxNnQrzy2ls7Wei3x8uWCirBwZrD_Ob5Pb0TduSDJ8aeS3EhhSgvcb29KCsBzSO2kKJdFmVVwxFbiLKBogQFx-wkpbUQUtYKnrDjSoJSjaoW7OY1JnLWEzchTjYFxx3tyCWOvucpGIuOdzTizobIezsMFDnmLR9mbyYbPA8DHzNMvaeUuPV8g3ETEk2Jn63QOTuN0f7gazRmnNP5U_Z4QJfo2d08ZV_fvvmyel9cf3r3YXV1XRgQbVOgBDm0SB31SrUEfX5SKZAAHTWAwhB2y55yICDArm-6ZdeV2A5mCQrr6pS9Ouhu525DvSE_RXR6G232d6sDWv33xttRfws73cpKVqCywNmdQAw3M6VJb2wy5Bx6CnPSZdm2dd007f6vlw_QdZijz_F07qURNdRLkakXfzq6t_K7jQycHwATQ0qRhntECr1vWuem9a-mM3v5gDV2wn0hOYx1_7v4bh3d_ltaX338fLj4CelFu_A |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2021_105279 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pneurobio_2020_101823 crossref_primary_10_3390_sym15040926 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23499 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani12213002 crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_023_04894_z crossref_primary_10_3390_sym13122349 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_220797 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_73811_9 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2024_106177 crossref_primary_10_30970_vlubs_2024_92_12 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajpa_24900 crossref_primary_10_3389_fvets_2023_1242906 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80053-X 10.1023/A:1020822401195 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90246-1 10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80014-8 10.1007/BF02381379 10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.149 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.003 10.1037/a0013089 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.009 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.004 10.1080/1357650X.2013.796965 10.1007/978-1-4613-8139-6 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.01.025 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00050-X 10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.538 10.1210/jcem.83.10.5164 10.1017/S0140525X00047695 10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.02.012 10.1016/S0010-9452(89)80021-8 10.1002/1098-2302(200103)38:2<110::AID-DEV1003>3.0.CO;2-# 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02893.1994 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1977.tb41921.x 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00037-2 10.1080/13576500244000265 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00413.x 10.1080/713754297 10.1037/a0024831 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1436 10.1016/j.paid.2008.08.019 10.1523/ENEURO.0154-19.2019 10.1038/s41598-018-28112-7 10.1093/brain/123.1.74 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.006 10.3758/BF03337470 10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.76 10.1016/0028-3932(85)90027-2 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.11.007 10.1371/journal.pone.0101165 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181b970ae 10.1080/1357650X.2011.615126 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.07.027 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.037 10.1037/0735-7044.108.6.1207 10.3758/BF03195341 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.810 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.001 10.46867/C4759T 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.005 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.040 10.1002/ajp.22924 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2019 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2019 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7QG 7SN 7TK 8BJ 8FD C1K FQK FR3 JBE K9. P64 RC3 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1002/ajp.23057 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management International Bibliography of the Social Sciences Engineering Research Database International Bibliography of the Social Sciences ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed Genetics Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Technology Research Database Animal Behavior Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Engineering Research Database Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | PubMed CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic Genetics Abstracts |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anthropology Zoology Psychology |
EISSN | 1098-2345 |
EndPage | n/a |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC9131356 31566763 10_1002_ajp_23057 AJP23057 |
Genre | article Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIH funderid: R01AG‐046266 – fundername: William Lee Science Impact fellowship – fundername: NIA NIH HHS grantid: R01 AG046266 |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ .3N .GA .Y3 05W 0R~ 10A 1L6 1OB 1OC 1ZS 23M 31~ 33P 36B 3SF 3WU 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51X 52M 52N 52O 52P 52S 52T 52U 52W 52X 53G 5GY 5RE 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A03 AAESR AAEVG AAHHS AAHQN AAMNL AANHP AANLZ AAONW AASGY AAXRX AAYCA AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABEML ABIJN ABIVO ABPVW ACAHQ ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFS ACPOU ACPRK ACRPL ACSCC ACXBN ACXQS ACYXJ ADBBV ADEOM ADIYS ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADNMO ADOZA ADXAS ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEIGN AEIMD AENEX AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFFPM AFGKR AFPWT AFRAH AFWVQ AFZJQ AHBTC AHMBA AITYG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE AJXKR ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN ALVPJ AMBMR AMYDB ASPBG ATUGU AUFTA AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BHBCM BMNLL BMXJE BNHUX BROTX BRXPI BY8 CS3 D-E D-F DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRSTM DVXWH EBS ECGQY EJD F00 F01 F04 F5P FEDTE G-S G.N GNP GODZA H.T H.X HBH HF~ HGLYW HHY HHZ HVGLF HZ~ IX1 J0M JPC KQQ LATKE LAW LC2 LC3 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES M66 MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRSTM MSFUL MSSTM MXFUL MXSTM N04 N05 N9A NF~ NNB O66 O9- OIG P2P P2W P2X P4D PALCI PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 QRW R.K RIWAO RJQFR ROL RWI RWV RX1 RYL SAMSI SUPJJ TN5 UB1 V2E VQA W8V W99 WBKPD WIB WIH WIK WJL WNSPC WOHZO WQJ WRC WTM WXSBR WYISQ XG1 XV2 ZCG ZZTAW ~02 ~IA ~WT AAYXX AEYWJ AGHNM AGQPQ AGYGG CITATION NPM VXZ 7QG 7SN 7TK 8BJ 8FD AAMMB AEFGJ AGXDD AIDQK AIDYY C1K FQK FR3 JBE K9. P64 RC3 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c5097-a151f9aebed669e5d9e51665155be75a0ceab8de4655e5abd7b8bb2a9fc856a43 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 0275-2565 1098-2345 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 13:57:16 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 16:32:08 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 01:55:41 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:31:04 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:53:19 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:10:34 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:38:13 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 9 |
Keywords | primate stress cognition hand preference laterality |
Language | English |
License | 2019 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c5097-a151f9aebed669e5d9e51665155be75a0ceab8de4655e5abd7b8bb2a9fc856a43 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0003-2890-9649 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/9131356 |
PMID | 31566763 |
PQID | 2307045480 |
PQPubID | 866340 |
PageCount | 10 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9131356 proquest_miscellaneous_2299447794 proquest_journals_2307045480 pubmed_primary_31566763 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23057 crossref_citationtrail_10_1002_ajp_23057 wiley_primary_10_1002_ajp_23057_AJP23057 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | September 2019 2019-09-00 20190901 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2019-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 09 year: 2019 text: September 2019 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: New York |
PublicationTitle | American journal of primatology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Am J Primatol |
PublicationYear | 2019 |
Publisher | Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
References | e_1_2_9_1_15_1 e_1_2_9_1_13_1 e_1_2_9_1_34_1 e_1_2_9_1_11_1 e_1_2_9_1_32_1 Wright L. (e_1_2_9_1_55_1) 2015; 6 e_1_2_9_1_30_1 e_1_2_9_1_4_1 e_1_2_9_1_6_1 e_1_2_9_1_29_1 e_1_2_9_1_27_1 e_1_2_9_1_25_1 e_1_2_9_1_48_1 e_1_2_9_1_23_1 e_1_2_9_1_46_1 Jaju D. S. (e_1_2_9_1_20_1) 2004; 48 e_1_2_9_1_21_1 e_1_2_9_1_44_1 e_1_2_9_1_42_1 e_1_2_9_1_40_1 e_1_2_9_1_50_1 e_1_2_9_1_52_1 e_1_2_9_1_54_1 e_1_2_9_1_18_1 e_1_2_9_1_56_1 e_1_2_9_1_16_1 e_1_2_9_1_39_1 e_1_2_9_1_14_1 e_1_2_9_1_37_1 Prichard E. (e_1_2_9_1_38_1) 2013; 4 e_1_2_9_1_12_1 e_1_2_9_1_35_1 e_1_2_9_1_10_1 e_1_2_9_1_33_1 e_1_2_9_1_31_1 Rogers L. (e_1_2_9_1_41_1) 1999; 12 e_1_2_9_1_3_1 e_1_2_9_1_5_1 e_1_2_9_1_28_1 e_1_2_9_1_49_1 e_1_2_9_1_26_1 e_1_2_9_1_47_1 e_1_2_9_1_24_1 e_1_2_9_1_45_1 e_1_2_9_1_22_1 e_1_2_9_1_43_1 Annett M. (e_1_2_9_1_2_1) 1985 e_1_2_9_1_7_1 Davidson R. (e_1_2_9_1_8_1) 1983 e_1_2_9_1_9_1 Porac C. (e_1_2_9_1_36_1) 1981 e_1_2_9_1_51_1 e_1_2_9_1_53_1 e_1_2_9_1_19_1 e_1_2_9_1_17_1 |
References_xml | – volume-title: Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory year: 1985 ident: e_1_2_9_1_2_1 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_46_1 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80053-X – ident: e_1_2_9_1_15_1 doi: 10.1023/A:1020822401195 – volume: 48 start-page: 31 issue: 1 year: 2004 ident: e_1_2_9_1_20_1 article-title: Heart rate and blood pressure responses of left‐handers and right‐handers to autonomic stressors publication-title: Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology – volume: 4 issue: 9 year: 2013 ident: e_1_2_9_1_38_1 article-title: Degree of handedness, but not direction, is a systematic predictor of cognitive performance publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology – ident: e_1_2_9_1_43_1 doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90246-1 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_12_1 doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80014-8 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_25_1 doi: 10.1007/BF02381379 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_5_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.2.149 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_45_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.003 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_4_1 doi: 10.1037/a0013089 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_7_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.009 – start-page: 249 year: 1983 ident: e_1_2_9_1_8_1 article-title: Affect and disorders of affect: Behavioral and electrophysiological studies publication-title: Laterality and Psychopathology – ident: e_1_2_9_1_42_1 doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.004 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_31_1 doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.796965 – start-page: 181 volume-title: Lateral preferences and human behavior year: 1981 ident: e_1_2_9_1_36_1 doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-8139-6 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_23_1 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.01.025 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_3_1 doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00050-X – volume: 6 issue: 58 year: 2015 ident: e_1_2_9_1_55_1 article-title: Left‐handers look before they leap: Handedness influences reactivity to novel Tower of Hanoi tasks publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology – ident: e_1_2_9_1_16_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.538 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_21_1 doi: 10.1210/jcem.83.10.5164 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_29_1 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00047695 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_49_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.02.012 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_32_1 doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(89)80021-8 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_51_1 doi: 10.1002/1098-2302(200103)38:2<110::AID-DEV1003>3.0.CO;2-# – ident: e_1_2_9_1_27_1 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02893.1994 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_40_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1977.tb41921.x – ident: e_1_2_9_1_13_1 doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00037-2 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_53_1 doi: 10.1080/13576500244000265 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_48_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00413.x – ident: e_1_2_9_1_14_1 doi: 10.1080/713754297 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_28_1 doi: 10.1037/a0024831 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_44_1 doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1436 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_56_1 doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.08.019 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_24_1 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0154-19.2019 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_19_1 doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28112-7 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_22_1 doi: 10.1093/brain/123.1.74 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_10_1 doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.006 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_50_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03337470 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_9_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.76 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_18_1 doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(85)90027-2 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_47_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.11.007 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_30_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101165 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_6_1 doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181b970ae – ident: e_1_2_9_1_54_1 doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2011.615126 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_34_1 doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.07.027 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_11_1 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.037 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_17_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.108.6.1207 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_39_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03195341 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_26_1 doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.810 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_33_1 doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.001 – volume: 12 issue: 2 year: 1999 ident: e_1_2_9_1_41_1 article-title: Factors associated with exploration in marmosets: Age, gender and hand preference publication-title: International Journal of Comparative Psychology doi: 10.46867/C4759T – ident: e_1_2_9_1_35_1 doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.005 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_37_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.040 – ident: e_1_2_9_1_52_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.22924 |
SSID | ssj0011465 |
Score | 2.313116 |
Snippet | Population hand preferences are rare in nonhuman primates, but individual hand preferences are consistent over a lifetime and considered to reflect an... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref wiley |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | e23057 |
SubjectTerms | Behavior change Callithrix jacchus cognition Cognitive ability Cognitive flexibility Cognitive tasks Correlation Cortisol Females hand preference Handedness Hormones laterality Learning Males Monkeys Monkeys & apes Preferences primate Primates Reactivity Reversal Reversal learning Separation Sex differences Social behavior Social factors Social interaction Social interactions stress Urinary cortisol |
Title | Baseline cortisol levels and social behavior differ as a function of handedness in marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) |
URI | https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002%2Fajp.23057 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31566763 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2307045480 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2299447794 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9131356 |
Volume | 81 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3da9RAEB9KQSiCH_WjqVVW8aE-pL1LsrsJPtViKQVFxEIRIcxu9rir16S95ED9653ZfNizCuLDQY6dkE3ym52ZzcxvAF5GURa5tLDhSDoKUDSFO2gVhgnTBCRW6ijl4uR379XxaXJyJs_W4HVfC9PyQwwbbqwZfr1mBUdT7_8iDcXzS85illxJzrla7BB9HKijuNjWpy9GWoZk1mXPKjSK9oczV23RDQfzZp7kdf_VG6Cju_Cln3qbd_J1b9mYPfvjN1bH_7y3e3Cnc0zFQYuk-7Dmyk24fa2PwvdN2BiWS_pz63Pljx7A1Rv0Ze1OUCjbzAjNYs65SLXAshDtrrzo-QBE25JFII0KtqqMDFFNBG_hu4JXXjErxQUuLqraNbXYPcQ5xQrTxeybOEdrp8v61UM4PXr76fA47Fo5hJY8Eh0iORaTDAkxhVKZkwX9xorbsEvjtMSRdWjSwjGbm5NoCm1SYyLMJjaVCpP4EayXVem2QBRqbJ3hXpPMjW8c8pfNTDlCXZoksQ5gt3-pue14zrndxjxvGZqjnJ5u7p9uAC8G0cuW3ONPQjs9MvJOv2se0UxemI4CeD4Mk2by5xYsXbUkGbL0SaJplgE8boE0XCXmsJmW9gD0CsQGAWb9Xh0pZ1PP_p2N43EsFd2mR9DfJ54fnHzwB9v_LvoENsgj7JLodmC9WSzdU_K6GvPMq9dPblsqJQ |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3da9RAEB9qRSyCH7Xa1Kqr-NA-pL1LsrsJ-FKL5axtEWmhCBJ2N3vctdekXnJQ_eud2XzYswriQyBhJ2ST_GZnZnf2NwBvgiAJbJwZv8ctBigSwx1lhPIjogmIDJdBTJuTD4_E4CTaP-WnC_C23QtT80N0E26kGW68JgWnCentX6yh6uyS0pi5vAW3qaK3C6g-d-RRtN3WJTAGkvto2HnLK9QLtrtb563RDRfzZqbkdQ_WmaC9B_C17XydeXK-Nav0lvnxG6_j_77dQ7jf-KZspwbTI1iw-TLcu1ZK4fsyLHUjJl7c-VK4s8fw7Z1yO9stw2i2GiOg2YTSkUqm8ozVE_OspQRgdVUWprCVkWElcLBiyGgW32Y0-LJxzi7U9KIobVWyjV01wXBhNB1fsTNlzGhWbq7Ayd77492B31Rz8A06JdJX6FsME4WgyYRILM_w6AuqxM61lVz1jFU6ziwRulmudCZ1rHWgkqGJuVBR-AQW8yK3q8Ay0TdWU7lJosfXVtHiZiIsAi-OolB6sNH-1dQ0VOdUcWOS1iTNQYpfN3Vf14PXnehlze_xJ6H1Fhppo-IltUjiL4x7HrzqmlE5acVF5baYoQwa-yiS2EsPntZI6p4SUuSMo7sHcg5jnQARf8-35OORIwBP-mE_5AJf00Ho7x1Pd_Y_uZO1fxd9CXcHx4cH6cGHo4_PYAkdxCanbh0Wq-nMPkcnrNIvnK79BOfALkA |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3da9RAEB9qRSmCH_UrWnUVH-pD2lyS3U3wqbYetWopYqGIEHY3e9zVa3JecqD-9c5sPuxZBfEhkLATskl-szOzO_sbgOdhmIY2yY0fcIsBisRwRxmh_JhoAmLDZZjQ5uT3h2L_OD444Scr8LLbC9PwQ_QTbqQZbrwmBZ_lo-1fpKHqdEZZzFxegsuxCBKC9N6HnjuKdtu6_MVQch_tOu9ohYJwu7912Rhd8DAvJkqed2CdBRregM9d35vEky9bi1pvmR-_0Tr-58vdhOutZ8p2GijdghVbrMO1c4UUvq_DWj9e4sWVT6U7uw1fXym3r90yjGXrCcKZTSkZqWKqyFkzLc86QgDW1GRhClsZmVWCBitHjObwbU5DL5sU7EzNz8rK1hXb3FVTDBbG88k3dqqMGS-qF3fgePj64-6-39Zy8A26JNJX6FmMUoWQyYVILc_xGAiqw861lVwFxiqd5Jbo3CxXOpc60TpU6cgkXKg4ugurRVnY-8ByMTBWU7FJIsfXVtHSZioswi6J40h6sNn91My0ROdUb2OaNRTNYYZfN3Nf14NnveisYff4k9BGh4ysVfCKWiSxFyaBB0_7ZlRNWm9RhS0XKIOmPo4l9tKDew2Q-qdEFDfj2O6BXIJYL0C038stxWTs6L_TQTSIuMDXdAj6e8eznYMjd_Lg30WfwNWjvWH27s3h24ewht5hm1C3Aav1fGEfoQdW68dO034CYqQs-A |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Baseline+cortisol+levels+and+social+behavior+differ+as+a+function+of+handedness+in+marmosets+%28Callithrix+jacchus%29&rft.jtitle=American+journal+of+primatology&rft.au=Vaughan%2C+Emma&rft.au=Le%2C+Annie&rft.au=Casey%2C+Michaela&rft.au=Workman%2C+Kathryn+P&rft.date=2019-09-01&rft.eissn=1098-2345&rft.volume=81&rft.issue=9&rft.spage=e23057&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002%2Fajp.23057&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F31566763&rft.externalDocID=31566763 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon |