Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in U.S. Zoos, Sanctuaries, and Research Facilities: A Survey-Based Comparison of Species-Typical Behaviors
A survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex, social group-size, rearing history, and enclosure. Respondents were also asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each ch...
Saved in:
Published in | Animals (Basel) Vol. 13; no. 2; p. 251 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Switzerland
MDPI AG
10.01.2023
MDPI |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | A survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex, social group-size, rearing history, and enclosure. Respondents were also asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each chimpanzee over the prior two years. Species- typical behaviors (STBs) were queried, including copulation, tool-use, nest-building, and social grooming. Tool-use was reported present for 94.3% of the sample (n = 982), active social grooming for 85.7% (n = 1121), copulation for 68.3% (n = 863) and nest-building for 58.9% (n = 982). Of the subjects for whom we had data regarding all four STBs (n = 860), 45.6% were reported to engage in all four. Logistic regression analyses using forward Wald criteria were conducted to determine the best model for each STB based on the predictors of age, sex, rearing history, group-size, facility-type, and a sex-by-rearing interaction. The best model for copulation (χ2(6) = 124.62, p < 0.001) included rearing, group-size, facility-type, and the sex-by-rearing interaction. Chimpanzees were more likely to copulate if they were mother-reared, in larger groups, living in research facilities, and, if not mother-reared (NOTMR), more likely to copulate if they were female. The best model for tool-use retained the predictors of age category, facility-type, and sex-by-rearing (χ2(5) = 55.78, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to use tools if they were adult, living in research facilities, and if NOTMR, were female. The best model for nest-building included facility-type and rearing (χ2(3) = 205.71, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to build nests if they were MR and if they were living in zoos or in sanctuaries. The best model for active social grooming retained the predictors of age, sex, rearing, and type of facility (χ2(6) = 102.15, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to engage in active social grooming if they were immature, female, mother-reared, and living in zoos. This research provides a basic behavioral profile for many chimpanzees living under human care in the United States and allows us to determine potential methods for improving the welfare of these and future chimpanzees in this population. |
---|---|
AbstractList | A survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex, social group-size, rearing history, and enclosure. Respondents were also asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each chimpanzee over the prior two years. Species- typical behaviors (STBs) were queried, including copulation, tool-use, nest-building, and social grooming. Tool-use was reported present for 94.3% of the sample (n = 982), active social grooming for 85.7% (n = 1121), copulation for 68.3% (n = 863) and nest-building for 58.9% (n = 982). Of the subjects for whom we had data regarding all four STBs (n = 860), 45.6% were reported to engage in all four. Logistic regression analyses using forward Wald criteria were conducted to determine the best model for each STB based on the predictors of age, sex, rearing history, group-size, facility-type, and a sex-by-rearing interaction. The best model for copulation (χ²(6) = 124.62, p < 0.001) included rearing, group-size, facility-type, and the sex-by-rearing interaction. Chimpanzees were more likely to copulate if they were mother-reared, in larger groups, living in research facilities, and, if not mother-reared (NOTMR), more likely to copulate if they were female. The best model for tool-use retained the predictors of age category, facility-type, and sex-by-rearing (χ²(5) = 55.78, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to use tools if they were adult, living in research facilities, and if NOTMR, were female. The best model for nest-building included facility-type and rearing (χ²(3) = 205.71, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to build nests if they were MR and if they were living in zoos or in sanctuaries. The best model for active social grooming retained the predictors of age, sex, rearing, and type of facility (χ²(6) = 102.15, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to engage in active social grooming if they were immature, female, mother-reared, and living in zoos. This research provides a basic behavioral profile for many chimpanzees living under human care in the United States and allows us to determine potential methods for improving the welfare of these and future chimpanzees in this population. Simple SummaryA survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. The behavioral profiles of 1122 chimpanzees were collected for this survey. Data collected included information about the animals’ age, sex, social group size, rearing history, and enclosure as well as information about each animal’s behavior. Each respondent was asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each chimpanzee over the prior two years. Species typical behaviors (STBs) were queried, including copulation, tool-use, nest-building, and social grooming. Tool-use was reported to be present for 94.3% of the sample, active grooming for 85.7%, copulation for 68.3% and nest-building for 58.9%. Male chimpanzees who were not reared by their conspecific mother were most likely to have deficits in STBs, and female chimpanzees who were mother-reared were generally the most likely to engage in STBs.AbstractA survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex, social group-size, rearing history, and enclosure. Respondents were also asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each chimpanzee over the prior two years. Species- typical behaviors (STBs) were queried, including copulation, tool-use, nest-building, and social grooming. Tool-use was reported present for 94.3% of the sample (n = 982), active social grooming for 85.7% (n = 1121), copulation for 68.3% (n = 863) and nest-building for 58.9% (n = 982). Of the subjects for whom we had data regarding all four STBs (n = 860), 45.6% were reported to engage in all four. Logistic regression analyses using forward Wald criteria were conducted to determine the best model for each STB based on the predictors of age, sex, rearing history, group-size, facility-type, and a sex-by-rearing interaction. The best model for copulation (χ2(6) = 124.62, p < 0.001) included rearing, group-size, facility-type, and the sex-by-rearing interaction. Chimpanzees were more likely to copulate if they were mother-reared, in larger groups, living in research facilities, and, if not mother-reared (NOTMR), more likely to copulate if they were female. The best model for tool-use retained the predictors of age category, facility-type, and sex-by-rearing (χ2(5) = 55.78, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to use tools if they were adult, living in research facilities, and if NOTMR, were female. The best model for nest-building included facility-type and rearing (χ2(3) = 205.71, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to build nests if they were MR and if they were living in zoos or in sanctuaries. The best model for active social grooming retained the predictors of age, sex, rearing, and type of facility (χ2(6) = 102.15, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to engage in active social grooming if they were immature, female, mother-reared, and living in zoos. This research provides a basic behavioral profile for many chimpanzees living under human care in the United States and allows us to determine potential methods for improving the welfare of these and future chimpanzees in this population. A survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex, social group-size, rearing history, and enclosure. Respondents were also asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each chimpanzee over the prior two years. Species- typical behaviors (STBs) were queried, including copulation, tool-use, nest-building, and social grooming. Tool-use was reported present for 94.3% of the sample (n = 982), active social grooming for 85.7% (n = 1121), copulation for 68.3% (n = 863) and nest-building for 58.9% (n = 982). Of the subjects for whom we had data regarding all four STBs (n = 860), 45.6% were reported to engage in all four. Logistic regression analyses using forward Wald criteria were conducted to determine the best model for each STB based on the predictors of age, sex, rearing history, group-size, facility-type, and a sex-by-rearing interaction. The best model for copulation (χ2(6) = 124.62, p < 0.001) included rearing, group-size, facility-type, and the sex-by-rearing interaction. Chimpanzees were more likely to copulate if they were mother-reared, in larger groups, living in research facilities, and, if not mother-reared (NOTMR), more likely to copulate if they were female. The best model for tool-use retained the predictors of age category, facility-type, and sex-by-rearing (χ2(5) = 55.78, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to use tools if they were adult, living in research facilities, and if NOTMR, were female. The best model for nest-building included facility-type and rearing (χ2(3) = 205.71, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to build nests if they were MR and if they were living in zoos or in sanctuaries. The best model for active social grooming retained the predictors of age, sex, rearing, and type of facility (χ2(6) = 102.15, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to engage in active social grooming if they were immature, female, mother-reared, and living in zoos. This research provides a basic behavioral profile for many chimpanzees living under human care in the United States and allows us to determine potential methods for improving the welfare of these and future chimpanzees in this population. A survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex, social group-size, rearing history, and enclosure. Respondents were also asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each chimpanzee over the prior two years. Species- typical behaviors (STBs) were queried, including copulation, tool-use, nest-building, and social grooming. Tool-use was reported present for 94.3% of the sample (n = 982), active social grooming for 85.7% (n = 1121), copulation for 68.3% (n = 863) and nest-building for 58.9% (n = 982). Of the subjects for whom we had data regarding all four STBs (n = 860), 45.6% were reported to engage in all four. Logistic regression analyses using forward Wald criteria were conducted to determine the best model for each STB based on the predictors of age, sex, rearing history, group-size, facility-type, and a sex-by-rearing interaction. The best model for copulation (χ2(6) = 124.62, p < 0.001) included rearing, group-size, facility-type, and the sex-by-rearing interaction. Chimpanzees were more likely to copulate if they were mother-reared, in larger groups, living in research facilities, and, if not mother-reared (NOTMR), more likely to copulate if they were female. The best model for tool-use retained the predictors of age category, facility-type, and sex-by-rearing (χ2(5) = 55.78, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to use tools if they were adult, living in research facilities, and if NOTMR, were female. The best model for nest-building included facility-type and rearing (χ2(3) = 205.71, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to build nests if they were MR and if they were living in zoos or in sanctuaries. The best model for active social grooming retained the predictors of age, sex, rearing, and type of facility (χ2(6) = 102.15, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to engage in active social grooming if they were immature, female, mother-reared, and living in zoos. This research provides a basic behavioral profile for many chimpanzees living under human care in the United States and allows us to determine potential methods for improving the welfare of these and future chimpanzees in this population.A survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex, social group-size, rearing history, and enclosure. Respondents were also asked to indicate if certain behaviors had been observed in each chimpanzee over the prior two years. Species- typical behaviors (STBs) were queried, including copulation, tool-use, nest-building, and social grooming. Tool-use was reported present for 94.3% of the sample (n = 982), active social grooming for 85.7% (n = 1121), copulation for 68.3% (n = 863) and nest-building for 58.9% (n = 982). Of the subjects for whom we had data regarding all four STBs (n = 860), 45.6% were reported to engage in all four. Logistic regression analyses using forward Wald criteria were conducted to determine the best model for each STB based on the predictors of age, sex, rearing history, group-size, facility-type, and a sex-by-rearing interaction. The best model for copulation (χ2(6) = 124.62, p < 0.001) included rearing, group-size, facility-type, and the sex-by-rearing interaction. Chimpanzees were more likely to copulate if they were mother-reared, in larger groups, living in research facilities, and, if not mother-reared (NOTMR), more likely to copulate if they were female. The best model for tool-use retained the predictors of age category, facility-type, and sex-by-rearing (χ2(5) = 55.78, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to use tools if they were adult, living in research facilities, and if NOTMR, were female. The best model for nest-building included facility-type and rearing (χ2(3) = 205.71, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to build nests if they were MR and if they were living in zoos or in sanctuaries. The best model for active social grooming retained the predictors of age, sex, rearing, and type of facility (χ2(6) = 102.15, p < 0.001). Chimpanzees were more likely to engage in active social grooming if they were immature, female, mother-reared, and living in zoos. This research provides a basic behavioral profile for many chimpanzees living under human care in the United States and allows us to determine potential methods for improving the welfare of these and future chimpanzees in this population. |
Author | Lambeth, Susan Jacobson, Sarah L. Clay, Andrea W. Pietsch, Rhonda Breaux, Sarah Ross, Stephen R. Bloomsmith, Mollie A. Fultz, Amy Lammey, Michael Vazquez, Maribel Perlman, Jaine E. |
AuthorAffiliation | 4 Southwest National Primate Research Center, 8715 W. Military Dr., San Antonio, TX 78227, USA 8 Coulston Foundation, Alamogordo, NM 88310, USA 1 Emory National Primate Research Center, 954 Gatewood Rd., Atlanta, GA 30322, USA 6 Center for Great Apes, P.O. Box 488, Wauchula, FL 33873, USA 7 Chimp Haven, 13600 Chimpanzee Pl, Keithville, LA 71047, USA 5 New Iberia Research Center, 4401 W. Admiral Doyle Dr., New Iberia, LA 70560, USA 2 Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, 2001 North Clark Street, Chicago, IL 60614, USA 9 The Graduate Center, Department of Psychology, City University of New York, 365 Fifth Avenue, New York, NY 10016, USA 3 National Center for Chimpanzee Care, Bastrop, TX 78602, USA |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 2 Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo, 2001 North Clark Street, Chicago, IL 60614, USA – name: 4 Southwest National Primate Research Center, 8715 W. Military Dr., San Antonio, TX 78227, USA – name: 6 Center for Great Apes, P.O. Box 488, Wauchula, FL 33873, USA – name: 1 Emory National Primate Research Center, 954 Gatewood Rd., Atlanta, GA 30322, USA – name: 5 New Iberia Research Center, 4401 W. Admiral Doyle Dr., New Iberia, LA 70560, USA – name: 7 Chimp Haven, 13600 Chimpanzee Pl, Keithville, LA 71047, USA – name: 9 The Graduate Center, Department of Psychology, City University of New York, 365 Fifth Avenue, New York, NY 10016, USA – name: 3 National Center for Chimpanzee Care, Bastrop, TX 78602, USA – name: 8 Coulston Foundation, Alamogordo, NM 88310, USA |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Andrea W. orcidid: 0000-0002-3930-8542 surname: Clay fullname: Clay, Andrea W. – sequence: 2 givenname: Stephen R. surname: Ross fullname: Ross, Stephen R. – sequence: 3 givenname: Susan surname: Lambeth fullname: Lambeth, Susan – sequence: 4 givenname: Maribel surname: Vazquez fullname: Vazquez, Maribel – sequence: 5 givenname: Sarah surname: Breaux fullname: Breaux, Sarah – sequence: 6 givenname: Rhonda surname: Pietsch fullname: Pietsch, Rhonda – sequence: 7 givenname: Amy orcidid: 0000-0002-9890-1848 surname: Fultz fullname: Fultz, Amy – sequence: 8 givenname: Michael surname: Lammey fullname: Lammey, Michael – sequence: 9 givenname: Sarah L. orcidid: 0000-0002-3439-1965 surname: Jacobson fullname: Jacobson, Sarah L. – sequence: 10 givenname: Jaine E. surname: Perlman fullname: Perlman, Jaine E. – sequence: 11 givenname: Mollie A. orcidid: 0000-0001-6286-0135 surname: Bloomsmith fullname: Bloomsmith, Mollie A. |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36670791$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFkk1vEzEQQFeoiJbSE3dkiUsRbLC9Xq-XA1IbUahUCUTChYs18c4mjjZ2sHcjhV_Az8YhLUorJHzxx7x5Go_maXbkvMMse87oqChq-hacZQXllJfsUXbCaSVzLll5dHA-zs5iXNK0qrJgJXuSHRdSVrSq2Un2a7ywqzW4n4iRnH8BR_rg551vtj3GV8Q68m00GZHv3sc3ZALO9AMEi-kCriFfMSIEsyBXYGxn-xR4Ry7IZAgb3OaXELEhY5_8wUbviG_JZI0mUfl0u7YGOnKJC9hYH-Kz7HELXcSz2_00m159mI4_5TefP16PL25yU1LV5yXFsm2bSkiJlLWoaiNqBaJAWpdl6oTkRrYzVrWzGoBj3apCKiixVQKNKU6z67228bDU62BXELbag9V_HnyYawi9NR1qIRpaCN4ILqio-GzWzERdc1YIYFS2VXK937vWw2yFjUHXB-juSe9HnF3oud_oWpVCMpkE57eC4H8MGHu9stFg14FDP0TNlapkKkGx_6OVVFwIxWlCXz5Al34ILjV1R1WMpx7xRL04LP5v1XejkQC2B0zwMQZstbE99Nbv_mI7zajeTaA-mMCU8_pBzp32X_RvYf_adQ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2024_106272 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani13162595 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23534 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani12182421 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10764_024_00462_2 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani13060961 |
Cites_doi | 10.1007/BF02436405 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2361(1997)16:3<225::AID-ZOO3>3.0.CO;2-C 10.1007/BF02382960 10.1079/9780851990040.0000 10.1007/BF02374033 10.1037/10080-000 10.7208/chicago/9780226728032.001.0001 10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.11.001 10.1007/s10071-005-0002-7 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.11.004 10.1163/156853990X00581 10.3390/ani10071211 10.1007/s10764-006-9074-8 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.002 10.1023/A:1005469302911 10.1007/978-0-387-25640-5 10.1017/CBO9780511606397.002 10.1007/s10764-020-00157-4 10.1111/desc.12106 10.2466/pr0.1967.20.3c.1106 10.1098/rspb.2008.1324 10.1080/09720502.2010.10700699 10.1159/000156335 10.1017/CBO9780511752414.017 10.1098/rsbl.2020.0033 10.1016/0168-1591(96)01061-1 10.1016/0168-1591(94)90017-5 10.1002/ajp.22947 10.1079/PNS19970113 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.10.009 10.1098/rsos.170932 10.1093/beheco/arz062 10.1002/zoo.1430140406 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01637.x 10.5993/AJHB.25.3.15 10.1002/zoo.20276 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80630-X 10.1007/BF01542561 10.1002/ajp.1350340403 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.05.009 10.3389/fvets.2019.00062 10.1007/s10806-005-4493-7 10.1007/s100710100082 10.1086/267981 10.1017/S0962728600029122 10.1080/10888705.2015.1075830 10.1126/science.370.6521.1152 10.7717/peerj.2225 10.1038/428715a 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198526223.003.0004 10.7717/peerj.579 10.1007/s10329-020-00839-w 10.1098/rspb.2009.0640 10.1007/978-3-642-35955-2 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1962.tb13652.x 10.1002/zoo.1430130107 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.04.006 10.1002/ajp.20265 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(200005)36:4<292::AID-DEV4>3.0.CO;2-T 10.1007/978-94-011-3110-0_9 10.30802/AALAS-JAALAS-18-000034 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.034 10.1007/BF02558143 10.1080/10888705.2013.827917 10.1002/ajp.1350030131 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90123-9 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.10.002 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1978.tb00485.x 10.4324/9780203311875_chapter_3 10.1080/10888705.2014.896213 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.06.011 10.1037/0003-066X.43.10.765 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.02.008 10.1007/BF02557701 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. 2023 by the authors. 2023 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. – notice: 2023 by the authors. 2023 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM ABUWG AFKRA AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PKEHL PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS 7X8 7S9 L.6 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.3390/ani13020251 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) Publicly Available Content Database (ProQuest) ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China MEDLINE - Academic AGRICOLA AGRICOLA - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) Acceso a contenido Full Text - Doaj |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) MEDLINE - Academic AGRICOLA AGRICOLA - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | AGRICOLA Publicly Available Content Database CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Zoology |
EISSN | 2076-2615 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_44d0342d4240472bbdb4992134a106f7 PMC9854616 36670791 10_3390_ani13020251 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GeographicLocations | United States--US |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: United States--US |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIH/OD Cooperative Agreement grantid: U42-OD 011197 – fundername: NIH grantid: P51-OD011133 |
GroupedDBID | 5VS 7XC 8FE 8FH AAFWJ AAHBH AAYXX ABDBF ACUHS AFKRA AFPKN ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS APEBS BENPR CCPQU CITATION DIK EAD EAP EPL ESX GROUPED_DOAJ HYE IAO ITC LK8 M48 MODMG M~E OK1 OZF PGMZT PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PROAC RPM TUS ZBA ATCPS BBNVY BHPHI HCIFZ ISR M0K M7P NPM PATMY PYCSY ABUWG AZQEC DWQXO PKEHL PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS 7X8 7S9 L.6 5PM PUEGO |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c508t-50e5ffd7466e01fe89c498a43e095530262c6fb17fb9aa2e9f8368a5ef84ecc3 |
IEDL.DBID | M48 |
ISSN | 2076-2615 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:24:59 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 18:38:52 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 08:15:02 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 07:15:50 EDT 2025 Mon Jun 30 07:31:02 EDT 2025 Thu Jan 02 22:53:05 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:58:25 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:56:02 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 2 |
Keywords | primate chimpanzee welfare ape species-typical behavior |
Language | English |
License | https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c508t-50e5ffd7466e01fe89c498a43e095530262c6fb17fb9aa2e9f8368a5ef84ecc3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-3930-8542 0000-0002-3439-1965 0000-0001-6286-0135 0000-0002-9890-1848 |
OpenAccessLink | https://doaj.org/article/44d0342d4240472bbdb4992134a106f7 |
PMID | 36670791 |
PQID | 2767122622 |
PQPubID | 2032438 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_44d0342d4240472bbdb4992134a106f7 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9854616 proquest_miscellaneous_2887634281 proquest_miscellaneous_2768244820 proquest_journals_2767122622 pubmed_primary_36670791 crossref_citationtrail_10_3390_ani13020251 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani13020251 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20230110 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2023-01-10 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 1 year: 2023 text: 20230110 day: 10 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | Switzerland |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Switzerland – name: Basel |
PublicationTitle | Animals (Basel) |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Animals (Basel) |
PublicationYear | 2023 |
Publisher | MDPI AG MDPI |
Publisher_xml | – name: MDPI AG – name: MDPI |
References | Mellor (ref_4) 2012; 1 Videan (ref_41) 2006; 68 Nishida (ref_71) 1988; 50 ref_90 Webb (ref_50) 2019; 81 ref_13 Hopper (ref_84) 2015; 112 ref_12 ref_96 Lonsdorf (ref_60) 2006; 9 ref_19 Walsh (ref_14) 1982; 3 Robinson (ref_18) 2017; 191 Merrick (ref_70) 1977; 18 Yamamoto (ref_86) 2010; 79 ref_25 ref_23 ref_22 Bard (ref_91) 2007; 69 Wiseman (ref_98) 1972; 36 Bloomsmith (ref_20) 2019; 58 Hill (ref_8) 2009; 28 Stokes (ref_82) 2001; 4 Bracke (ref_7) 2006; 19 Freeman (ref_55) 2014; 2 Fultz (ref_59) 2017; 6 Jacobson (ref_78) 2017; 4 ref_74 Goodall (ref_39) 1962; 102 Grimm (ref_97) 2020; 370 Nadler (ref_56) 1993; 22 Novak (ref_11) 1988; 43 Yamanashi (ref_67) 2020; 61 Tomasello (ref_36) 1987; 2 ref_83 Fay (ref_32) 1994; 34 Gomes (ref_44) 2009; 276 ref_81 King (ref_16) 1994; 13 Watts (ref_73) 2000; 21 ref_89 Alford (ref_95) 1995; 14 Steyerberg (ref_52) 2011; 64 Fedurek (ref_69) 2009; 115 Grund (ref_85) 2019; 30 Ross (ref_66) 2020; 16 Bloomsmith (ref_75) 2006; 68 Okamoto (ref_87) 2001; 42 Dienske (ref_28) 1978; 19 ref_54 Sanz (ref_37) 2007; 52 Muller (ref_94) 2010; 58 Henzi (ref_47) 1999; 40 Scott (ref_57) 2013; 85 Bogart (ref_77) 2014; 17 Bernstein (ref_42) 1967; 20 Martin (ref_27) 2005; 14 Baker (ref_68) 1997; 16 Lambeth (ref_65) 2001; 54 Clay (ref_79) 2017; 156 Mallapur (ref_15) 2005; 91 Pusey (ref_72) 1990; 115 Kurtycz (ref_10) 2014; 17 Bloomsmith (ref_63) 2000; 51 Jacobson (ref_21) 2016; 4 ref_35 ref_33 Bernstein (ref_80) 1962; 10 Hopper (ref_17) 2016; 176 Brakes (ref_29) 2019; 6 ref_31 ref_30 Baker (ref_5) 1996; 49 Price (ref_34) 2009; 276 Koyama (ref_46) 2006; 27 King (ref_26) 1980; 21 Freeman (ref_88) 2013; 75 Hopper (ref_62) 2017; 16 ref_38 Midi (ref_51) 2010; 13 Young (ref_92) 1997; 56 Kagan (ref_2) 2015; 18 Schapiro (ref_93) 2012; 137 Koene (ref_9) 2013; 16 ref_43 ref_40 ref_1 Lonsdorf (ref_61) 2004; 428 Bloomsmith (ref_24) 1994; 39 ref_3 Yerkes (ref_58) 1939; 11 ref_49 ref_48 Peng (ref_53) 2001; 25 Stewart (ref_76) 2020; 41 Hopkins (ref_64) 2000; 36 Hemelrijk (ref_45) 1991; 41 ref_6 |
References_xml | – volume: 2 start-page: 175 year: 1987 ident: ref_36 article-title: Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees publication-title: Hum. Evol. doi: 10.1007/BF02436405 – ident: ref_74 – volume: 16 start-page: 225 year: 1997 ident: ref_68 article-title: Straw and forage material amerliorate abnormal behaviors in adult chimpanzees publication-title: Zoo Biol. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2361(1997)16:3<225::AID-ZOO3>3.0.CO;2-C – volume: 18 start-page: 215 year: 1977 ident: ref_70 article-title: Social grooming and play behavior of a captive group of chimpanzees publication-title: Primates doi: 10.1007/BF02382960 – ident: ref_35 doi: 10.1079/9780851990040.0000 – ident: ref_6 doi: 10.1079/9780851990040.0000 – ident: ref_1 – volume: 21 start-page: 198 year: 1980 ident: ref_26 article-title: Social behavior in a captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) group publication-title: Primates doi: 10.1007/BF02374033 – ident: ref_12 doi: 10.1037/10080-000 – ident: ref_23 doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226728032.001.0001 – volume: 52 start-page: 420 year: 2007 ident: ref_37 article-title: Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo publication-title: J. Hum. Evol. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.11.001 – ident: ref_48 – volume: 69 start-page: 123 year: 2007 ident: ref_91 article-title: Studies of attachment in nursery-reared chimpanzees: From peers to response caregivers publication-title: Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. – volume: 9 start-page: 36 year: 2006 ident: ref_60 article-title: What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? publication-title: Anim. Cogn. doi: 10.1007/s10071-005-0002-7 – ident: ref_13 – volume: 137 start-page: 183 year: 2012 ident: ref_93 article-title: Physiological and welfare consequences of transport, relocation, and acclimatization of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) publication-title: Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.11.004 – volume: 115 start-page: 203 year: 1990 ident: ref_72 article-title: Behavioural changes at adolescence in chimpanzees publication-title: Behaviour doi: 10.1163/156853990X00581 – ident: ref_31 doi: 10.3390/ani10071211 – volume: 27 start-page: 1293 year: 2006 ident: ref_46 article-title: Interchange of grooming and agonistic support in chimpanzees publication-title: Int. J. Primatol. doi: 10.1007/s10764-006-9074-8 – volume: 176 start-page: 112 year: 2016 ident: ref_17 article-title: Reconsidering coprophagy as an indicator of negative welfare for captive chimpanzees publication-title: Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.002 – volume: 21 start-page: 189 year: 2000 ident: ref_73 article-title: Grooming between male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. I. Partner number and diversity and grooming reciprocity publication-title: Int. J. Primatol. doi: 10.1023/A:1005469302911 – volume: 75 start-page: 46 year: 2013 ident: ref_88 article-title: The relationship between early human exposure and current behavior in former pet/performer chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. – ident: ref_25 doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-25640-5 – ident: ref_30 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511606397.002 – volume: 41 start-page: 849 year: 2020 ident: ref_76 article-title: Sex bias and social influences on savanna chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) nest building behavior publication-title: Int. J. Primatol. doi: 10.1007/s10764-020-00157-4 – volume: 17 start-page: 161 year: 2014 ident: ref_77 article-title: Different early rearing experiences have long term effects on cortical organization in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) publication-title: Dev. Sci. doi: 10.1111/desc.12106 – volume: 20 start-page: 1106 year: 1967 ident: ref_42 article-title: Age and experience in chimpanzee nest building publication-title: Psychol. Rep. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1967.20.3c.1106 – volume: 276 start-page: 699 year: 2009 ident: ref_44 article-title: Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees publication-title: Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1324 – volume: 13 start-page: 253 year: 2010 ident: ref_51 article-title: Collinearity diagnostics of binary logistic regression model publication-title: J. Interdiscip. Math. doi: 10.1080/09720502.2010.10700699 – volume: 50 start-page: 109 year: 1988 ident: ref_71 article-title: Development of social grooming between mother and offspring in wild chimpanzees publication-title: Folia Primatol. doi: 10.1159/000156335 – ident: ref_33 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511752414.017 – volume: 16 start-page: 20200033 year: 2020 ident: ref_66 article-title: A review of research in primate sanctuaries publication-title: Biol. Lett. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2020.0033 – volume: 49 start-page: 403 year: 1996 ident: ref_5 article-title: An analysis of regurgitation and reingestion in captive chimpanzees publication-title: Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(96)01061-1 – volume: 39 start-page: 73 year: 1994 ident: ref_24 article-title: Juvenile and adolescent chimpanzee behavioral development in complex groups publication-title: Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/0168-1591(94)90017-5 – volume: 81 start-page: e22947 year: 2019 ident: ref_50 article-title: Does (group) size matter? Captive chimpanzee behavior as a function of group size and composition publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22947 – volume: 56 start-page: 1095 year: 1997 ident: ref_92 article-title: The importance of food presentation for animal welfare and conservation publication-title: Proc. Nutr. Soc. doi: 10.1079/PNS19970113 – volume: 112 start-page: 120 year: 2015 ident: ref_84 article-title: The importance of witnessed agency in chimpanzee social learning of tool use publication-title: Behav. Process. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.10.009 – volume: 4 start-page: 170932 year: 2017 ident: ref_78 article-title: Atypical experiences of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are associated with higher hair cortisol concentrations as adults publication-title: R. Soc. Open Sci. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170932 – volume: 1 start-page: 445 year: 2012 ident: ref_4 article-title: Affective states and the assessment of laboratory-induced animal welfare impacts publication-title: ALTEX Proc. – volume: 30 start-page: 1136 year: 2019 ident: ref_85 article-title: Necessity creates opportunities for chimpanzee tool use publication-title: Behav. Ecol. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arz062 – volume: 14 start-page: 347 year: 1995 ident: ref_95 article-title: Wounding aggression during the formation and maintenance of captive multimale chimpanzee groups publication-title: Zoo Biol. doi: 10.1002/zoo.1430140406 – volume: 115 start-page: 566 year: 2009 ident: ref_69 article-title: What does mutual grooming tell us about why chimpanzees groom? publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01637.x – ident: ref_89 – volume: 25 start-page: 278 year: 2001 ident: ref_53 article-title: Modeling categorical variables by logistic regression publication-title: Am. J. Health Behav. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.25.3.15 – volume: 28 start-page: 531 year: 2009 ident: ref_8 article-title: Measuring zoo animal welfare: Theory and practice publication-title: Zoo Biol. doi: 10.1002/zoo.20276 – volume: 41 start-page: 923 year: 1991 ident: ref_45 article-title: Reciprocity and interchange of grooming and “support” in captive chimpanzees publication-title: Anim. Behav. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80630-X – volume: 64 start-page: 501 year: 2011 ident: ref_52 article-title: Regression modeling strategies publication-title: Rev. Española Cardiol. Engl. Ed. – volume: 22 start-page: 477 year: 1993 ident: ref_56 article-title: Effects of an oral contraceptive on sexual behavior of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) publication-title: Arch. Sex. Behav. doi: 10.1007/BF01542561 – volume: 34 start-page: 309 year: 1994 ident: ref_32 article-title: Chimpanzee tool use for honey and termite extraction in Central Africa publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350340403 – volume: 6 start-page: 9 year: 2017 ident: ref_59 article-title: A guide for modern sanctuaries with examples from a captive chimpanzee sanctuary publication-title: Anim. Stud. J. – volume: 58 start-page: 440 year: 2010 ident: ref_94 article-title: Dynamics of social and energetic stress in wild female chimpanzees publication-title: Horm. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.05.009 – volume: 6 start-page: 62 year: 2019 ident: ref_29 article-title: Sociality and wild animal welfare: Future directions publication-title: Front. Vet. Sci. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2019.00062 – ident: ref_49 – volume: 19 start-page: 77 year: 2006 ident: ref_7 article-title: Assessing the importance of natural behavior for animal welfare publication-title: J. Agric. Environ. Ethics doi: 10.1007/s10806-005-4493-7 – volume: 4 start-page: 11 year: 2001 ident: ref_82 article-title: Cognitive capacities for behavioural flexibility in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effect of snare injury on complex manual food processing publication-title: Anim. Cogn. doi: 10.1007/s100710100082 – ident: ref_90 – volume: 36 start-page: 105 year: 1972 ident: ref_98 article-title: Methodological bias in public opinion surveys publication-title: Public Opin. Q. doi: 10.1086/267981 – volume: 14 start-page: 125 year: 2005 ident: ref_27 article-title: The effects of rearing conditions on grooming and play behaviour in captive chimpanzees publication-title: Anim. Welf. doi: 10.1017/S0962728600029122 – volume: 18 start-page: S1 year: 2015 ident: ref_2 article-title: A universal animal welfare framework for zoos publication-title: J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2015.1075830 – volume: 54 start-page: 62 year: 2001 ident: ref_65 article-title: Control over videotape enrichment for socially housed chimpanzees: Subsequent challenge tests publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. – volume: 370 start-page: 1152 year: 2020 ident: ref_97 article-title: Saving sanctuaries publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.370.6521.1152 – ident: ref_19 doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226728032.001.0001 – volume: 4 start-page: e2225 year: 2016 ident: ref_21 article-title: Characterizing abnormal behavior in a large population of zoo-housed chimpanzees: Prevalence and potential influencing factors publication-title: PeerJ doi: 10.7717/peerj.2225 – volume: 428 start-page: 715 year: 2004 ident: ref_61 article-title: Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/428715a – ident: ref_83 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198526223.003.0004 – volume: 2 start-page: e579 year: 2014 ident: ref_55 article-title: The impact of atypical early histories on pet or performer chimpanzees publication-title: PeerJ doi: 10.7717/peerj.579 – volume: 61 start-page: 639 year: 2020 ident: ref_67 article-title: Development of bed-building behaviors in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implication for critical period hypothesis and captive management publication-title: Primates doi: 10.1007/s10329-020-00839-w – volume: 276 start-page: 3377 year: 2009 ident: ref_34 article-title: A potent effect of observational learning on chimpanzee tool construction publication-title: Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0640 – ident: ref_22 doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-35955-2 – volume: 102 start-page: 455 year: 1962 ident: ref_39 article-title: Nest building behavior in the free ranging chimpanzee publication-title: Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1962.tb13652.x – ident: ref_3 doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226728032.001.0001 – ident: ref_38 – volume: 51 start-page: 45 year: 2000 ident: ref_63 article-title: Control over computer-assisted enrichment for socially housed chimpanzees publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. – volume: 13 start-page: 51 year: 1994 ident: ref_16 article-title: The effects of early experience on adult copulatory behavior in zoo-born chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) publication-title: Zoo Biol. doi: 10.1002/zoo.1430130107 – volume: 16 start-page: 100 year: 2017 ident: ref_62 article-title: Cognitive research in zoos publication-title: Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.04.006 – volume: 68 start-page: 745 year: 2006 ident: ref_41 article-title: Bed-building in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The importance of early rearing publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20265 – volume: 36 start-page: 292 year: 2000 ident: ref_64 article-title: A longitudinal study of hand preference in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) publication-title: Dev. Psychobiol. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(200005)36:4<292::AID-DEV4>3.0.CO;2-T – ident: ref_40 doi: 10.1007/978-94-011-3110-0_9 – volume: 58 start-page: 160 year: 2019 ident: ref_20 article-title: Survey of behavioral indices of welfare in research chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the United States publication-title: J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. doi: 10.30802/AALAS-JAALAS-18-000034 – volume: 79 start-page: 595 year: 2010 ident: ref_86 article-title: The influence of kin relationship and reciprocal context on chimpanzees′ other-regarding preferences publication-title: Anim. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.034 – volume: 42 start-page: 161 year: 2001 ident: ref_87 article-title: Greeting behavior during party encounters in captive chimpanzees publication-title: Primates doi: 10.1007/BF02558143 – volume: 16 start-page: 360 year: 2013 ident: ref_9 article-title: Behavioral ecology of captive species: Using behavioral adaptations to assess and enhance welfare of nonhuman zoo animals publication-title: J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2013.827917 – volume: 3 start-page: 315 year: 1982 ident: ref_14 article-title: A vocabulary of abnormal behaviors in restrictively reared chimpanzees publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350030131 – volume: 10 start-page: 1 year: 1962 ident: ref_80 article-title: Response to nesting materials of wild born and captive born chimpanzees publication-title: Anim. Behav. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90123-9 – ident: ref_96 – volume: 91 start-page: 337 year: 2005 ident: ref_15 article-title: Factors influencing the behaviour and welfare of captive lion-tailed macaques in Indian zoos publication-title: Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.10.002 – volume: 68 start-page: 48 year: 2006 ident: ref_75 article-title: Cross-sectional study of the behavioral development of young male chimpanzees in twenty zoos publication-title: Am. J. Primatol. – volume: 19 start-page: 387 year: 1978 ident: ref_28 article-title: Abnormal behaviour patterns developing in chimpanzee infants during nursery care—A note publication-title: J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1978.tb00485.x – volume: 85 start-page: 1 year: 2013 ident: ref_57 article-title: Behaviour across the menstrual cycle of captive female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) taking a combined oral contraceptive pill publication-title: Folia Primatol. – ident: ref_54 – ident: ref_81 doi: 10.4324/9780203311875_chapter_3 – volume: 17 start-page: 185 year: 2014 ident: ref_10 article-title: The choice to access outdoor areas affects the behavior of great apes publication-title: J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2014.896213 – volume: 156 start-page: 58 year: 2017 ident: ref_79 article-title: Effects of sex and early rearing condition on adult behavior, health, and well-being in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) publication-title: Behav. Process. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.06.011 – volume: 43 start-page: 765 year: 1988 ident: ref_11 article-title: Psychological well-being of primates in captivity publication-title: Am. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.10.765 – ident: ref_43 – volume: 191 start-page: 90 year: 2017 ident: ref_18 article-title: Chimpanzees with positive welfare are happier, extraverted, and emotionally stable publication-title: Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.02.008 – volume: 11 start-page: 78 year: 1939 ident: ref_58 article-title: Sexual behavior in the chimpanzee publication-title: Hum. Biol. – volume: 40 start-page: 47 year: 1999 ident: ref_47 article-title: The value of grooming to female primates publication-title: Primates doi: 10.1007/BF02557701 |
SSID | ssj0000753151 |
Score | 2.2769935 |
Snippet | A survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. Data collected included information about 1122 chimpanzees’ age, sex,... Simple SummaryA survey was sent to zoos, research facilities, and sanctuaries which housed chimpanzees. The behavioral profiles of 1122 chimpanzees were... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 251 |
SubjectTerms | Accreditation adults Affect (Psychology) Animal welfare ape Aquariums Behavior chimpanzee copulation Entertainment industry females group size humans Monkeys & apes Pan troglodytes primate regression analysis species-typical behavior surveys welfare Zoos |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: Acceso a contenido Full Text - Doaj dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Na9wwEBUlUOilNP3cJi0q5NCWKFnbsmTllixdQqGlsAmEXoxkSc2WIJe1t5D-gv7szMhes1tCc8nVMxhZM8O8sUZvCNkbAwTNIWwY1B4Fg0gUzHCVsVxxbaQ11kXegi9fxek5_3yRX6yN-sKesI4euNu4Q84tstRZDqmHy9QYC-9SyEOmoZrx8R455Ly1YupndzyXQS7rLuRlUNcf6jDHMzqE1BspKDL13wYv_-2SXEs70yfkcY8X6XG3zm3ywIWn5OH3Ov4Nf0b-Ti7xnmP441xD33_TgbaL-sdVba8BQn6g80DPD2YHFPSbfToDC7fLWBzvUx0sXbXd0amusEcWBEf0mM6Wi9_ump1AgrN0MgwqpLWncVy9axiUr2hd2tMrLprn5Gz66WxyyvrhCqwCTNayfOxy763kQrhx4l2hKrCW5plDUjrYKZFWwptEeqO0Tp3yRSYKnTtfcDB79oJshTq4V4Qq47mXKrGZ9Tx1uRJWCu6VVJUf-yQZkY-r7S6rnngc519clVCAoG3KNduMyN6g_Kvj27hd7QTtNqggSXZ8AK5T9q5T3uU6I7K7snrZR25TplLIBDBpmo7Iu0EMMYcHKTq4ehl1CoBFAJ7-o1Mg1x8Ud7DWl50jDavNhEBiQpDIDRfb-JxNSZhfRu5vVeRcJOL1fXz_DnkEm4ntdJCMd8lWu1i6NwCxWvM2RtMN4n8jGg priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – databaseName: ProQuest Central dbid: BENPR link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3fa9swEBZby2AvY7-XrS0a9GEbVRvbsmT1ZTShoQxWytJC2YuRLakNFLmznUH3F-zP3p2juM0ofY3uQdGn030nnb8jZHsIFDQFt2GQe2QMPFGwgquEpYrrQprC2E634PuxODrj387T83Dh1oSyyuWZ2B3UpirxjnwvlkJGwBXi-Ov1L4Zdo_B1NbTQeEzW4QjOIPlaHx0en_zob1kgICYQ0xYf5iWQ3-9pP8O3OqTWK6GoU-y_j2b-Xy15J_xMnpNngTfSgwXQL8gj61-SJz-r7lb8Ffk7vsTvHf0faxv66UR72tbVxVVlboBKfqYzT892p7sU7JsdOgWk23mXJO9Q7Q1dlt_RiS6xVhYG9ukBnc7r3_aGjSDQGTruGxbSytGubb1tGKSxiDINMot185qcTg5Px0csNFlgJXCzlqVDmzpnJBfCDiNnM1UCaponFsXpYKVEXApXRNIVSuvYKpclItOpdRkH-JM3ZM1X3r4jVBWOO6kikxjHY5sqYaTgTklVuqGLogH5slzuvAwC5NgH4yqHRASxye9gMyDbvfH1QnfjfrMR4taboFh290NVX-TB93LODQodGg7shcu4KAxsR4VSdhoSYicHZGOJeh48uMlv99uAfOyHwffwQUV7W807mwzoEZCoB2wy1PyDJA_m-naxkfrZJkKgQCGMyJUttvJ3Vkf87LLTAFdZykUk3j889Q_kKSwTFsxBuN0ga209t5tAotpiK3jKP4kYHYM priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest |
Title | Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in U.S. Zoos, Sanctuaries, and Research Facilities: A Survey-Based Comparison of Species-Typical Behaviors |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36670791 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2767122622 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2768244820 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2887634281 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9854616 https://doaj.org/article/44d0342d4240472bbdb4992134a106f7 |
Volume | 13 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3fa9swEBZdy2AvY7-XrQsa9GEbVRbbsmQNxmhCQxm0lKWBshcjW1KbEeTNdsayv2B_9k6yY5oSxl6tM8i6O-77LOk7hA6GAEFjSBsC3CMhkImMZFREJBZUZlxlSnvdgtMzdjKjny_jyx20bsbZLmC1ldq5flKzcjH49WP1CRL-o2OcQNnfSzt3228OLd9Be1CSuMvQ0xbnf2t266LAt2IMgbcTYA1xc1fv9vsb1cmL-G9DnrcPUN6oSJMH6H4LJfFR4_uHaEfbR-ju18L_KH-M_oyv3RVI-1vrCr85lxbXZXG1KNQK0OVbPLd4NpgOMNhXh3gKzq-XnjcfYmkVXp_IwxOZu-OzMPABH-HpsvypV2QEtU_hcdfDEBcG-072uiLAbJ3jcau8WFZP0MXk-GJ8Qtq-CyQHuFaTeKhjYxSnjOlhYHQicnCkpJF2enWwUizMmckCbjIhZaiFSSKWyFibhEJERE_Rri2sfo6wyAw1XAQqUoaGOhZMcUaN4CI3QxMEPfRuvdxp3mqSu9YYixS4ifNNesM3PXTQGX9vpDi2m42c3zoTp5_tHxTlVdqmY0qpctqHigKgoTzMMgURKpy6nQSObHgP7a-9nq5jMg054wHA1TDsodfdMKSj22ORVhdLb5MAYgJc9Q-bxMkAAu-DuT5rAqmbbcSY0yyEEb4RYhufszli59deFlwkMWUBe_Efc3uJ7sFauYN0UIb30W5dLvUrAFd11kd7o-Oz8y99_3Oi79PoL9abJL0 |
linkProvider | Scholars Portal |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1tb9MwELZGJ8S-IF5HxwAjDQnQsjWxY8dICK1lVce2aqKdNPElcmJ7qzQlpUlB5RfwZ_iPnPPGiqZ929f6VDm-O99z9vk5hLY6AEF9cBsHco_AAU9kTkQFcXxBZcRVpHTBW3A8ZINT-uXMP1tBf-q3MLasst4Ti41apbE9I9_1OOMuYAXP-zT97tiuUfZ2tW6hUZrFoV78hJQt-3jwGfT7xvP6--PewKm6CjgxgJHc8TvaN0ZxypjuuEYHIoZpSkq0ZWMjkJN4MTORy00kpPS0MAFhgfS1CSh8L4G_vYNWKYFMpoVWu_vDk6_NoQ7EXwIhtHwHSIjo7MpkYq8GLZJfinxFg4DrUO3_xZlXol3_AbpfwVS8V9rVQ7Sik0fo7re0OIR_jH73LuzzyuSX1hl-eyITnM_S88tULQC5vsOTBJ_ujHYwyGfbeASGlc-LnHwby0ThutoP92VsS3Nh4APew6P57IdeOF2Iqwr3mv6IODV4NNWwB2UOZM3WqHDF6jjLnqDxbaz-U9RK0kQ_Q1hEhhouXEWUoZ72BVOcUSO4iE3HuG4bva-XO4wrvnPbduMyhLzH6ia8ops22mqEpyXNx_ViXau3RsRycxc_pLPzsHL1kFJleRUVBbBEuRdFCqxfWOY8Cfm34W20WWs9rDaMLPxn3m30uhkGV7f3NzLR6byQCQCNAWa7QSawFIOQU8Jc10tDamZLGLN8iDDCl0xs6XOWR5LJRUE5LgKfMpdt3Dz1V-jeYHx8FB4dDA-fozVYMlurB5F-E7Xy2Vy_APyWRy8rr8EovGU__QvjNFpc |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3bbhMxELVKKhAviDuBAkYqEqBusxevvUZCqEkbtRSiiLRSxcvKu7bbSJU37Cag8AX8En_HeG80qOpbX-NR5PXMeM7Y4zMIbboAQUNwGwdyj8gBT6ROQnjghJyIhMlEqpK34MuI7h-TTyfhyRr607yFsWWVzZ5YbtQyS-0Zec9nlHmAFXy_p-uyiPHu8OPsu2M7SNmb1qadRmUih2r5E9K34sPBLuj6te8P944G-07dYcBJAZjMndBVodaSEUqV62kV8RSmLEigLDNbAPmJn1KdeEwnXAhfcR0FNBKh0hGBbw_gb2-gdQZJkdtB6_290fhre8ADsTiAcFq9CQwC7vaEmdprQovqV6Jg2SzgMoT7f6Hmhcg3vIvu1JAV71Q2dg-tKXMf3fyWlQfyD9DvwZl9aml-KVXgN2Nh8DzPTs8zuQQU-xZPDT7enmxjkC-28ASMbL4o8_MtLIzETeUfHorUlunCwHu8gyeL_IdaOn2IsRIP2l6JONN4MlOwHxUOZNDWwHDN8JgXD9HRdaz-I9QxmVFPEOaJJppxTwZSE1-FnEpGieaMp9rVntdF75rljtOa-9y24DiPIQeyuokv6KaLNlvhWUX5cblY3-qtFbE83eUPWX4a124fEyItx6IkAJwI85NEgidwy6InIBfXrIs2Gq3H9eZRxP9MvYtetcPg9vYuRxiVLUqZCJAZ4LcrZCJLNwj5Jcz1cWVI7WwDSi03IoywFRNb-ZzVETM9K-nHeRQS6tGnV0_9JboF_hl_PhgdPkO3YcVs2R4E_Q3UmecL9Ryg3Dx5UTsNRvE1u-lf4rRekQ |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Chimpanzees+%28Pan+troglodytes%29+in+U.S.+Zoos%2C+Sanctuaries%2C+and+Research+Facilities%3A+A+Survey-Based+Comparison+of+Species-Typical+Behaviors&rft.jtitle=Animals+%28Basel%29&rft.au=Clay%2C+Andrea+W&rft.au=Ross%2C+Stephen+R&rft.au=Lambeth%2C+Susan&rft.au=Vazquez%2C+Maribel&rft.date=2023-01-10&rft.issn=2076-2615&rft.eissn=2076-2615&rft.volume=13&rft.issue=2&rft_id=info:doi/10.3390%2Fani13020251&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2076-2615&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2076-2615&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2076-2615&client=summon |