Phase of Spontaneous Slow Oscillations during Sleep Influences Memory-Related Processing of Auditory Cues

Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyper...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inThe Journal of neuroscience Vol. 36; no. 4; pp. 1401 - 1409
Main Authors Batterink, Laura J, Creery, Jessica D, Paller, Ken A
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Society for Neuroscience 27.01.2016
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates. Slow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates.
AbstractList Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates. Slow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates.
Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates.
Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENTSlow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates.
Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat–meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Slow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates.
Author Creery, Jessica D
Batterink, Laura J
Paller, Ken A
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Laura J
  surname: Batterink
  fullname: Batterink, Laura J
  email: lbatterink@northwestern.edu
  organization: Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710 lbatterink@northwestern.edu
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Jessica D
  surname: Creery
  fullname: Creery, Jessica D
  organization: Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Ken A
  orcidid: 0000-0003-4415-4143
  surname: Paller
  fullname: Paller, Ken A
  organization: Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26818525$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqNUU1PGzEUtCpQCbR_AfnYywZ_rr2XSijiIwgIIuVseXffgquNndq7IP49DtAIbpye9GbeaObNPtrxwQNCh5RMqWT86OL65O52sZzNp5wqWVA5ZYSW39Ako1XBBKE7aEKYIkUplNhD-yn9JYQoQtV3tMdKTbVkcoLczYNNgEOHl-vgB-shjAkv-_CEF6lxfW8HF3zC7Ridv88AwBrPfdeP4BtI-ApWIT4Xt5CJ0OKbGPI2bahZ8nhs3ZBhPBsh_UC7ne0T_HyfB-ju9OTP7Ly4XJzNZ8eXRSMJGYpaA-XAOhCd0IzXtVRSW103UOVQNYCSoIW1bU0Z1SWteEOJ5aJVuqy0EPwA_X7TXY_1CtoG_BBtb9bRrWx8NsE68xnx7sHch0cjFNOK8yzw610ghn_Z-GBWLjWQX_H6HENVqaVi2e1XqFTISpEqU8s3ahNDShG6rSNKzKZSs63UbCrNO7OpNB8efsyzPfvfIX8BQEahiQ
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2020_02_024
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2123430119
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2022_12_004
crossref_primary_10_1152_physiol_00004_2019
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2016_00426
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11055_019_00853_6
crossref_primary_10_1093_sleep_zsz315
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_017_0542_8
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2018_04_020
crossref_primary_10_3390_cells9010054
crossref_primary_10_1093_braincomms_fcac089
crossref_primary_10_3389_fncir_2022_1076354
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00109
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2023_105379
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_39178_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nlm_2021_107529
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnsys_2018_00040
crossref_primary_10_3724_SP_J_1042_2021_01251
crossref_primary_10_1097_WNR_0000000000001215
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pneurobio_2021_102174
crossref_primary_10_1088_1741_2552_ab0933
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2018_00028
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_40583
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2017_11_020
crossref_primary_10_3389_fncel_2019_00071
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_13855
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_017_1313_9
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41593_023_01324_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nlm_2018_03_003
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_48881
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2020_525970
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2018_03_009
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00644_2016
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1008758
crossref_primary_10_1007_s40675_024_00291_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2023_03_005
crossref_primary_10_1042_ETLS20230109
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2021_108106
crossref_primary_10_1002_hipo_23526
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1427_19_2019
crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_024_05947_7
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15628
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1784_21_2023
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jsmc_2019_11_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jsmc_2022_06_013
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_smrv_2018_01_008
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2016_07_027
crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niw014
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhaa228
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2123428119
Cites_doi 10.1038/nn.3152
10.1126/science.aaa3841
10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.006
10.1371/journal.pone.0101567
10.1162/jocn_a_00471
10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00169-0
10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01096-6
10.1038/nn.3203
10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.006
10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00186-3
10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419
10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01039.x
10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-08.2008
10.1126/science.290.5492.812
10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
10.1093/cercor/bhu139
10.1126/science.1179013
10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.11.007
10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00079-9
10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7
10.1038/nn1825
10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00007-7
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0113-09.2009
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10941.2002
10.5665/sleep.4670
10.1038/nature05278
10.1073/pnas.0437938100
10.1126/science.1138581
10.3389/fneur.2012.00040
10.1073/pnas.91.15.7041
10.1126/science.8036517
10.1038/nrn2762
10.1038/nrn1607
10.1098/rstb.1971.0078
10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_433
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0.
Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0 2016
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0.
– notice: Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0 2016
DBID CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
7X8
7QG
7TK
5PM
DOI 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3175-15.2016
DatabaseName Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
CrossRef
MEDLINE - Academic
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
CrossRef
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
Animal Behavior Abstracts
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic

CrossRef
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1529-2401
EndPage 1409
ExternalDocumentID 10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3175_15_2016
26818525
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Journal Article
Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS
  grantid: F32 HD078223
– fundername: NICHD NIH HHS
  grantid: F32 HD 078223
– fundername: NINDS NIH HHS
  grantid: T32 NS047987
– fundername: NINDS NIH HHS
  grantid: T32 NS 047987
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-~X
.55
18M
2WC
34G
39C
53G
5GY
5RE
5VS
AAFWJ
ABBAR
ABIVO
ACGUR
ACNCT
ADBBV
ADCOW
AENEX
AFCFT
AFHIN
AFOSN
AHWXS
AIZTS
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
BAWUL
BTFSW
CGR
CS3
CUY
CVF
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EBS
ECM
EIF
EJD
F5P
GX1
H13
HYE
H~9
KQ8
L7B
NPM
OK1
P0W
P2P
QZG
R.V
RHF
RHI
RPM
TFN
TR2
W8F
WH7
WOQ
X7M
XJT
YBU
YHG
YKV
YNH
YSK
AAYXX
CITATION
7X8
7QG
7TK
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-b8e13e2fe4f4823bb5758a8bce9401bee75e84aadb12186193c10a34d78698443
IEDL.DBID RPM
ISSN 0270-6474
IngestDate Tue Sep 17 21:27:26 EDT 2024
Fri Oct 25 03:45:58 EDT 2024
Fri Oct 25 03:15:23 EDT 2024
Fri Aug 23 02:33:54 EDT 2024
Sat Sep 28 08:22:17 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 4
Keywords phase
slow-wave sleep
memory consolidation
slow oscillation
memory reactivation
Language English
License Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c500t-b8e13e2fe4f4823bb5758a8bce9401bee75e84aadb12186193c10a34d78698443
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
Author contributions: K.A.P. designed research; J.D.C. performed research; L.J.B. analyzed data; L.J.B., J.D.C., and K.A.P. wrote the paper.
ORCID 0000-0003-4415-4143
OpenAccessLink https://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneuro/36/4/1401.full.pdf
PMID 26818525
PQID 1761459709
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 9
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4728733
proquest_miscellaneous_1768572500
proquest_miscellaneous_1761459709
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3175_15_2016
pubmed_primary_26818525
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2016-Jan-27
2016-01-27
20160127
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2016-01-27
PublicationDate_xml – month: 01
  year: 2016
  text: 2016-Jan-27
  day: 27
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
PublicationTitle The Journal of neuroscience
PublicationTitleAlternate J Neurosci
PublicationYear 2016
Publisher Society for Neuroscience
Publisher_xml – name: Society for Neuroscience
References 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.7
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.6
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.4
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.9
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.8
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.17
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.18
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.15
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.37
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.16
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.3
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.2
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.1
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.19
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.31
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.10
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.32
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.30
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.13
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.35
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.14
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.36
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.11
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.33
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.12
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.34
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.28
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.29
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.26
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.27
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.20
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.21
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.24
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.25
2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.22
Benjamini (2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.5) 1995; 57
Mölle (2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.23) 2002; 22
References_xml – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.3
  doi: 10.1038/nn.3152
– volume: 57
  start-page: 289
  year: 1995
  ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.5
  article-title: Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing
  publication-title: J R Stat Soc Ser B
  contributor:
    fullname: Benjamini
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.14
  doi: 10.1126/science.aaa3841
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.26
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.006
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.8
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101567
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.32
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00471
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.27
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00169-0
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.18
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01096-6
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.4
  doi: 10.1038/nn.3203
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.25
  doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.006
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.1
  doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00186-3
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.21
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.24
  doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01039.x
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.6
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.15
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-08.2008
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.10
  doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.812
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.11
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.33
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu139
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.29
  doi: 10.1126/science.1179013
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.30
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.11.007
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.36
  doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00079-9
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.34
  doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.16
  doi: 10.1038/nn1825
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.22
  doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00007-7
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.7
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0113-09.2009
– volume: 22
  start-page: 10941
  year: 2002
  ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.23
  article-title: Grouping of spindle activity during slow oscillations in human non-rapid eye movement sleep
  publication-title: J Neurosci
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10941.2002
  contributor:
    fullname: Mölle
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.9
  doi: 10.5665/sleep.4670
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.20
  doi: 10.1038/nature05278
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.35
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0437938100
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.28
  doi: 10.1126/science.1138581
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.31
  doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00040
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.2
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.15.7041
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.37
  doi: 10.1126/science.8036517
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.12
  doi: 10.1038/nrn2762
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.13
  doi: 10.1038/nrn1607
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.19
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078
– ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.17
  doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_433
SSID ssj0007017
Score 2.4679167
Snippet Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
crossref
pubmed
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
StartPage 1401
SubjectTerms Acoustic Stimulation
Auditory Perception - physiology
Awareness
Biological Clocks - physiology
Brain Mapping
Cues
Electroencephalography
Evoked Potentials, Auditory - physiology
Female
Fourier Analysis
Humans
Male
Memory - physiology
Sleep - physiology
Young Adult
Title Phase of Spontaneous Slow Oscillations during Sleep Influences Memory-Related Processing of Auditory Cues
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26818525
https://search.proquest.com/docview/1761459709
https://search.proquest.com/docview/1768572500
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4728733
Volume 36
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LaxsxEB7inHopTdOHmzaoUHqT96GRpT0a05CkOE1xA7ktq5WWGOy1aRxK_n1n9mHygB56XWlfM8PO92m_0QB8Cd5769HIkCktMfVBOpc4iVh5R3BWJ5Zrh2cX49MrPL_W13ug-1qYRrRfusWoXq5G9eKm0VZuVmXU68Siy9kUDeF8paIBDChAe4refX5N3LTZJbpFvAgNdmXBRLii8wuWx82nZyPOmjLRLO3iJkbpmPMWt8t-mJyeIc6nwskHmejkFbzsIKSYtI96AHuhfg2Hk5ro8-pefBWNqLNZLT-ExeUNpSmxrsR8s64JCAZi-mK-XP8RPyj5LTslnGirFWkghI046xuX3IoZC3HvZSOZC150ZQU8lS454YoOGhZTev43cHXy7df0VHbdFWSp43grnQ2JCmkVsEKbKucIuNnCujJkxLlcCEYHi0XhXcJ9qwjolUlcKPTGjjOLqN7Cfr2uw3sQyurCpqgU79dFlix8jInLsMqKzDsMQ4h6s-abdhONnMkH-STf-SRnn9CxnH0yhM-99XOKd_6J0VooTwwBCmJBcfbPOVYbAnfxEN61Htvdt3f1EMwjX-4m8H7bj0coDJt9t7uw-_DfZx7BC345XsFJzUfY3_6-C58I02zdMQy-_7THTST_BVmj914
link.rule.ids 230,315,730,783,787,888,27938,27939,53806,53808
linkProvider National Library of Medicine
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3fb9MwED6N8QAvMBiMwgZGQrw5v-zUzmNVbWrHWiZ1Q3uL4tjRKtqkYq3Q-Ou5y49qGxISvMZOIvuzc985390BfHLWWm2l4i4RMZeRddyY0HApC2uQzsahptjhybQ_upSnV_HVDsRdLEwt2s_N3CsXS6-cX9faytUy9zudmH8-GUqFPF8I_xE8xv0a9Dsnvf0Aq6AutIsOF3pGUsk2MBhdLv90SgK52XDskd3kYUziLipjFPXJclHB7Lvm6Q_O-VA6eccWnTyHb90oGgnKd2-zNl7-60GCx38e5h48a9kpGzTNL2DHlS9hf1CiZ768ZZ9ZrRetD-L3YX5-jRaQVQWbraoSOaarNjdstqh-sq9oVxetyI41gZDY4NyKjbuaKDdsQhrfW16r8ZxlbcQCdcVHDihYBJvZECfmFVyeHF8MR7wt3MDzOAjW3GgXChcVThZSR8IY5IQ60yZ3CbpzxjkVOy2zzJqQSmIhh8zDIBPSKt1PtJTiNeyWVeneABM6znQkhaBUYAhRZgMZmkQWSZZYI10P_A6vdNXk50jJr0Gw0y3YKYGN11ICuwcfO1hT3Er0f6SZoTRUyFXQwQqSv_bRsULeGPTgoFkK2_d2a6gH6t4i2XagVN73WxD6OqV3C_Xb_77zAzwZXUzO0rPx9Ms7eEoDpYOiSB3C7vrHxh0hdVqb9_VG-Q3KbBhy
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3fb9MwED7BkBAvbDAYHQOMhHjLTzu181h1q9ZBS6UyaeIlimNHq2iTiLVC46_nLj-qbkg87DV2EtmfnfvO-e4O4JM1xigjpGNjHjkiNNbROtCOELnRSGejQFHs8GTaP78UF1fR1U6pr1q0n-mFWyxXbrG4rrWV1SrzOp2YN5sMhUSez7lXmdx7DE9wz_qqc9Tbj7D062K76HShdySkaIOD0e3yLqYkkpsPxy7ZTieISOBFpYzCPlkvKpq9a6L-4Z335ZM79mi0Dz-6kTQylJ_uZq3d7M-9JI8PGuoBPG9ZKhs0XV7AI1u8hMNBgR766pZ9ZrVutD6QP4TF7BotIStzNq_KArmmLTc3bL4sf7NvaF-XrdiONQGR2GBtxcZdbZQbNiGt761Tq_KsYW3kAnXFRw4oaASb2RAn5xVcjs6-D8-dtoCDk0W-v3a0sgG3YW5FLlTItUZuqFKlMxujW6etlZFVIk2NDqg0FnLJLPBTLoxU_VgJwV_DXlEW9g0wrqJUhYJzSgmGMKXGF4GORR6nsdHC9sDrMEuqJk9HQv4NAp5sAU8IcLyWEOA9-NhBm-CWov8kzQwlgUTOgo6WH_-3j4ok8ke_B0fNcti-t1tHPZB3Fsq2A6X0vtuC8NepvVu4jx985wd4OjsdJV_H0y9v4RmNk86LQnkCe-tfG_sOGdRav6_3yl-TLBry
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Phase+of+Spontaneous+Slow+Oscillations+during+Sleep+Influences+Memory-Related+Processing+of+Auditory+Cues&rft.jtitle=The+Journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Batterink%2C+Laura+J&rft.au=Creery%2C+Jessica+D&rft.au=Paller%2C+Ken+A&rft.date=2016-01-27&rft.eissn=1529-2401&rft.volume=36&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=1401&rft_id=info:doi/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.3175-15.2016&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F26818525&rft.externalDocID=26818525
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon