Phase of Spontaneous Slow Oscillations during Sleep Influences Memory-Related Processing of Auditory Cues
Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyper...
Saved in:
Published in | The Journal of neuroscience Vol. 36; no. 4; pp. 1401 - 1409 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Society for Neuroscience
27.01.2016
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates.
Slow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates.
Slow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates. Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates. Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat-meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENTSlow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates. Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow oscillations appear as high-amplitude, synchronized EEG activity, corresponding to upstates of neuronal depolarization and downstates of hyperpolarization. Memory reactivations occur spontaneously during SWS, and can also be induced by presenting learning-related cues associated with a prior learning episode during sleep. This technique, targeted memory reactivation (TMR), selectively enhances memory consolidation. Given that memory reactivation is thought to occur preferentially during the slow-oscillation upstate, we hypothesized that TMR stimulation effects would depend on the phase of the slow oscillation. Participants learned arbitrary spatial locations for objects that were each paired with a characteristic sound (eg, cat–meow). Then, during SWS periods of an afternoon nap, one-half of the sounds were presented at low intensity. When object location memory was subsequently tested, recall accuracy was significantly better for those objects cued during sleep. We report here for the first time that this memory benefit was predicted by slow-wave phase at the time of stimulation. For cued objects, location memories were categorized according to amount of forgetting from pre- to post-nap. Conditions of high versus low forgetting corresponded to stimulation timing at different slow-oscillation phases, suggesting that learning-related stimuli were more likely to be processed and trigger memory reactivation when they occurred at the optimal phase of a slow oscillation. These findings provide insight into mechanisms of memory reactivation during sleep, supporting the idea that reactivation is most likely during cortical upstates. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Slow-wave sleep (SWS) is characterized by synchronized neural activity alternating between active upstates and quiet downstates. The slow-oscillation upstates are thought to provide a window of opportunity for memory consolidation, particularly conducive to cortical plasticity. Recent evidence shows that sensory cues associated with previous learning can be delivered subtly during SWS to selectively enhance memory consolidation. Our results demonstrate that this behavioral benefit is predicted by slow-oscillation phase at stimulus presentation time. Cues associated with high versus low forgetting based on analysis of subsequent recall performance were delivered at opposite slow-oscillation phases. These results provide evidence of an optimal slow-oscillation phase for memory consolidation during sleep, supporting the idea that memory processing occurs preferentially during cortical upstates. |
Author | Creery, Jessica D Batterink, Laura J Paller, Ken A |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Laura J surname: Batterink fullname: Batterink, Laura J email: lbatterink@northwestern.edu organization: Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710 lbatterink@northwestern.edu – sequence: 2 givenname: Jessica D surname: Creery fullname: Creery, Jessica D organization: Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710 – sequence: 3 givenname: Ken A orcidid: 0000-0003-4415-4143 surname: Paller fullname: Paller, Ken A organization: Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710 |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26818525$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNUU1PGzEUtCpQCbR_AfnYywZ_rr2XSijiIwgIIuVseXffgquNndq7IP49DtAIbpye9GbeaObNPtrxwQNCh5RMqWT86OL65O52sZzNp5wqWVA5ZYSW39Ako1XBBKE7aEKYIkUplNhD-yn9JYQoQtV3tMdKTbVkcoLczYNNgEOHl-vgB-shjAkv-_CEF6lxfW8HF3zC7Ridv88AwBrPfdeP4BtI-ApWIT4Xt5CJ0OKbGPI2bahZ8nhs3ZBhPBsh_UC7ne0T_HyfB-ju9OTP7Ly4XJzNZ8eXRSMJGYpaA-XAOhCd0IzXtVRSW103UOVQNYCSoIW1bU0Z1SWteEOJ5aJVuqy0EPwA_X7TXY_1CtoG_BBtb9bRrWx8NsE68xnx7sHch0cjFNOK8yzw610ghn_Z-GBWLjWQX_H6HENVqaVi2e1XqFTISpEqU8s3ahNDShG6rSNKzKZSs63UbCrNO7OpNB8efsyzPfvfIX8BQEahiQ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2020_02_024 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2123430119 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2022_12_004 crossref_primary_10_1152_physiol_00004_2019 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2016_00426 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11055_019_00853_6 crossref_primary_10_1093_sleep_zsz315 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_017_0542_8 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2018_04_020 crossref_primary_10_3390_cells9010054 crossref_primary_10_1093_braincomms_fcac089 crossref_primary_10_3389_fncir_2022_1076354 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00109 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2023_105379 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_39178_2 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nlm_2021_107529 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnsys_2018_00040 crossref_primary_10_3724_SP_J_1042_2021_01251 crossref_primary_10_1097_WNR_0000000000001215 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pneurobio_2021_102174 crossref_primary_10_1088_1741_2552_ab0933 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2018_00028 crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_40583 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2017_11_020 crossref_primary_10_3389_fncel_2019_00071 crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_13855 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_017_1313_9 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41593_023_01324_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nlm_2018_03_003 crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_48881 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2020_525970 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2018_03_009 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00644_2016 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1008758 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40675_024_00291_y crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2023_03_005 crossref_primary_10_1042_ETLS20230109 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2021_108106 crossref_primary_10_1002_hipo_23526 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1427_19_2019 crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_024_05947_7 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15628 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1784_21_2023 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jsmc_2019_11_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jsmc_2022_06_013 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_smrv_2018_01_008 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2016_07_027 crossref_primary_10_1093_nc_niw014 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhaa228 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2123428119 |
Cites_doi | 10.1038/nn.3152 10.1126/science.aaa3841 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.006 10.1371/journal.pone.0101567 10.1162/jocn_a_00471 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00169-0 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01096-6 10.1038/nn.3203 10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.006 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00186-3 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01039.x 10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-08.2008 10.1126/science.290.5492.812 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 10.1093/cercor/bhu139 10.1126/science.1179013 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.11.007 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00079-9 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7 10.1038/nn1825 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00007-7 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0113-09.2009 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10941.2002 10.5665/sleep.4670 10.1038/nature05278 10.1073/pnas.0437938100 10.1126/science.1138581 10.3389/fneur.2012.00040 10.1073/pnas.91.15.7041 10.1126/science.8036517 10.1038/nrn2762 10.1038/nrn1607 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078 10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_433 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0. Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0 2016 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0. – notice: Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0 2016 |
DBID | CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM AAYXX CITATION 7X8 7QG 7TK 5PM |
DOI | 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3175-15.2016 |
DatabaseName | Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic Animal Behavior Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts Animal Behavior Abstracts |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1529-2401 |
EndPage | 1409 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3175_15_2016 26818525 |
Genre | Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS grantid: F32 HD078223 – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS grantid: F32 HD 078223 – fundername: NINDS NIH HHS grantid: T32 NS047987 – fundername: NINDS NIH HHS grantid: T32 NS 047987 |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .55 18M 2WC 34G 39C 53G 5GY 5RE 5VS AAFWJ ABBAR ABIVO ACGUR ACNCT ADBBV ADCOW AENEX AFCFT AFHIN AFOSN AHWXS AIZTS ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS BAWUL BTFSW CGR CS3 CUY CVF DIK DU5 E3Z EBS ECM EIF EJD F5P GX1 H13 HYE H~9 KQ8 L7B NPM OK1 P0W P2P QZG R.V RHF RHI RPM TFN TR2 W8F WH7 WOQ X7M XJT YBU YHG YKV YNH YSK AAYXX CITATION 7X8 7QG 7TK 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-b8e13e2fe4f4823bb5758a8bce9401bee75e84aadb12186193c10a34d78698443 |
IEDL.DBID | RPM |
ISSN | 0270-6474 |
IngestDate | Tue Sep 17 21:27:26 EDT 2024 Fri Oct 25 03:45:58 EDT 2024 Fri Oct 25 03:15:23 EDT 2024 Fri Aug 23 02:33:54 EDT 2024 Sat Sep 28 08:22:17 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 4 |
Keywords | phase slow-wave sleep memory consolidation slow oscillation memory reactivation |
Language | English |
License | Copyright © 2016 the authors 0270-6474/16/361401-09$15.00/0. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c500t-b8e13e2fe4f4823bb5758a8bce9401bee75e84aadb12186193c10a34d78698443 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 Author contributions: K.A.P. designed research; J.D.C. performed research; L.J.B. analyzed data; L.J.B., J.D.C., and K.A.P. wrote the paper. |
ORCID | 0000-0003-4415-4143 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneuro/36/4/1401.full.pdf |
PMID | 26818525 |
PQID | 1761459709 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 9 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4728733 proquest_miscellaneous_1768572500 proquest_miscellaneous_1761459709 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3175_15_2016 pubmed_primary_26818525 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2016-Jan-27 2016-01-27 20160127 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-01-27 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2016 text: 2016-Jan-27 day: 27 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | The Journal of neuroscience |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Neurosci |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | Society for Neuroscience |
Publisher_xml | – name: Society for Neuroscience |
References | 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.7 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.6 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.4 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.9 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.8 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.17 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.18 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.15 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.37 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.16 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.3 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.2 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.1 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.19 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.31 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.10 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.32 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.30 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.13 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.35 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.14 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.36 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.11 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.33 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.12 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.34 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.28 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.29 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.26 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.27 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.20 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.21 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.24 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.25 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.22 Benjamini (2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.5) 1995; 57 Mölle (2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.23) 2002; 22 |
References_xml | – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.3 doi: 10.1038/nn.3152 – volume: 57 start-page: 289 year: 1995 ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.5 article-title: Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing publication-title: J R Stat Soc Ser B contributor: fullname: Benjamini – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.14 doi: 10.1126/science.aaa3841 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.26 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.006 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101567 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.32 doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00471 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.27 doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00169-0 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.18 doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01096-6 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.4 doi: 10.1038/nn.3203 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.25 doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.006 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.1 doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00186-3 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.21 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.24 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01039.x – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.6 doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.15 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-08.2008 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.10 doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.812 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.11 doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.33 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu139 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.29 doi: 10.1126/science.1179013 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.30 doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.11.007 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.36 doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00079-9 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.34 doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.16 doi: 10.1038/nn1825 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.22 doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00007-7 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.7 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0113-09.2009 – volume: 22 start-page: 10941 year: 2002 ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.23 article-title: Grouping of spindle activity during slow oscillations in human non-rapid eye movement sleep publication-title: J Neurosci doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10941.2002 contributor: fullname: Mölle – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.9 doi: 10.5665/sleep.4670 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.20 doi: 10.1038/nature05278 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.35 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0437938100 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.28 doi: 10.1126/science.1138581 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.31 doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00040 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.15.7041 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.37 doi: 10.1126/science.8036517 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.12 doi: 10.1038/nrn2762 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.13 doi: 10.1038/nrn1607 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.19 doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078 – ident: 2023041803170137000_36.4.1401.17 doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_433 |
SSID | ssj0007017 |
Score | 2.4679167 |
Snippet | Slow oscillations during slow-wave sleep (SWS) may facilitate memory consolidation by regulating interactions between hippocampal and cortical networks. Slow... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest crossref pubmed |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | 1401 |
SubjectTerms | Acoustic Stimulation Auditory Perception - physiology Awareness Biological Clocks - physiology Brain Mapping Cues Electroencephalography Evoked Potentials, Auditory - physiology Female Fourier Analysis Humans Male Memory - physiology Sleep - physiology Young Adult |
Title | Phase of Spontaneous Slow Oscillations during Sleep Influences Memory-Related Processing of Auditory Cues |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26818525 https://search.proquest.com/docview/1761459709 https://search.proquest.com/docview/1768572500 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4728733 |
Volume | 36 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LaxsxEB7inHopTdOHmzaoUHqT96GRpT0a05CkOE1xA7ktq5WWGOy1aRxK_n1n9mHygB56XWlfM8PO92m_0QB8Cd5769HIkCktMfVBOpc4iVh5R3BWJ5Zrh2cX49MrPL_W13ug-1qYRrRfusWoXq5G9eKm0VZuVmXU68Siy9kUDeF8paIBDChAe4refX5N3LTZJbpFvAgNdmXBRLii8wuWx82nZyPOmjLRLO3iJkbpmPMWt8t-mJyeIc6nwskHmejkFbzsIKSYtI96AHuhfg2Hk5ro8-pefBWNqLNZLT-ExeUNpSmxrsR8s64JCAZi-mK-XP8RPyj5LTslnGirFWkghI046xuX3IoZC3HvZSOZC150ZQU8lS454YoOGhZTev43cHXy7df0VHbdFWSp43grnQ2JCmkVsEKbKucIuNnCujJkxLlcCEYHi0XhXcJ9qwjolUlcKPTGjjOLqN7Cfr2uw3sQyurCpqgU79dFlix8jInLsMqKzDsMQ4h6s-abdhONnMkH-STf-SRnn9CxnH0yhM-99XOKd_6J0VooTwwBCmJBcfbPOVYbAnfxEN61Htvdt3f1EMwjX-4m8H7bj0coDJt9t7uw-_DfZx7BC345XsFJzUfY3_6-C58I02zdMQy-_7THTST_BVmj914 |
link.rule.ids | 230,315,730,783,787,888,27938,27939,53806,53808 |
linkProvider | National Library of Medicine |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3fb9MwED6N8QAvMBiMwgZGQrw5v-zUzmNVbWrHWiZ1Q3uL4tjRKtqkYq3Q-Ou5y49qGxISvMZOIvuzc985390BfHLWWm2l4i4RMZeRddyY0HApC2uQzsahptjhybQ_upSnV_HVDsRdLEwt2s_N3CsXS6-cX9faytUy9zudmH8-GUqFPF8I_xE8xv0a9Dsnvf0Aq6AutIsOF3pGUsk2MBhdLv90SgK52XDskd3kYUziLipjFPXJclHB7Lvm6Q_O-VA6eccWnTyHb90oGgnKd2-zNl7-60GCx38e5h48a9kpGzTNL2DHlS9hf1CiZ768ZZ9ZrRetD-L3YX5-jRaQVQWbraoSOaarNjdstqh-sq9oVxetyI41gZDY4NyKjbuaKDdsQhrfW16r8ZxlbcQCdcVHDihYBJvZECfmFVyeHF8MR7wt3MDzOAjW3GgXChcVThZSR8IY5IQ60yZ3CbpzxjkVOy2zzJqQSmIhh8zDIBPSKt1PtJTiNeyWVeneABM6znQkhaBUYAhRZgMZmkQWSZZYI10P_A6vdNXk50jJr0Gw0y3YKYGN11ICuwcfO1hT3Er0f6SZoTRUyFXQwQqSv_bRsULeGPTgoFkK2_d2a6gH6t4i2XagVN73WxD6OqV3C_Xb_77zAzwZXUzO0rPx9Ms7eEoDpYOiSB3C7vrHxh0hdVqb9_VG-Q3KbBhy |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3fb9MwED7BkBAvbDAYHQOMhHjLTzu181h1q9ZBS6UyaeIlimNHq2iTiLVC46_nLj-qbkg87DV2EtmfnfvO-e4O4JM1xigjpGNjHjkiNNbROtCOELnRSGejQFHs8GTaP78UF1fR1U6pr1q0n-mFWyxXbrG4rrWV1SrzOp2YN5sMhUSez7lXmdx7DE9wz_qqc9Tbj7D062K76HShdySkaIOD0e3yLqYkkpsPxy7ZTieISOBFpYzCPlkvKpq9a6L-4Z335ZM79mi0Dz-6kTQylJ_uZq3d7M-9JI8PGuoBPG9ZKhs0XV7AI1u8hMNBgR766pZ9ZrVutD6QP4TF7BotIStzNq_KArmmLTc3bL4sf7NvaF-XrdiONQGR2GBtxcZdbZQbNiGt761Tq_KsYW3kAnXFRw4oaASb2RAn5xVcjs6-D8-dtoCDk0W-v3a0sgG3YW5FLlTItUZuqFKlMxujW6etlZFVIk2NDqg0FnLJLPBTLoxU_VgJwV_DXlEW9g0wrqJUhYJzSgmGMKXGF4GORR6nsdHC9sDrMEuqJk9HQv4NAp5sAU8IcLyWEOA9-NhBm-CWov8kzQwlgUTOgo6WH_-3j4ok8ke_B0fNcti-t1tHPZB3Fsq2A6X0vtuC8NepvVu4jx985wd4OjsdJV_H0y9v4RmNk86LQnkCe-tfG_sOGdRav6_3yl-TLBry |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Phase+of+Spontaneous+Slow+Oscillations+during+Sleep+Influences+Memory-Related+Processing+of+Auditory+Cues&rft.jtitle=The+Journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Batterink%2C+Laura+J&rft.au=Creery%2C+Jessica+D&rft.au=Paller%2C+Ken+A&rft.date=2016-01-27&rft.eissn=1529-2401&rft.volume=36&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=1401&rft_id=info:doi/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.3175-15.2016&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F26818525&rft.externalDocID=26818525 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon |