Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory
Rightward deviation on line bisection is considered one of the most classic clinical signs of unilateral visual neglect—a cognitive disorder of spatial processing that commonly follows right brain damage. Recently, short-term adaptation to wedge prisms has been shown to significantly reduce neglect...
Saved in:
Published in | Neuropsychologia Vol. 41; no. 1; pp. 25 - 39 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Oxford
Elsevier Ltd
01.01.2003
Elsevier Science Elsevier |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Rightward deviation on line bisection is considered one of the most classic clinical signs of unilateral visual neglect—a cognitive disorder of spatial processing that commonly follows right brain damage. Recently, short-term adaptation to wedge prisms has been shown to significantly reduce neglect on this and other conventional diagnostic tasks. Our previous study has shown that visuomotor adaptation in normals produces a similar pattern of directional bias on a line bisection task. Based on the good working knowledge of how neglect patients perform on different versions of the standard diagnostic task, we showed here that using leftward-deviating prisms in normals, it is possible to produce: (1) a reliable bias on line bisection, (2) a rightward specific deviation, (3) a modulation of rightward deviation, which depends on the relative spatial location of the target lines and (4) a line length effect. A final experiment confirmed that these after-effects are specific to prism adaptation rather than passive prism exposure. Collectively, these findings confirm that adaptation to left-deviating prisms in normals produces a reliable right-sided bias and as shown by a previous visuospatial judgement task, these findings cannot be adequately explained by the symmetric sensori–motor effects of prism adaptation. Taken together with the improvement of spatial neglect shown by right-deviating prisms only, the present study suggests that low level sensori–motor adaptations play a greater role in right hemisphere organisation for spatial cognition than previously thought. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Rightward deviation on line bisection is considered one of the most classic clinical signs of unilateral visual neglect--a cognitive disorder of spatial processing that commonly follows right brain damage. Recently, short-term adaptation to wedge prisms has been shown to significantly reduce neglect on this and other conventional diagnostic tasks. Our previous study has shown that visuomotor adaptation in normals produces a similar pattern of directional bias on a line bisection task. Based on the good working knowledge of how neglect patients perform on different versions of the standard diagnostic task, we showed here that using leftward-deviating prisms in normals, it is possible to produce: (1) a reliable bias on line bisection, (2) a rightward specific deviation, (3) a modulation of rightward deviation, which depends on the relative spatial location of the target lines and (4) a line length effect. A final experiment confirmed that these after-effects are specific to prism adaptation rather than passive prism exposure. Collectively, these findings confirm that adaptation to left-deviating prisms in normals produces a reliable right-sided bias and as shown by a previous visuospatial judgement task, these findings cannot be adequately explained by the symmetric sensori-motor effects of prism adaptation. Taken together with the improvement of spatial neglect shown by right-deviating prisms only, the present study suggests that low level sensori-motor adaptations play a greater role in right hemisphere organisation for spatial cognition than previously thought.Rightward deviation on line bisection is considered one of the most classic clinical signs of unilateral visual neglect--a cognitive disorder of spatial processing that commonly follows right brain damage. Recently, short-term adaptation to wedge prisms has been shown to significantly reduce neglect on this and other conventional diagnostic tasks. Our previous study has shown that visuomotor adaptation in normals produces a similar pattern of directional bias on a line bisection task. Based on the good working knowledge of how neglect patients perform on different versions of the standard diagnostic task, we showed here that using leftward-deviating prisms in normals, it is possible to produce: (1) a reliable bias on line bisection, (2) a rightward specific deviation, (3) a modulation of rightward deviation, which depends on the relative spatial location of the target lines and (4) a line length effect. A final experiment confirmed that these after-effects are specific to prism adaptation rather than passive prism exposure. Collectively, these findings confirm that adaptation to left-deviating prisms in normals produces a reliable right-sided bias and as shown by a previous visuospatial judgement task, these findings cannot be adequately explained by the symmetric sensori-motor effects of prism adaptation. Taken together with the improvement of spatial neglect shown by right-deviating prisms only, the present study suggests that low level sensori-motor adaptations play a greater role in right hemisphere organisation for spatial cognition than previously thought. Rightward deviation on line bisection is considered one of the most classic clinical signs of unilateral visual neglect--a cognitive disorder of spatial processing that commonly follows right brain damage. Recently, short-term adaptation to wedge prisms has been shown to significantly reduce neglect on this and other conventional diagnostic tasks. Our previous study has shown that visuomotor adaptation in normals produces a similar pattern of directional bias on a line bisection task. Based on the good working knowledge of how neglect patients perform on different versions of the standard diagnostic task, we showed here that using leftward-deviating prisms in normals, it is possible to produce: (1) a reliable bias on line bisection, (2) a rightward specific deviation, (3) a modulation of rightward deviation, which depends on the relative spatial location of the target lines and (4) a line length effect. A final experiment confirmed that these after-effects are specific to prism adaptation rather than passive prism exposure. Collectively, these findings confirm that adaptation to left-deviating prisms in normals produces a reliable right-sided bias and as shown by a previous visuospatial judgement task, these findings cannot be adequately explained by the symmetric sensori-motor effects of prism adaptation. Taken together with the improvement of spatial neglect shown by right-deviating prisms only, the present study suggests that low level sensori-motor adaptations play a greater role in right hemisphere organisation for spatial cognition than previously thought. |
Author | Pisella, Laure Rossetti, Yves Halligan, Peter W Michel, Carine Luauté, Jacques Rode, Gilles Boisson, Dominique |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Carine surname: Michel fullname: Michel, Carine organization: Espace et Action, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Unité 534, Bron, France – sequence: 2 givenname: Laure surname: Pisella fullname: Pisella, Laure organization: Espace et Action, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Unité 534, Bron, France – sequence: 3 givenname: Peter W surname: Halligan fullname: Halligan, Peter W organization: School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK – sequence: 4 givenname: Jacques surname: Luauté fullname: Luauté, Jacques organization: Espace et Action, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Unité 534, Bron, France – sequence: 5 givenname: Gilles surname: Rode fullname: Rode, Gilles organization: Espace et Action, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Unité 534, Bron, France – sequence: 6 givenname: Dominique surname: Boisson fullname: Boisson, Dominique organization: Espace et Action, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Unité 534, Bron, France – sequence: 7 givenname: Yves surname: Rossetti fullname: Rossetti, Yves email: rossetti@lyon151.inserm.fr organization: Espace et Action, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Unité 534, Bron, France |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=14024036$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12427563$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed https://hal.science/hal-02151001$$DView record in HAL |
BookMark | eNqFkV9rFDEUxYNU7Lb6EZR5UezD6E0ymcnqg5SirbDgQxV8C9nMTRvJJNtkptBvb3Zn3YIvzUv-8Dsnl3NOyFGIAQl5TeEDBdp-vAZgsuZLzt4DOwOgXNTiGVlQ2fGaC9ockcUBOSYnOf8BgEYw-YIcU9awTrR8QX5fu2HyenThppqCKydM2lcBbzyasXKhCjEN2udqyltmk1weKt3rzVhEMXyq3LDxzuwuubIxVeMtxvTwkjy3RYav9vsp-fXt68-Lq3r14_L7xfmqNs2SjjVFrpfWAl-3EiW166YH1sp2CZojXWNnre1QGk55I6CTBQehwVjWawkg-Sk5m31vtVdlukGnBxW1U1fnK7V9A0YFLfnc08K-m9lNincT5lENLhv0XgeMU1Ydk2UxKOCbPTitB-wPvv9yK8DbPaCz0d4mHYzLj1wDrAHeFu7zzJkUc05olXFzcGPSzisKatum2rWptlWVedWuTSWKWvynPnzwhO7LrMMS_L3DpLJxGAz2LpVWVR_dEw5_AQcctcU |
CODEN | NUPSA6 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1097_01_wnr_0000209013_01470_f8 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhac298 crossref_primary_10_1177_1073858403256689 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2007_08_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2011_12_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_rehab_2014_10_004 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_016_0784_z crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2008_01_021 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_14266 crossref_primary_10_1097_WCO_0b013e328010924b crossref_primary_10_1007_s10339_017_0833_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2012_01_023 crossref_primary_10_1080_13825580600826454 crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0310_17_2017 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_018_29283_z crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2021_e06877 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00429_017_1572_2 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_016_0840_z crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2019_107297 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2014_02_012 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3184_13_2014 crossref_primary_10_3233_THC_191973 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2013_08_006 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2006_06_003 crossref_primary_10_13066_kspm_2013_8_3_337 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2005_04_009 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2010_04_001 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhw386 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2022_11_003 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10670_024_00829_0 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01668 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2020_103219 crossref_primary_10_1080_1357650X_2010_551128 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4678_5 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1468_1331_2006_01364_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2004_10_009 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0010_9452_08_70166_7 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_909686 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_4923_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2017_12_021 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2019_02_007 crossref_primary_10_1017_S1355617706060553 crossref_primary_10_1017_S1355617710000597 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2009_06_002 crossref_primary_10_1097_j_pain_0000000000002053 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_023_06697_4 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2012_07_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2015_02_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2011_09_029 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01979 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_4934_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2005_09_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2013_02_007 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_0956_7976_2004_00696_x crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_850495 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_rehab_2015_01_002 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_5038_9 crossref_primary_10_11596_asiajot_20_142 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2013_11_013 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci13040641 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2018_11_013 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2007_12_011 crossref_primary_10_12688_f1000research_2_215_v1 crossref_primary_10_12688_f1000research_2_232_v1 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2012_01_008 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_02_017 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2013_08_067 crossref_primary_10_1097_WNR_0b013e328337f95f crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_915260 crossref_primary_10_1111_jnp_12316 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0028_3932_03_00003_4 crossref_primary_10_1097_00001756_200405190_00024 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_1153_15_2016 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_77027_z crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2021_04_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2019_01_015 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_09_009 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2007_19_2_341 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107692 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2024_05_014 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_02_024 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2018_10_018 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_survophthal_2010_08_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2007_09_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2018_10_014 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2018_10_017 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2024_105814 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2005_02_003 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2010_01_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2019_01_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2012_11_008 crossref_primary_10_11596_asiajot_19_117 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_09_015 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2009_21218 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_007_1152_4 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2009_05_009 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2004_03_008 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2012_03_029 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2017_02_003 crossref_primary_10_1093_brain_awp327 crossref_primary_10_1097_WNR_0b013e3283313e9e |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/h0046207 10.1037/h0044984 10.1093/ageing/22.1.46 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941618 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06283.2001 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.87 10.2466/pms.2000.91.1.217 10.1136/jnnp.57.10.1228 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00107-5 10.1093/oso/9780195081237.001.0001 10.1016/0278-2626(83)90027-1 10.1002/ana.410030609 10.1016/0028-3932(74)90013-X 10.1068/p240415 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.171 10.1097/00001756-200006260-00019 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002155 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90079-F 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01218-2 10.1080/00222899709600827 10.1093/brain/121.10.2003 10.1007/s002219900322 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00064-1 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00115-8 10.1093/oso/9780198510697.003.0004 10.3758/BF03211756 10.1002/ana.410050210 10.1097/00001756-200005150-00031 10.1038/383618a0 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80369-3 10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00143-1 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00125-0 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1960 10.1016/0278-2626(90)90018-J 10.1136/jnnp.57.4.464 10.3758/BF03207568 10.2466/pms.1965.20.3c.1070 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941619 10.1212/WNL.54.6.1324 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00151-8 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90055-Q 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198508335.003.0027 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00131-0 10.1093/brain/121.5.907 10.1007/BF00243223 10.1016/S0010-9452(88)80040-6 10.1037/0033-2909.83.5.783 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61717-3 10.1038/25988 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2003 2003 INIST-CNRS Copyright 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2003 – notice: 2003 INIST-CNRS – notice: Copyright 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. – notice: Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 1XC |
DOI | 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine |
EISSN | 1873-3514 |
EndPage | 39 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_HAL_hal_02151001v1 12427563 14024036 10_1016_S0028_3932_02_00135_5 S0028393202001355 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Comparative Study |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M --Z -~X .GJ .~1 0R~ 123 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29N 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 53G 5RE 5VS 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAGJA AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAXLA AAXUO AAYJJ ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABMAC ABOYX ABXDB ABYKQ ACCUC ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACIUM ACNCT ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS ADMUD AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AENEX AETEA AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ ASPBG AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC C45 CS3 D0S DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q GBLVA HMQ HMW HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IHE J1W KOM L7B M2V M3V M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OKEIE OVD OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSY SSZ T5K TEORI TN5 UHB UQL WH7 WUQ XJT XOL YQT ZGI ZKB ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADNMO ADXHL AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH 08R AAPBV ABPIF ABPTK IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM PKN 7X8 1XC |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c491t-1e3a9ff03b68e81fb4d0268690a3e1be7fff7e8c31345078e3a05a0cf2da80083 |
IEDL.DBID | AIKHN |
ISSN | 0028-3932 |
IngestDate | Tue Jun 17 06:51:28 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 03:20:23 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:34:22 EST 2025 Sun Oct 22 16:04:18 EDT 2023 Tue Jul 01 04:13:49 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:09:10 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:28:17 EST 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 1 |
Keywords | Prism adaptation Visuomotor Bisection Space representation Spatial neglect Plasticity Human Perceptual adaptation Healthy subject Cognition Experimental study Sensorimotor coordination Prism Spatial representation Bisection task |
Language | English |
License | https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0 CC BY 4.0 Copyright 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c491t-1e3a9ff03b68e81fb4d0268690a3e1be7fff7e8c31345078e3a05a0cf2da80083 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0003-2751-1217 0000-0002-6387-9572 0000-0001-8867-4496 0000-0001-6550-3774 0009-0002-5207-3546 0000-0002-7136-7235 |
PMID | 12427563 |
PQID | 72888820 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 15 |
ParticipantIDs | hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_02151001v1 proquest_miscellaneous_72888820 pubmed_primary_12427563 pascalfrancis_primary_14024036 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_S0028_3932_02_00135_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0028_3932_02_00135_5 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_S0028_3932_02_00135_5 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2003-01-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2003-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2003 text: 2003-01-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Oxford |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Oxford – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Neuropsychologia |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Neuropsychologia |
PublicationYear | 2003 |
Publisher | Elsevier Ltd Elsevier Science Elsevier |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Ltd – name: Elsevier Science – name: Elsevier |
References | Karnath, Niemeier (BIB21) 2002; 1387 MacLeod, Turnbull (BIB25) 1999; 37 Pisella, Rode, Farné, Boisson, Rossetti (BIB40) 2001; 40 Redding, Wallace (BIB44) 1996; 22 Colent, Pisella, Bernieri, Rode, Rossetti (BIB7) 2000; 11 Halligan, Marshall (BIB11) 1988; 24 Reuter-Lorenz, Kinsbourne, Moscovitch (BIB46) 1990; 12 Clower, Hoffman, Votaw, Faber, Woods, Alexander (BIB5) 1996; 383 Karnath, Sievering, Fetter (BIB20) 1994; 101 Stone, Halligan, Greenwood (BIB55) 1993; 22 McCourt, Olafson (BIB30) 1997; 35 Harvey, Milner (BIB13) 1999; 37 Redding, Wallace (BIB42) 1985; 37 Thach, Goodkin, Keating (BIB56) 1992; 15 Jeannerod, Rossetti (BIB19) 1993; 2 Mozer, Halligan, Marshall (BIB37) 1997; 9 Ingram, Van Donkelaar, Cole, Vercher, Gauthier, Miall (BIB18) 2000; 132 Milner, Brechmann, Pagliarini (BIB34) 1992; 30 Redding, Wallace (BIB43) 1992; 24 Chatterjee A. Representational anysometry and representational release in neglect. In: Karnath HO, Milner AD, Vallar G, editors. The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect. Oxford University Press; in press. Held, Hein (BIB16) 1958; 8 Mattingley, Bradshaw, Bradshaw (BIB28) 1994; 57 Vallar, Rusconi, Bignamini, Geminiani, Perani (BIB58) 1994; 57 Milner AD. Animal models for the syndrome of spatial neglect. In: Jeannerod M, editor. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier; 1987. p. 259–88. Redding, Rader, Lucas (BIB41) 1992; 24 Welch RB. Adaptation of space perception. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP, editors. Handbook of perception and human performance. Sensory process and perception, vol. 1. New York: Wiley; 1986. p. 24.1–45. Held, Bossom (BIB17) 1961; 54 Rossetti Y, Pisella L. Several ‘vision for action’ systems: a guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In: Prinz W, Hommel B, editors. Attention and Performance XIX Common mechanisms in perception and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 62–119. Fink, Marshall, Shah, Weiss, Halligan, Grosse-Ruyken (BIB9) 2000; 54 Pick, Hay (BIB38) 1965; 20 Heilman, Valenstein (BIB14) 1979; 5 Bisiach, Bulgarelli, Sterzi, Vallar (BIB3) 1983; 2 Rossetti, Rode, Pisella, Farné, Li, Boisson (BIB50) 1998; 395 Welch, Bridgeman, Anand, Browman (BIB61) 1993; 54 Tilikete, Rode, Rossetti, Pichon, Li, Boisson (BIB57) 2001; 11 Mattingley, Bradshaw, Bradshaw (BIB29) 1995; 24 Sandor, Bachtold, Henn, Brugger (BIB52) 2000; 13 Watson, Miller, Heilman (BIB59) 1978; 3 Schmahmann (BIB53) 1998; 2 Halligan PW. Drawing attention to neglect. The Psychologist 1995;257–64. Rode, Rossetti, Boisson (BIB47) 2001; 39 Stevens, Guirao (BIB54) 1963; 66 Marshall, Halligan (BIB26) 1995; 31 Mcdonald-Nethercott, Kinnear, Venneri (BIB31) 2000; 91 Redding, Wallace (BIB45) 1997; 29 Pisella L, Michel C, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Rossetti Y. Prism adaptation following a bilateral lesion of the posterior parietal cortex, submitted for publication. Marshall, Lazar, Krakauer, Sharma (BIB27) 1998; 121 Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L, Farné A, Li L, Boisson D. Sensori–motor plasticity and cognition: prism adaptation can affect various levels of space representation. In: Grealy M, Thomson JA, editors. Studies in perception and action. New York: Erlbaum Ass; 1999. p. 265–69. Flemming, Behrmann (BIB10) 1998; 36 McIntosh RD, Rossetti Y, Milner AD. Prism adaptation improves chronic visual and haptic neglect. Cortex; in press. Rossetti Y, Rode G. Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: non-cognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder. In: Karnath HO, Milner AD, Vallar G, editors. The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 375–96. Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E. Neglect and related disorders. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical neuropsychology. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. p. 279–336. Monaghan, Shillcock (BIB36) 1998; 121 Luauté J, Michel C, Rode G, Boisson D, Halligan PW, Costes N, et al. Cerebral plasticity induced by prismatic adaptation in neglect patients. A PET study. In: Proceedings of the Third World Congress on Neurological Rehabilitation, Venice, Italy, April 2002. Baizer, Kralj-Hans, Glickstein (BIB1) 1999; 81 Clower, Robert, Lynch, Strick (BIB6) 2001; 21 Kornheiser (BIB23) 1976; 83 Milner, Harvey, Roberts, Fosrest (BIB35) 1993; 31 Kinsbourne (BIB22) 1974; 12 Fierro, Brighina, Oliveri, Piazza, La Bua, Buffa (BIB8) 2000; 11 Bedford (BIB2) 1989; 15 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB15 Stone (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB55) 1993; 22 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB12 Redding (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB43) 1992; 24 Fierro (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB8) 2000; 11 Thach (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB56) 1992; 15 Halligan (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB11) 1988; 24 Redding (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB45) 1997; 29 Heilman (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB14) 1979; 5 Rode (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB47) 2001; 39 Held (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB16) 1958; 8 Karnath (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB21) 2002; 1387 Clower (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB5) 1996; 383 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB60 Mozer (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB37) 1997; 9 Watson (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB59) 1978; 3 Mattingley (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB29) 1995; 24 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB48 Flemming (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB10) 1998; 36 Tilikete (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB57) 2001; 11 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB49 McCourt (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB30) 1997; 35 Pick (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB38) 1965; 20 Held (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB17) 1961; 54 Kornheiser (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB23) 1976; 83 Colent (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB7) 2000; 11 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB51 Kinsbourne (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB22) 1974; 12 Fink (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB9) 2000; 54 Milner (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB34) 1992; 30 Schmahmann (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB53) 1998; 2 Bisiach (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB3) 1983; 2 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB32 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB33 Monaghan (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB36) 1998; 121 Harvey (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB13) 1999; 37 MacLeod (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB25) 1999; 37 Redding (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB41) 1992; 24 Bedford (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB2) 1989; 15 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB39 Redding (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB42) 1985; 37 Jeannerod (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB19) 1993; 2 Welch (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB61) 1993; 54 Karnath (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB20) 1994; 101 Clower (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB6) 2001; 21 Pisella (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB40) 2001; 40 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB4 Ingram (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB18) 2000; 132 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB24 Marshall (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB26) 1995; 31 Marshall (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB27) 1998; 121 Redding (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB44) 1996; 22 Milner (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB35) 1993; 31 Mattingley (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB28) 1994; 57 Mcdonald-Nethercott (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB31) 2000; 91 Stevens (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB54) 1963; 66 Rossetti (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB50) 1998; 395 Vallar (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB58) 1994; 57 Sandor (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB52) 2000; 13 Baizer (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB1) 1999; 81 Reuter-Lorenz (10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB46) 1990; 12 |
References_xml | – volume: 24 start-page: 226 year: 1992 end-page: 237 ident: BIB43 article-title: Effects of pointing rate and availability of visual feedback on visual and proprioceptive components of prism adaptation publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – volume: 30 start-page: 515 year: 1992 end-page: 526 ident: BIB34 article-title: To halve and to halve not: an analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 2 start-page: 362 year: 1998 end-page: 371 ident: BIB53 article-title: Dysmetria of thought: clinical consequences of cerebellar dysfunction on cognition and affect publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences – volume: 3 start-page: 505 year: 1978 end-page: 508 ident: BIB59 article-title: Nonsensory neglect publication-title: Annals of Neurology – volume: 101 start-page: 140 year: 1994 end-page: 146 ident: BIB20 article-title: The interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation to subjective “Straight ahead” orientation in man publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 22 start-page: 379 year: 1996 end-page: 394 ident: BIB44 article-title: Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 395 start-page: 166 year: 1998 end-page: 169 ident: BIB50 article-title: Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect publication-title: Nature – volume: 37 start-page: 225 year: 1985 end-page: 230 ident: BIB42 article-title: Cognitive interference in prism adaptation publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – volume: 15 start-page: 232 year: 1989 end-page: 248 ident: BIB2 article-title: Constraints on learning new mappings between perceptual dimensions publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 31 start-page: 367 year: 1995 end-page: 376 ident: BIB26 article-title: Within- and between-task dissociations in visuo-spatial neglect: a case study publication-title: Cortex – volume: 1387 start-page: 1 year: 2002 end-page: 9 ident: BIB21 article-title: Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behaviour of patients with spatial neglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 54 start-page: 33 year: 1961 end-page: 37 ident: BIB17 article-title: Neonatal deprivation and adult rearrangement: elementary techniques for analysing plastic sensori–motor coordinations publication-title: Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology – reference: Luauté J, Michel C, Rode G, Boisson D, Halligan PW, Costes N, et al. Cerebral plasticity induced by prismatic adaptation in neglect patients. A PET study. In: Proceedings of the Third World Congress on Neurological Rehabilitation, Venice, Italy, April 2002. – volume: 15 start-page: 403 year: 1992 end-page: 442 ident: BIB56 article-title: The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement publication-title: Annual Review of Neuroscience – volume: 8 start-page: 87 year: 1958 end-page: 90 ident: BIB16 article-title: Adaptation of disarranged hand–eye coordination contingent upon re-afferent stimulation publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills – volume: 5 start-page: 166 year: 1979 end-page: 170 ident: BIB14 article-title: Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect publication-title: Annals of Neurology – volume: 37 start-page: 745 year: 1999 end-page: 750 ident: BIB13 article-title: Residual perceptual distorsion in ‘recovered’ hemispatial neglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 91 start-page: 217 year: 2000 end-page: 226 ident: BIB31 article-title: Bisection of shapes and lines: analysis of the visual and motor aspects of pseudoneglect publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills – volume: 11 start-page: 1 year: 2001 end-page: 20 ident: BIB57 article-title: Prism adaptation to rightward optical deviation improves postural imbalance in neglect hemiparetic patients publication-title: Current Biology – reference: Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E. Neglect and related disorders. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical neuropsychology. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993. p. 279–336. – volume: 21 start-page: 6283 year: 2001 end-page: 6291 ident: BIB6 article-title: The inferior parietal lobule is the target of output from the superior colliculus, hippocampus and cerebellum publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – reference: Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L, Farné A, Li L, Boisson D. Sensori–motor plasticity and cognition: prism adaptation can affect various levels of space representation. In: Grealy M, Thomson JA, editors. Studies in perception and action. New York: Erlbaum Ass; 1999. p. 265–69. – volume: 24 start-page: 321 year: 1988 end-page: 328 ident: BIB11 article-title: How long is a piece of string? A study of line bisection in a case of visual neglect publication-title: Cortex – volume: 24 start-page: 415 year: 1995 end-page: 433 ident: BIB29 article-title: The effects of unilateral visuospatial neglect of perception on Muller–Lyer illusory figures publication-title: Perception – volume: 9 start-page: 171 year: 1997 end-page: 190 ident: BIB37 article-title: The end of the line for a brain-damaged model of unilateral neglect publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 11 start-page: 1519 year: 2000 end-page: 1521 ident: BIB8 article-title: Controlateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rTMS in healthy subjects publication-title: NeuroReport – reference: Milner AD. Animal models for the syndrome of spatial neglect. In: Jeannerod M, editor. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier; 1987. p. 259–88. – volume: 54 start-page: 195 year: 1993 end-page: 204 ident: BIB61 article-title: Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – reference: Pisella L, Michel C, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Rossetti Y. Prism adaptation following a bilateral lesion of the posterior parietal cortex, submitted for publication. – volume: 11 start-page: 1899 year: 2000 end-page: 1902 ident: BIB7 article-title: Cognitive bias induced by visuo-motor adaptation to prisms: a simulation of unilateral neglect in normal individuals? publication-title: NeuroReport – volume: 20 start-page: 1070 year: 1965 end-page: 1072 ident: BIB38 article-title: A passive test of the Held reafference theory publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills – reference: Welch RB. Adaptation of space perception. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP, editors. Handbook of perception and human performance. Sensory process and perception, vol. 1. New York: Wiley; 1986. p. 24.1–45. – volume: 39 start-page: 1250 year: 2001 end-page: 1254 ident: BIB47 article-title: Prism adaptation improves representational neglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 57 start-page: 1228 year: 1994 end-page: 1235 ident: BIB28 article-title: Horizontal visual motion modulates focal attention in left unilateral spatial neglect publication-title: Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry – volume: 36 start-page: 469 year: 1998 end-page: 475 ident: BIB10 article-title: Visuospatial neglect in normal subjects: altered spatial representations induced by a perceptual illusion publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 31 start-page: 39 year: 1993 end-page: 49 ident: BIB35 article-title: Line bisection errors in visual neglect: misguided action or size distorsion? publication-title: Neuropsychologia – reference: Halligan PW. Drawing attention to neglect. The Psychologist 1995;257–64. – volume: 40 start-page: 327 year: 2001 end-page: 334 ident: BIB40 article-title: Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 22 start-page: 46 year: 1993 end-page: 52 ident: BIB55 article-title: The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke publication-title: Age Ageing – volume: 13 start-page: 188 year: 2000 end-page: 194 ident: BIB52 article-title: Effects of optokinetically induced rotatory self-motion on spatial perception and representation publication-title: Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychological Behavioural Neurology – volume: 35 start-page: 369 year: 1997 end-page: 380 ident: BIB30 article-title: Cognitive and perceptual influence on visual line bisection: psychological and chronometric analyses of pseudoneglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 66 start-page: 177 year: 1963 end-page: 186 ident: BIB54 article-title: Subjective scaling of length and area and the matching of length to loudness and brightness publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology – reference: McIntosh RD, Rossetti Y, Milner AD. Prism adaptation improves chronic visual and haptic neglect. Cortex; in press. – volume: 12 start-page: 240 year: 1990 end-page: 266 ident: BIB46 article-title: Hemispheric control of spatial attention publication-title: Brain and Cognition – volume: 132 start-page: 114 year: 2000 end-page: 126 ident: BIB18 article-title: The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 54 start-page: 1324 year: 2000 end-page: 1331 ident: BIB9 article-title: Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fRMI publication-title: Neurology – volume: 2 start-page: 32 year: 1983 end-page: 38 ident: BIB3 article-title: Line bisection and cognitive plasticity of unilateral neglect of space publication-title: Brain and Cognition – volume: 81 start-page: 1960 year: 1999 end-page: 1965 ident: BIB1 article-title: Cerellar lesions and prism adaptation in macaque monkeys publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology – volume: 57 start-page: 464 year: 1994 end-page: 470 ident: BIB58 article-title: Anatomical correlates of visual and tactile extinction in humans: a clinical CT scan study publication-title: Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry – reference: Rossetti Y, Pisella L. Several ‘vision for action’ systems: a guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In: Prinz W, Hommel B, editors. Attention and Performance XIX Common mechanisms in perception and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 62–119. – volume: 29 start-page: 119 year: 1997 end-page: 130 ident: BIB45 article-title: Prism adaptation during target pointing from visible and nonvisible starting locations publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – volume: 37 start-page: 707 year: 1999 end-page: 713 ident: BIB25 article-title: Motor and perceptual factor in pseudoneglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 121 start-page: 907 year: 1998 end-page: 921 ident: BIB36 article-title: The cross-over effect in unilateral neglect modelling detailed data in the line-bisection task publication-title: Brain – reference: Rossetti Y, Rode G. Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: non-cognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder. In: Karnath HO, Milner AD, Vallar G, editors. The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 375–96. – volume: 24 start-page: 238 year: 1992 end-page: 246 ident: BIB41 article-title: Cognitive load and prism adaptation publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior – reference: Chatterjee A. Representational anysometry and representational release in neglect. In: Karnath HO, Milner AD, Vallar G, editors. The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect. Oxford University Press; in press. – volume: 83 start-page: 783 year: 1976 end-page: 816 ident: BIB23 article-title: Adaptation to laterally displaced vision: a review publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 2 start-page: 439 year: 1993 end-page: 460 ident: BIB19 article-title: Visuomotor coordination as a dissociable visual function: experimental and clinical evidence publication-title: Bailliére’s Clinical Neurology – volume: 12 start-page: 279 year: 1974 end-page: 281 ident: BIB22 article-title: Direction of gaze and distribution of cerebral thought processes publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 121 start-page: 2003 year: 1998 end-page: 2010 ident: BIB27 article-title: Stimulus context in hemineglect publication-title: Brain – volume: 383 start-page: 618 year: 1996 end-page: 621 ident: BIB5 article-title: Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching publication-title: Nature – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB24 – volume: 54 start-page: 33 year: 1961 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB17 article-title: Neonatal deprivation and adult rearrangement: elementary techniques for analysing plastic sensori–motor coordinations publication-title: Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology doi: 10.1037/h0046207 – volume: 66 start-page: 177 year: 1963 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB54 article-title: Subjective scaling of length and area and the matching of length to loudness and brightness publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1037/h0044984 – volume: 22 start-page: 46 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB55 article-title: The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke publication-title: Age Ageing doi: 10.1093/ageing/22.1.46 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB4 – volume: 24 start-page: 226 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB43 article-title: Effects of pointing rate and availability of visual feedback on visual and proprioceptive components of prism adaptation publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941618 – volume: 21 start-page: 6283 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB6 article-title: The inferior parietal lobule is the target of output from the superior colliculus, hippocampus and cerebellum publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06283.2001 – volume: 8 start-page: 87 year: 1958 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB16 article-title: Adaptation of disarranged hand–eye coordination contingent upon re-afferent stimulation publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.87 – volume: 91 start-page: 217 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB31 article-title: Bisection of shapes and lines: analysis of the visual and motor aspects of pseudoneglect publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.1.217 – volume: 57 start-page: 1228 issue: 10 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB28 article-title: Horizontal visual motion modulates focal attention in left unilateral spatial neglect publication-title: Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.10.1228 – volume: 40 start-page: 327 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB40 article-title: Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00107-5 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB15 doi: 10.1093/oso/9780195081237.001.0001 – volume: 2 start-page: 32 year: 1983 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB3 article-title: Line bisection and cognitive plasticity of unilateral neglect of space publication-title: Brain and Cognition doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(83)90027-1 – volume: 3 start-page: 505 year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB59 article-title: Nonsensory neglect publication-title: Annals of Neurology doi: 10.1002/ana.410030609 – volume: 12 start-page: 279 year: 1974 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB22 article-title: Direction of gaze and distribution of cerebral thought processes publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(74)90013-X – volume: 24 start-page: 415 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB29 article-title: The effects of unilateral visuospatial neglect of perception on Muller–Lyer illusory figures publication-title: Perception doi: 10.1068/p240415 – volume: 9 start-page: 171 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB37 article-title: The end of the line for a brain-damaged model of unilateral neglect publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.171 – volume: 11 start-page: 1899 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB7 article-title: Cognitive bias induced by visuo-motor adaptation to prisms: a simulation of unilateral neglect in normal individuals? publication-title: NeuroReport doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006260-00019 – volume: 1387 start-page: 1 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB21 article-title: Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behaviour of patients with spatial neglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 15 start-page: 403 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB56 article-title: The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement publication-title: Annual Review of Neuroscience doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002155 – volume: 31 start-page: 39 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB35 article-title: Line bisection errors in visual neglect: misguided action or size distorsion? publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90079-F – volume: 2 start-page: 362 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB53 article-title: Dysmetria of thought: clinical consequences of cerebellar dysfunction on cognition and affect publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01218-2 – volume: 22 start-page: 379 year: 1996 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB44 article-title: Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 29 start-page: 119 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB45 article-title: Prism adaptation during target pointing from visible and nonvisible starting locations publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222899709600827 – volume: 121 start-page: 2003 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB27 article-title: Stimulus context in hemineglect publication-title: Brain doi: 10.1093/brain/121.10.2003 – volume: 132 start-page: 114 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB18 article-title: The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s002219900322 – volume: 39 start-page: 1250 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB47 article-title: Prism adaptation improves representational neglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00064-1 – volume: 37 start-page: 707 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB25 article-title: Motor and perceptual factor in pseudoneglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00115-8 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB48 doi: 10.1093/oso/9780198510697.003.0004 – volume: 54 start-page: 195 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB61 article-title: Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03211756 – volume: 5 start-page: 166 year: 1979 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB14 article-title: Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect publication-title: Annals of Neurology doi: 10.1002/ana.410050210 – volume: 2 start-page: 439 year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB19 article-title: Visuomotor coordination as a dissociable visual function: experimental and clinical evidence publication-title: Bailliére’s Clinical Neurology – volume: 11 start-page: 1519 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB8 article-title: Controlateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rTMS in healthy subjects publication-title: NeuroReport doi: 10.1097/00001756-200005150-00031 – volume: 383 start-page: 618 year: 1996 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB5 article-title: Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/383618a0 – volume: 31 start-page: 367 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB26 article-title: Within- and between-task dissociations in visuo-spatial neglect: a case study publication-title: Cortex doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80369-3 – volume: 35 start-page: 369 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB30 article-title: Cognitive and perceptual influence on visual line bisection: psychological and chronometric analyses of pseudoneglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00143-1 – volume: 37 start-page: 745 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB13 article-title: Residual perceptual distorsion in ‘recovered’ hemispatial neglect publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00125-0 – volume: 81 start-page: 1960 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB1 article-title: Cerellar lesions and prism adaptation in macaque monkeys publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.4.1960 – volume: 12 start-page: 240 year: 1990 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB46 article-title: Hemispheric control of spatial attention publication-title: Brain and Cognition doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(90)90018-J – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB32 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB51 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB39 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB60 – volume: 57 start-page: 464 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB58 article-title: Anatomical correlates of visual and tactile extinction in humans: a clinical CT scan study publication-title: Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.4.464 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB12 – volume: 37 start-page: 225 year: 1985 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB42 article-title: Cognitive interference in prism adaptation publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03207568 – volume: 20 start-page: 1070 year: 1965 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB38 article-title: A passive test of the Held reafference theory publication-title: Perceptual and Motor Skills doi: 10.2466/pms.1965.20.3c.1070 – volume: 24 start-page: 238 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB41 article-title: Cognitive load and prism adaptation publication-title: Journal of Motor Behavior doi: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941619 – volume: 54 start-page: 1324 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB9 article-title: Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fRMI publication-title: Neurology doi: 10.1212/WNL.54.6.1324 – volume: 11 start-page: 1 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB57 article-title: Prism adaptation to rightward optical deviation improves postural imbalance in neglect hemiparetic patients publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00151-8 – volume: 13 start-page: 188 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB52 article-title: Effects of optokinetically induced rotatory self-motion on spatial perception and representation publication-title: Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychological Behavioural Neurology – volume: 30 start-page: 515 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB34 article-title: To halve and to halve not: an analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90055-Q – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB49 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198508335.003.0027 – volume: 36 start-page: 469 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB10 article-title: Visuospatial neglect in normal subjects: altered spatial representations induced by a perceptual illusion publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00131-0 – volume: 121 start-page: 907 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB36 article-title: The cross-over effect in unilateral neglect modelling detailed data in the line-bisection task publication-title: Brain doi: 10.1093/brain/121.5.907 – volume: 101 start-page: 140 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB20 article-title: The interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation to subjective “Straight ahead” orientation in man publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/BF00243223 – volume: 15 start-page: 232 year: 1989 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB2 article-title: Constraints on learning new mappings between perceptual dimensions publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 24 start-page: 321 year: 1988 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB11 article-title: How long is a piece of string? A study of line bisection in a case of visual neglect publication-title: Cortex doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(88)80040-6 – volume: 83 start-page: 783 year: 1976 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB23 article-title: Adaptation to laterally displaced vision: a review publication-title: Psychological Bulletin doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.83.5.783 – ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB33 doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61717-3 – volume: 395 start-page: 166 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5_BIB50 article-title: Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/25988 |
SSID | ssj0004528 |
Score | 2.094957 |
Snippet | Rightward deviation on line bisection is considered one of the most classic clinical signs of unilateral visual neglect—a cognitive disorder of spatial... Rightward deviation on line bisection is considered one of the most classic clinical signs of unilateral visual neglect--a cognitive disorder of spatial... |
SourceID | hal proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 25 |
SubjectTerms | Adaptation, Physiological Adolescent Adult Analysis of Variance Anatomical correlates of behavior Behavioral psychophysiology Biological and medical sciences Bisection Female Functional Laterality - physiology Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Lenses Life Sciences Male Middle Aged Neurons and Cognition Neuropsychological Tests Perceptual Disorders - diagnosis Perceptual Disorders - physiopathology Perceptual Disorders - rehabilitation Photic Stimulation Plasticity Prism adaptation Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Psychomotor Performance - physiology Space Perception - physiology Space representation Spatial neglect Vision, Ocular - physiology Visual Fields Visuomotor |
Title | Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12427563 https://www.proquest.com/docview/72888820 https://hal.science/hal-02151001 |
Volume | 41 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1La9wwEB6yCZRCKenbTbMVpYf24MS2JFvubQkN20dyaQN7E7ItbQypsnR3C730t2dGtrPJIQTqkxEaWWhGms-jeQC8R4hheOVs3JRNFQvlXGwKJWIheNFIBOhNRfaOk9N8eia-zuRsC46GWBhyq-zP_u5MD6d133LYr-bhom0pxhdVY0n1vwnHSDmCnYyXOYr2zuTLt-npzaThakjGTASbQJ5ukND4Ick-hnFieZeKGp2Tr-SjhVni8rmu7sXdwDQoqONdeNwjSzbpJv8Etqx_Cg9O-rvzZzD70f4Ktbr8nK19i29kjmLezslyz1rPPOHXiyUjX_g5CwkSmWnMorut_8TaG97nDMEuC1GQf5_D2fHnn0fTuK-rENeiTFdxarkpnUt4lSurUleJBv_EqDSV4TatbOGcK6yqecoF8lJh90SapHZZYxRhthew7S-9fQXMyKJOeY0gQVHG8xLxmqyNairB8yKVTQRiWEpd90nHqfbFhd54lyEHNHFAJ5kOHNAygoNrskWXdeM-AjXwSd8SH42a4T7Sd8jX689Quu3p5LumtoCHsNufNILxLbZvZiUoSRzPI3g7yIHGDUq3Lsbby_VSF5nCJ0sieNmJx4YW4VEhc_76_ye_Bw-Dd2GwCb2B7dXvtd1HlLSqxjA6-JeO-71wBT21CLU |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1La9wwEB6SFNpCCH3XfSSi9NAenNiWZMu5hdCwbXdzaQJ7E7IlbQ2ps3R3C7n0t3dGtrPJIQTqkxEaWWg0ms-jeQB8RIhheOVdbEtbxUJ5H5tCiVgIXliJAN1WZO-YnOajc_FtKqcbcDzEwpBbZX_2d2d6OK37loN-NQ_mTUMxvqgaS6r_TThGyk14IFB8STr3_6Y3U4arIRUzdV-H8XRDhMZPSfY5jBLLuxTU5k_ylNyemwUunu-qXtwNS4N6OnkCOz2uZEfd1J_ChmufwcNJf3P-HKY_ml-hUlc7Y6u2wTcyRrHWzchuz5qWtYReLxaMPOFnLKRHZMaaeXdXf8iaG77nDKEuCzGQVy_g_OTL2fEo7qsqxLUo02WcOm5K7xNe5cqp1FfC4n8YFaYy3KWVK7z3hVM1T7lATirsnkiT1D6zRhFiewlb7WXrXgMzsqhTXiNEUJTvvES0JmujbCV4XqTSRiCGpdR1n3KcKl9c6LVvGXJAEwd0kunAAS0j2L8mm3c5N-4jUAOf9K3No1Ev3Ef6Afl6_RlKtj06GmtqC2gIu_1JI9i9xfb1rASliON5BHvDPtAonnTnYlp3uVroIlP4ZEkEr7rtsaZFcFTInL_5_8nvwaPR2WSsx19Pv7-Fx8HPMFiH3sHW8vfKvUe8tKx2gzz8A1kaCXk |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Simulating+unilateral+neglect+in+normals+using+prism+adaptation%3A+implications+for+theory&rft.jtitle=Neuropsychologia&rft.au=MICHEL%2C+Carine&rft.au=PISELLA%2C+Laure&rft.au=HALLIGAN%2C+Peter+W&rft.au=LUAUTE%2C+Jacques&rft.date=2003-01-01&rft.pub=Elsevier+Science&rft.issn=0028-3932&rft.eissn=1873-3514&rft.volume=41&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=25&rft.epage=39&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2FS0028-3932%2802%2900135-5&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=14024036 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon |