Knowing How You Know: Toddlers Reevaluate Words Learned From an Unreliable Speaker
There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their imp...
Saved in:
Published in | Open mind (Cambridge, Mass.) Vol. 5; pp. 1 - 19 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA
MIT Press
01.02.2021
MIT Press Journals, The The MIT Press |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward,
): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know. |
---|---|
AbstractList | There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one's knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one's knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know. There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know. There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, ): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know. There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010 ): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know. |
Author | Dautriche, Isabelle Smith, Kenny Goupil, Louise Rabagliati, Hugh |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Isabelle surname: Dautriche fullname: Dautriche, Isabelle email: isabelle.dautriche@gmail.com organization: Institute of Language, Communication and the Brain, Aix-Marseille University, CNRS, Aix-en-Provence, France – sequence: 2 givenname: Louise surname: Goupil fullname: Goupil, Louise organization: School of Psychology, University of East London, London, UK – sequence: 3 givenname: Kenny surname: Smith fullname: Smith, Kenny organization: University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK – sequence: 4 givenname: Hugh surname: Rabagliati fullname: Rabagliati, Hugh organization: University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK |
BackLink | https://hal.science/hal-03097987$$DView record in HAL |
BookMark | eNp1kl1rFDEUhgep2A975w8IeFPB1XxPIlQopXWLC0JtEa9CMslss84ka2Zmi_56M06R7VKvkpw87_uehHNY7IUYXFG8QvAdQhy_j-vWK60ghEQ8Kw4wLeEMS8H3tvb7xXHXrTKCZJbg8kWxTygVrJT0oLj-HOK9D0swj_fgexzAeP4AbqK1jUsduHZuo5tB9w58i8l2YOF0Cs6CyxRboAO4Dck1XpvGga9rp3-49LJ4Xuumc8cP61Fxe3lxcz6fLb58ujo_W8wqKpGYGUrrmtSCIUNLIUvEEWGCaWa0pIIQzGzNicCWCc10hgQpGTYGWWGssIgcFVeTr416pdbJtzr9UlF79bcQ01Lp1PuqcQpz4jSimGNUUWeszFkECs0ZJ5IYk70-Tl7rwbTOVi70STePTB_fBH-nlnGjBEUYSZkN3kwGdzuy-dlCjTVIoCylKDdj4ycPYSn-HFzXq9Z3lWsaHVwcOoUZlxxDiWBGX--gqzikkL9VYSGzI5NEZOrtRFUpdl1y9b8OEFTjnKjtOck43sEr3-vex_Fpvvmf6HQStX6riRHZMEUopyVSGGKioFS4VL_9eld_8oT-yag_gn_hBg |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2021_104954 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13373 crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976221105208 crossref_primary_10_1177_17456916241262693 crossref_primary_10_1080_10489223_2022_2078211 crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_022_03730_7 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_13223 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2022_105517 crossref_primary_10_3389_fdpys_2025_1469550 |
Cites_doi | 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00726.x 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x 10.1214/aoms/1177729885 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01082.x 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012 10.1111/1467-8624.00334 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.05.005 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630 10.2307/1130499 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00002-7 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.347 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01015 10.1177/014272379801805302 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.04.001 10.1080/15475441.2017.1311260 10.1177/0956797611416998 10.1111/desc.12416 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x 10.1177/0963721415613962 10.1017/S0305000916000209 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.008 10.1006/jecp.1999.2555 10.1073/pnas.1803505115 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.001 10.1177/0963721419848672 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x 10.1111/desc.12592 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216 10.1098/rsos.191451 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00186-5 10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00728-7 10.2307/1131282 10.1111/infa.12277 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01629.x 10.1002/aur.1487 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x 10.1111/infa.12027 10.1111/desc.12904 10.1111/cdev.12707 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01543.x 10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.390 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.06.003 10.1037/a0035657 10.1073/pnas.1712966114 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.01.003 10.1017/S0305000900013817 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.245 10.1111/desc.12663 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_31 10.1037/a0019664 10.1348/026151008X310229 10.1075/swll.11.14per 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.002 10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.99 10.1016/0022-0965(91)90065-Z 10.1037/a0031615 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010 10.1017/S0305000900013842 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104701 10.1006/cogp.2001.0764 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02341.x 10.1177/0956797613478198 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2021. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Attribution 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2021. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. – notice: 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology. – notice: Attribution – notice: 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION ABUWG AFKRA AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PKEHL PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS 7X8 1XC VOOES 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.1162/opmi_a_00038 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China MEDLINE - Academic Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access) PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central China ProQuest Central ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic Publicly Available Content Database CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 2470-2986 |
EndPage | 19 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb PMC8412199 oai_HAL_hal_03097987v1 10_1162_opmi_a_00038 opmi_a_00038.pdf |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: ; grantid: ES/N017404/1 – fundername: ; grantid: ES/N005635/1 |
GroupedDBID | AAFWJ ADBBV AFPKN AKSEZ ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BCNDV EBS GROUPED_DOAJ JMNJE MCG M~E OK1 RMI RPM AAYXX AFKRA BENPR CCPQU CITATION EJD MINIK PGMZT PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY ABUWG AZQEC DWQXO PKEHL PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS 7X8 1XC VOOES 5PM PUEGO |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c4918-b44ff3f851b478971613585a5ba9483325df6382d58a5a1b483752bb1d8bd8d13 |
IEDL.DBID | DOA |
ISSN | 2470-2986 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:31:40 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 17:42:36 EDT 2025 Fri May 09 12:19:27 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 22:06:12 EDT 2025 Mon Jun 30 05:15:07 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:56:09 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:29:16 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 29 05:14:18 EDT 2023 Thu Sep 28 12:19:33 EDT 2023 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Language | English |
License | Attribution: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4918-b44ff3f851b478971613585a5ba9483325df6382d58a5a1b483752bb1d8bd8d13 |
Notes | 2021 ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Competing Interests: The authors declare have declared that no competing interests exist. |
ORCID | 0000-0002-2297-985X 0000-0003-4342-9408 |
OpenAccessLink | https://doaj.org/article/263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb |
PMID | 34485794 |
PQID | 2890975938 |
PQPubID | 6535870 |
PageCount | 19 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_journals_2890975938 doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb crossref_citationtrail_10_1162_opmi_a_00038 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_8412199 crossref_primary_10_1162_opmi_a_00038 hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_03097987v1 mit_journals_10_1162_opmi_a_00038 proquest_miscellaneous_2569620910 mit_journals_opmiv5_346471_2023_09_27_zip_opmi_a_00038 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20210201 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2021-02-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 2 year: 2021 text: 20210201 day: 1 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA – name: Cambridge – name: One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209USAjournals-info@mit.edu |
PublicationTitle | Open mind (Cambridge, Mass.) |
PublicationYear | 2021 |
Publisher | MIT Press MIT Press Journals, The The MIT Press |
Publisher_xml | – name: MIT Press – name: MIT Press Journals, The – name: The MIT Press |
References | bib72 Carey S. (bib8) 1978; 15 bib73 bib70 bib71 bib36 bib37 bib34 bib35 bib32 bib31 bib29 bib27 bib28 bib47 bib45 bib46 bib43 bib44 bib41 bib42 bib9 bib7 bib5 bib6 bib38 bib4 bib39 bib1 Hunter M. A. (bib33) 1988; 5 bib50 bib51 Smith L. B. (bib63) 2000 bib14 bib58 bib15 bib59 bib12 bib56 bib13 bib57 bib10 bib54 bib11 bib55 bib52 bib53 bib49 Markman E. M. (bib48) 1989 bib61 bib62 bib60 bib25 bib69 Bates D. (bib2) 2007 bib26 Harris P. (bib30) 2015 bib23 bib67 bib24 bib68 bib21 bib65 bib22 Kuzyk O. (bib40) 2019; 23 bib66 bib20 bib64 bib18 bib19 bib16 bib17 Begus K. (bib3) 2018 |
References_xml | – ident: bib9 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00726.x – ident: bib7 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x – ident: bib27 doi: 10.1214/aoms/1177729885 – ident: bib42 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01082.x – ident: bib49 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012 – ident: bib58 doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00334 – ident: bib18 doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.05.005 – ident: bib19 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630 – ident: bib24 doi: 10.2307/1130499 – ident: bib37 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00002-7 – ident: bib11 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.347 – ident: bib65 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01015 – ident: bib26 doi: 10.1177/014272379801805302 – ident: bib16 doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.04.001 – ident: bib21 doi: 10.1080/15475441.2017.1311260 – ident: bib20 doi: 10.1177/0956797611416998 – start-page: 169 volume-title: Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition year: 2000 ident: bib63 – ident: bib31 doi: 10.1111/desc.12416 – ident: bib47 doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 – ident: bib5 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008 – ident: bib36 doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x – ident: bib56 doi: 10.1177/0963721415613962 – ident: bib22 doi: 10.1017/S0305000916000209 – ident: bib38 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x – ident: bib1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.008 – ident: bib32 doi: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2555 – ident: bib35 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1803505115 – ident: bib66 doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.001 – ident: bib25 doi: 10.1177/0963721419848672 – ident: bib52 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x – ident: bib13 doi: 10.1111/desc.12592 – ident: bib54 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216 – ident: bib59 doi: 10.1098/rsos.191451 – ident: bib29 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00186-5 – ident: bib61 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00728-7 – ident: bib67 doi: 10.2307/1131282 – ident: bib14 doi: 10.1111/infa.12277 – volume-title: Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction year: 1989 ident: bib48 – ident: bib68 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01629.x – ident: bib28 doi: 10.1002/aur.1487 – ident: bib64 doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x – ident: bib6 doi: 10.1111/infa.12027 – volume: 23 start-page: Article e12904 issue: 3 year: 2019 ident: bib40 publication-title: Developmental Science doi: 10.1111/desc.12904 – ident: bib44 doi: 10.1111/cdev.12707 – ident: bib71 doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01543.x – ident: bib53 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.390 – ident: bib17 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.06.003 – ident: bib15 doi: 10.1037/a0035657 – ident: bib4 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712966114 – ident: bib60 doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.01.003 – ident: bib51 doi: 10.1017/S0305000900013817 – ident: bib72 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.245 – ident: bib46 doi: 10.1111/desc.12663 – ident: bib34 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x – ident: bib57 doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_31 – volume: 15 start-page: 17 year: 1978 ident: bib8 publication-title: Papers and Reports on Child Language Development – ident: bib39 doi: 10.1037/a0019664 – volume-title: Ime4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes year: 2007 ident: bib2 – ident: bib12 doi: 10.1348/026151008X310229 – ident: bib55 doi: 10.1075/swll.11.14per – volume: 5 start-page: 69 year: 1988 ident: bib33 publication-title: Advances in Infancy Research – ident: bib50 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.002 – volume-title: Trusting what youre told: How children learn from others year: 2015 ident: bib30 – ident: bib23 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.99 – ident: bib41 doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(91)90065-Z – ident: bib43 doi: 10.1037/a0031615 – ident: bib62 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010 – ident: bib10 doi: 10.1017/S0305000900013842 – volume-title: Curious learners: How infants’ motivation to learn shapes and is shaped by infants’ interactions with the social world year: 2018 ident: bib3 – ident: bib45 doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104701 – ident: bib69 doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0764 – ident: bib73 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02341.x – ident: bib70 doi: 10.1177/0956797613478198 |
SSID | ssj0001916227 |
Score | 2.2208848 |
Snippet | There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition.... There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one's knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition.... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral hal proquest crossref mit |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Enrichment Source Index Database Publisher |
StartPage | 1 |
SubjectTerms | Cognitive science Labeling Linguistics metacognition Psychology selective trust source monitoring word learning |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: ProQuest Central dbid: BENPR link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELbo9tIL4ikCBRkEJxRRvxKbC2pRVyuEKrR0RW-WHTs0opss2W0R_Hpm0uxug1SOiScve8bzzDeEvBa5L30ey5Q5F1OZBRApp8FrLUOhYzAY7Mdqi5NsMpOfztRZH3Bb9mWV6z2x26hDU2CM_B0mxEyujNAfFj9T7BqF2dW-hcYO2YUtWOsR2T06Pvky3UZZwPrhPF9XvGccXNF5ZZ3tUmIDXdRB9oOGOceCyJ15tRrYnMOKyRsqaHyP3O1tR3p4vdj3yZ1YPyB7my3s90MyxS7WoIzopPlFQZApHr-npw1GIdolncYe3TvSb-B1LmkHrxoDHbfNnLqazoCbLir8nYp-XUT3I7aPyGx8fPpxkvZdE9JCGqZTL2VZihIsKS9zjQhRTIBP4JR3RmohuAolCB0PSjvlgAhcVMW9Z0H7oAMTj8mobur4hNCYg_FkVBGkKSWXznOnWGSFYFkoYywT8nY9f7boIcWxs8WF7VyLjNubs52QNxvqxTWUxi10R7gUGxoEwO5ONO1328uT5ZmIjiGcPitk9MBU4GwfaAfmqTDC-4S8goUc3GNy-NniOUwq5UbnVywhL2GdbS-1y1veJhvQ4NiVskJmoNYtNp-3B8by3P6pFv9cuL9mn-3VWy6GZ2-GQaIxTePq2FwCjcpMxtGOS0g-YLvB5wxH6uq8wwbXkoEOMk____BnZI9jbU5Xfb5PRqv2Mj4H42rlX_QS9BdXVSTq priority: 102 providerName: ProQuest |
Title | Knowing How You Know: Toddlers Reevaluate Words Learned From an Unreliable Speaker |
URI | https://direct.mit.edu/opmi/article/doi/10.1162/opmi_a_00038 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2890975938 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2569620910 https://hal.science/hal-03097987 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC8412199 https://doaj.org/article/263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb |
Volume | 5 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3db9MwELdgvOwF8SkCo_IQPKGI-iuxedvQqgqhCZVV7M2yY0eLtiZV2w1tfz13aVoapIkXHmOfI8e-83348jtC3ovclz6PZcqci6nMAoiU0-C1lqHQMRgM9mO2xWk2nsqv5-p8p9QX5oSt4YHXC_eJZyI6hrjprJDRw2jwqobagR0ijPAeT1_QeTvOVBtdAauH83yT6Z5xcEFnlXW2vQrr6aAWqh80ywUmQj6cVauerdnPlNxRPaMn5HFnM9Kj9Vyfkgexfkb2t0fX7XMywerVoITouPlFQYApPn-mZw1GHxZLOokdqnekP8HbXNIWVjUGOlo0M-pqOgUuuqrwNyr6Yx7dZVy8INPRydmXcdpVS0gLaZhOvZRlKUqwoLzMNSJDMQG-gFPeGamF4CqUIGw8KO2UAyJwTRX3ngXtgw5MvCR7dVPHV4TGHIwmo4ogTSm5dJ47xSIrBMtCGWOZkI-b9bNFByWOFS2ubOtSZNzurnZCPmyp52sIjXvojnErtjQIfN02ADvYjh3sv9ghIe9gI3vvGB99s9iGl0m50fkNS8gh7LPtpHV5z2yyHg323SgrZAbq3GLReTs0luf2rpr_NfBgwz5_RuN1rsmVwe7DbTdIMl7PuDo210CjMpMhLw8TkvfYrvc5_Z66umgxwbVkoHvM6_-xhm_IPsfMnTY3_YDsrRbX8S2YXis_II-OT06_TwattA3a2Nhv_q8tag |
linkProvider | Directory of Open Access Journals |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9NAEF616YFeEE8RKLAgekIR2YftXSSEWmiU0hChkIjell17TSMaOyRpq_Kj-I3MOHZSI5Vbj_aOX7vz-GZnPEPIKxG51EU-bTFrfUuGCYiUVeC1pkmsfKJxsx-zLfphdyQ_HQfHG-RP9S8MplVWOrFQ1Eke4x75GwyI6SjQQr2f_mph1yiMrlYtNJZsceQvL8Blm787_Ajru8t552D4odsquwq0YqmZajkp01SkgDScjBRWUGICMLMNnNVSCcGDJAWm5EmgbGCBCFy4gDvHEuUSlTAB990kW1KEbd4gW_sH_S-D9a4OoC3OoyrDPuTg-k7GxpoiBFezfUWLALBoJ5iAuTkZL2oYt56hecXkde6Q2yVWpXtL5rpLNnx2j2yvVOblfTLArtlg_Gg3v6CgOCgev6XDHHc9ZnM68GU1cU-_4ZTRopyrT2hnlk-ozegIuPd0jL9v0a9Tb3_62QMyupH5fEgaWZ75R4T6CMCaDuJE6lRyaR23AfMsFixMUu_TJnldzZ-JyxLm2Enj1BSuTMjN1dlukt0V9XRZuuMaun1cihUNFtwuTuSzH6aUX8ND4S3D8v0slt4BE4Nz31YW4LDQwrkmeQkLWbtHd69n8BwGsSKtonPWJC9gnU2pJebXvE1Yo8Gx88AIGQKMMNjs3rS14ZH5PZ7-c-FOxT7rq9dSA89eDYMGwbCQzXx-BjRBqEOOuLFJohrb1T6nPpKNT4pa5EoysHn68f8f_pzc6g4_90zvsH_0hGxzzAsqMt93SGMxO_NPAdgt3LNSmij5ftMC_Be271-1 |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Knowing+How+You+Know%3A+Toddlers+Reevaluate+Words+Learned+From+an+Unreliable+Speaker&rft.jtitle=Open+mind+%28Cambridge%2C+Mass.%29&rft.au=Isabelle+Dautriche&rft.au=Louise+Goupil&rft.au=Kenny+Smith&rft.au=Hugh+Rabagliati&rft.date=2021-02-01&rft.pub=The+MIT+Press&rft.eissn=2470-2986&rft.volume=5&rft_id=info:doi/10.1162%2Fopmi_a_00038&rft.externalDBID=DOA&rft.externalDocID=oai_doaj_org_article_263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2470-2986&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2470-2986&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2470-2986&client=summon |