Knowing How You Know: Toddlers Reevaluate Words Learned From an Unreliable Speaker

There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their imp...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inOpen mind (Cambridge, Mass.) Vol. 5; pp. 1 - 19
Main Authors Dautriche, Isabelle, Goupil, Louise, Smith, Kenny, Rabagliati, Hugh
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA MIT Press 01.02.2021
MIT Press Journals, The
The MIT Press
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, ): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.
AbstractList There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one's knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one's knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.
There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.
There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, ): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.
There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition. In two experiments, we tested whether toddlers (mean age 30 months) can monitor the source of their lexical knowledge and reevaluate their implicit belief about a word mapping when this source is proven to be unreliable. Experiment 1 replicated previous research (Koenig & Woodward, 2010 ): children displayed better performance in a word learning test when they learned words from a speaker who has previously revealed themself as reliable (correctly labeling familiar objects) as opposed to an unreliable labeler (incorrectly labeling familiar objects). Experiment 2 then provided the critical test for source monitoring: children first learned novel words from a speaker before watching that speaker labeling familiar objects correctly or incorrectly. Children who were exposed to the reliable speaker were significantly more likely to endorse the word mappings taught by the speaker than children who were exposed to a speaker who they later discovered was an unreliable labeler. Thus, young children can reevaluate recently learned word mappings upon discovering that the source of their knowledge is unreliable. This suggests that children can monitor the source of their knowledge in order to decide whether that knowledge is justified, even at an age where they are not credited with the ability to verbally report how they have come to know what they know.
Author Dautriche, Isabelle
Smith, Kenny
Goupil, Louise
Rabagliati, Hugh
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Isabelle
  surname: Dautriche
  fullname: Dautriche, Isabelle
  email: isabelle.dautriche@gmail.com
  organization: Institute of Language, Communication and the Brain, Aix-Marseille University, CNRS, Aix-en-Provence, France
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Louise
  surname: Goupil
  fullname: Goupil, Louise
  organization: School of Psychology, University of East London, London, UK
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Kenny
  surname: Smith
  fullname: Smith, Kenny
  organization: University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Hugh
  surname: Rabagliati
  fullname: Rabagliati, Hugh
  organization: University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
BackLink https://hal.science/hal-03097987$$DView record in HAL
BookMark eNp1kl1rFDEUhgep2A975w8IeFPB1XxPIlQopXWLC0JtEa9CMslss84ka2Zmi_56M06R7VKvkpw87_uehHNY7IUYXFG8QvAdQhy_j-vWK60ghEQ8Kw4wLeEMS8H3tvb7xXHXrTKCZJbg8kWxTygVrJT0oLj-HOK9D0swj_fgexzAeP4AbqK1jUsduHZuo5tB9w58i8l2YOF0Cs6CyxRboAO4Dck1XpvGga9rp3-49LJ4Xuumc8cP61Fxe3lxcz6fLb58ujo_W8wqKpGYGUrrmtSCIUNLIUvEEWGCaWa0pIIQzGzNicCWCc10hgQpGTYGWWGssIgcFVeTr416pdbJtzr9UlF79bcQ01Lp1PuqcQpz4jSimGNUUWeszFkECs0ZJ5IYk70-Tl7rwbTOVi70STePTB_fBH-nlnGjBEUYSZkN3kwGdzuy-dlCjTVIoCylKDdj4ycPYSn-HFzXq9Z3lWsaHVwcOoUZlxxDiWBGX--gqzikkL9VYSGzI5NEZOrtRFUpdl1y9b8OEFTjnKjtOck43sEr3-vex_Fpvvmf6HQStX6riRHZMEUopyVSGGKioFS4VL_9eld_8oT-yag_gn_hBg
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2021_104954
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13373
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976221105208
crossref_primary_10_1177_17456916241262693
crossref_primary_10_1080_10489223_2022_2078211
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_022_03730_7
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_13223
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2022_105517
crossref_primary_10_3389_fdpys_2025_1469550
Cites_doi 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00726.x
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x
10.1214/aoms/1177729885
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01082.x
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012
10.1111/1467-8624.00334
10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.05.005
10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630
10.2307/1130499
10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00002-7
10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.347
10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01015
10.1177/014272379801805302
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.04.001
10.1080/15475441.2017.1311260
10.1177/0956797611416998
10.1111/desc.12416
10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024
10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008
10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x
10.1177/0963721415613962
10.1017/S0305000916000209
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.008
10.1006/jecp.1999.2555
10.1073/pnas.1803505115
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.001
10.1177/0963721419848672
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x
10.1111/desc.12592
10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216
10.1098/rsos.191451
10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00186-5
10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00728-7
10.2307/1131282
10.1111/infa.12277
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01629.x
10.1002/aur.1487
10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x
10.1111/infa.12027
10.1111/desc.12904
10.1111/cdev.12707
10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01543.x
10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.390
10.1016/j.cognition.2015.06.003
10.1037/a0035657
10.1073/pnas.1712966114
10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.01.003
10.1017/S0305000900013817
10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.245
10.1111/desc.12663
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x
10.1207/s15516709cog0000_31
10.1037/a0019664
10.1348/026151008X310229
10.1075/swll.11.14per
10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.002
10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.99
10.1016/0022-0965(91)90065-Z
10.1037/a0031615
10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010
10.1017/S0305000900013842
10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104701
10.1006/cogp.2001.0764
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02341.x
10.1177/0956797613478198
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2021. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Attribution
2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Copyright_xml – notice: 2021. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
– notice: 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
– notice: Attribution
– notice: 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology 2020 Massachusetts Institute of Technology
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
ABUWG
AFKRA
AZQEC
BENPR
CCPQU
DWQXO
PHGZM
PHGZT
PIMPY
PKEHL
PQEST
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
7X8
1XC
VOOES
5PM
DOA
DOI 10.1162/opmi_a_00038
DatabaseName CrossRef
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest Central
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Central Korea
ProQuest Central Premium
ProQuest One Academic
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central China
MEDLINE - Academic
Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)
Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access)
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Central China
ProQuest Central
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central Korea
ProQuest Central (New)
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic (New)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic

Publicly Available Content Database

CrossRef

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: DOA
  name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 2
  dbid: BENPR
  name: ProQuest Central
  url: https://www.proquest.com/central
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 2470-2986
EndPage 19
ExternalDocumentID oai_doaj_org_article_263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb
PMC8412199
oai_HAL_hal_03097987v1
10_1162_opmi_a_00038
opmi_a_00038.pdf
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: ;
  grantid: ES/N017404/1
– fundername: ;
  grantid: ES/N005635/1
GroupedDBID AAFWJ
ADBBV
AFPKN
AKSEZ
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
BCNDV
EBS
GROUPED_DOAJ
JMNJE
MCG
M~E
OK1
RMI
RPM
AAYXX
AFKRA
BENPR
CCPQU
CITATION
EJD
MINIK
PGMZT
PHGZM
PHGZT
PIMPY
ABUWG
AZQEC
DWQXO
PKEHL
PQEST
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
7X8
1XC
VOOES
5PM
PUEGO
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c4918-b44ff3f851b478971613585a5ba9483325df6382d58a5a1b483752bb1d8bd8d13
IEDL.DBID DOA
ISSN 2470-2986
IngestDate Wed Aug 27 01:31:40 EDT 2025
Thu Aug 21 17:42:36 EDT 2025
Fri May 09 12:19:27 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 22:06:12 EDT 2025
Mon Jun 30 05:15:07 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:56:09 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 03:29:16 EDT 2025
Fri Sep 29 05:14:18 EDT 2023
Thu Sep 28 12:19:33 EDT 2023
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Language English
License Attribution: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For a full description of the license, please visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4918-b44ff3f851b478971613585a5ba9483325df6382d58a5a1b483752bb1d8bd8d13
Notes 2021
ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
Competing Interests: The authors declare have declared that no competing interests exist.
ORCID 0000-0002-2297-985X
0000-0003-4342-9408
OpenAccessLink https://doaj.org/article/263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb
PMID 34485794
PQID 2890975938
PQPubID 6535870
PageCount 19
ParticipantIDs proquest_journals_2890975938
doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb
crossref_citationtrail_10_1162_opmi_a_00038
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_8412199
crossref_primary_10_1162_opmi_a_00038
hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_03097987v1
mit_journals_10_1162_opmi_a_00038
proquest_miscellaneous_2569620910
mit_journals_opmiv5_346471_2023_09_27_zip_opmi_a_00038
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 20210201
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2021-02-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 2
  year: 2021
  text: 20210201
  day: 1
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA
PublicationPlace_xml – name: One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209, USA
– name: Cambridge
– name: One Rogers Street, Cambridge, MA 02142-1209USAjournals-info@mit.edu
PublicationTitle Open mind (Cambridge, Mass.)
PublicationYear 2021
Publisher MIT Press
MIT Press Journals, The
The MIT Press
Publisher_xml – name: MIT Press
– name: MIT Press Journals, The
– name: The MIT Press
References bib72
Carey S. (bib8) 1978; 15
bib73
bib70
bib71
bib36
bib37
bib34
bib35
bib32
bib31
bib29
bib27
bib28
bib47
bib45
bib46
bib43
bib44
bib41
bib42
bib9
bib7
bib5
bib6
bib38
bib4
bib39
bib1
Hunter M. A. (bib33) 1988; 5
bib50
bib51
Smith L. B. (bib63) 2000
bib14
bib58
bib15
bib59
bib12
bib56
bib13
bib57
bib10
bib54
bib11
bib55
bib52
bib53
bib49
Markman E. M. (bib48) 1989
bib61
bib62
bib60
bib25
bib69
Bates D. (bib2) 2007
bib26
Harris P. (bib30) 2015
bib23
bib67
bib24
bib68
bib21
bib65
bib22
Kuzyk O. (bib40) 2019; 23
bib66
bib20
bib64
bib18
bib19
bib16
bib17
Begus K. (bib3) 2018
References_xml – ident: bib9
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00726.x
– ident: bib7
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x
– ident: bib27
  doi: 10.1214/aoms/1177729885
– ident: bib42
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01082.x
– ident: bib49
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012
– ident: bib58
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00334
– ident: bib18
  doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.05.005
– ident: bib19
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630
– ident: bib24
  doi: 10.2307/1130499
– ident: bib37
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00002-7
– ident: bib11
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.347
– ident: bib65
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01015
– ident: bib26
  doi: 10.1177/014272379801805302
– ident: bib16
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2018.04.001
– ident: bib21
  doi: 10.1080/15475441.2017.1311260
– ident: bib20
  doi: 10.1177/0956797611416998
– start-page: 169
  volume-title: Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition
  year: 2000
  ident: bib63
– ident: bib31
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12416
– ident: bib47
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024
– ident: bib5
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008
– ident: bib36
  doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x
– ident: bib56
  doi: 10.1177/0963721415613962
– ident: bib22
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000916000209
– ident: bib38
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
– ident: bib1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.008
– ident: bib32
  doi: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2555
– ident: bib35
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1803505115
– ident: bib66
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.001
– ident: bib25
  doi: 10.1177/0963721419848672
– ident: bib52
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x
– ident: bib13
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12592
– ident: bib54
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216
– ident: bib59
  doi: 10.1098/rsos.191451
– ident: bib29
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00186-5
– ident: bib61
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00728-7
– ident: bib67
  doi: 10.2307/1131282
– ident: bib14
  doi: 10.1111/infa.12277
– volume-title: Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction
  year: 1989
  ident: bib48
– ident: bib68
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01629.x
– ident: bib28
  doi: 10.1002/aur.1487
– ident: bib64
  doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x
– ident: bib6
  doi: 10.1111/infa.12027
– volume: 23
  start-page: Article e12904
  issue: 3
  year: 2019
  ident: bib40
  publication-title: Developmental Science
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12904
– ident: bib44
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.12707
– ident: bib71
  doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01543.x
– ident: bib53
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.390
– ident: bib17
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.06.003
– ident: bib15
  doi: 10.1037/a0035657
– ident: bib4
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712966114
– ident: bib60
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.01.003
– ident: bib51
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900013817
– ident: bib72
  doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.245
– ident: bib46
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12663
– ident: bib34
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x
– ident: bib57
  doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_31
– volume: 15
  start-page: 17
  year: 1978
  ident: bib8
  publication-title: Papers and Reports on Child Language Development
– ident: bib39
  doi: 10.1037/a0019664
– volume-title: Ime4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes
  year: 2007
  ident: bib2
– ident: bib12
  doi: 10.1348/026151008X310229
– ident: bib55
  doi: 10.1075/swll.11.14per
– volume: 5
  start-page: 69
  year: 1988
  ident: bib33
  publication-title: Advances in Infancy Research
– ident: bib50
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.002
– volume-title: Trusting what youre told: How children learn from others
  year: 2015
  ident: bib30
– ident: bib23
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.99
– ident: bib41
  doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(91)90065-Z
– ident: bib43
  doi: 10.1037/a0031615
– ident: bib62
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010
– ident: bib10
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000900013842
– volume-title: Curious learners: How infants’ motivation to learn shapes and is shaped by infants’ interactions with the social world
  year: 2018
  ident: bib3
– ident: bib45
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104701
– ident: bib69
  doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0764
– ident: bib73
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02341.x
– ident: bib70
  doi: 10.1177/0956797613478198
SSID ssj0001916227
Score 2.2208848
Snippet There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one’s knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition....
There has been little investigation of the way source monitoring, the ability to track the source of one's knowledge, may be involved in lexical acquisition....
SourceID doaj
pubmedcentral
hal
proquest
crossref
mit
SourceType Open Website
Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Enrichment Source
Index Database
Publisher
StartPage 1
SubjectTerms Cognitive science
Labeling
Linguistics
metacognition
Psychology
selective trust
source monitoring
word learning
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: ProQuest Central
  dbid: BENPR
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Lb9QwELbo9tIL4ikCBRkEJxRRvxKbC2pRVyuEKrR0RW-WHTs0opss2W0R_Hpm0uxug1SOiScve8bzzDeEvBa5L30ey5Q5F1OZBRApp8FrLUOhYzAY7Mdqi5NsMpOfztRZH3Bb9mWV6z2x26hDU2CM_B0mxEyujNAfFj9T7BqF2dW-hcYO2YUtWOsR2T06Pvky3UZZwPrhPF9XvGccXNF5ZZ3tUmIDXdRB9oOGOceCyJ15tRrYnMOKyRsqaHyP3O1tR3p4vdj3yZ1YPyB7my3s90MyxS7WoIzopPlFQZApHr-npw1GIdolncYe3TvSb-B1LmkHrxoDHbfNnLqazoCbLir8nYp-XUT3I7aPyGx8fPpxkvZdE9JCGqZTL2VZihIsKS9zjQhRTIBP4JR3RmohuAolCB0PSjvlgAhcVMW9Z0H7oAMTj8mobur4hNCYg_FkVBGkKSWXznOnWGSFYFkoYywT8nY9f7boIcWxs8WF7VyLjNubs52QNxvqxTWUxi10R7gUGxoEwO5ONO1328uT5ZmIjiGcPitk9MBU4GwfaAfmqTDC-4S8goUc3GNy-NniOUwq5UbnVywhL2GdbS-1y1veJhvQ4NiVskJmoNYtNp-3B8by3P6pFv9cuL9mn-3VWy6GZ2-GQaIxTePq2FwCjcpMxtGOS0g-YLvB5wxH6uq8wwbXkoEOMk____BnZI9jbU5Xfb5PRqv2Mj4H42rlX_QS9BdXVSTq
  priority: 102
  providerName: ProQuest
Title Knowing How You Know: Toddlers Reevaluate Words Learned From an Unreliable Speaker
URI https://direct.mit.edu/opmi/article/doi/10.1162/opmi_a_00038
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2890975938
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2569620910
https://hal.science/hal-03097987
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC8412199
https://doaj.org/article/263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb
Volume 5
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3db9MwELdgvOwF8SkCo_IQPKGI-iuxedvQqgqhCZVV7M2yY0eLtiZV2w1tfz13aVoapIkXHmOfI8e-83348jtC3ovclz6PZcqci6nMAoiU0-C1lqHQMRgM9mO2xWk2nsqv5-p8p9QX5oSt4YHXC_eJZyI6hrjprJDRw2jwqobagR0ijPAeT1_QeTvOVBtdAauH83yT6Z5xcEFnlXW2vQrr6aAWqh80ywUmQj6cVauerdnPlNxRPaMn5HFnM9Kj9Vyfkgexfkb2t0fX7XMywerVoITouPlFQYApPn-mZw1GHxZLOokdqnekP8HbXNIWVjUGOlo0M-pqOgUuuqrwNyr6Yx7dZVy8INPRydmXcdpVS0gLaZhOvZRlKUqwoLzMNSJDMQG-gFPeGamF4CqUIGw8KO2UAyJwTRX3ngXtgw5MvCR7dVPHV4TGHIwmo4ogTSm5dJ47xSIrBMtCGWOZkI-b9bNFByWOFS2ubOtSZNzurnZCPmyp52sIjXvojnErtjQIfN02ADvYjh3sv9ghIe9gI3vvGB99s9iGl0m50fkNS8gh7LPtpHV5z2yyHg323SgrZAbq3GLReTs0luf2rpr_NfBgwz5_RuN1rsmVwe7DbTdIMl7PuDo210CjMpMhLw8TkvfYrvc5_Z66umgxwbVkoHvM6_-xhm_IPsfMnTY3_YDsrRbX8S2YXis_II-OT06_TwattA3a2Nhv_q8tag
linkProvider Directory of Open Access Journals
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9NAEF616YFeEE8RKLAgekIR2YftXSSEWmiU0hChkIjell17TSMaOyRpq_Kj-I3MOHZSI5Vbj_aOX7vz-GZnPEPIKxG51EU-bTFrfUuGCYiUVeC1pkmsfKJxsx-zLfphdyQ_HQfHG-RP9S8MplVWOrFQ1Eke4x75GwyI6SjQQr2f_mph1yiMrlYtNJZsceQvL8Blm787_Ajru8t552D4odsquwq0YqmZajkp01SkgDScjBRWUGICMLMNnNVSCcGDJAWm5EmgbGCBCFy4gDvHEuUSlTAB990kW1KEbd4gW_sH_S-D9a4OoC3OoyrDPuTg-k7GxpoiBFezfUWLALBoJ5iAuTkZL2oYt56hecXkde6Q2yVWpXtL5rpLNnx2j2yvVOblfTLArtlg_Gg3v6CgOCgev6XDHHc9ZnM68GU1cU-_4ZTRopyrT2hnlk-ozegIuPd0jL9v0a9Tb3_62QMyupH5fEgaWZ75R4T6CMCaDuJE6lRyaR23AfMsFixMUu_TJnldzZ-JyxLm2Enj1BSuTMjN1dlukt0V9XRZuuMaun1cihUNFtwuTuSzH6aUX8ND4S3D8v0slt4BE4Nz31YW4LDQwrkmeQkLWbtHd69n8BwGsSKtonPWJC9gnU2pJebXvE1Yo8Gx88AIGQKMMNjs3rS14ZH5PZ7-c-FOxT7rq9dSA89eDYMGwbCQzXx-BjRBqEOOuLFJohrb1T6nPpKNT4pa5EoysHn68f8f_pzc6g4_90zvsH_0hGxzzAsqMt93SGMxO_NPAdgt3LNSmij5ftMC_Be271-1
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Knowing+How+You+Know%3A+Toddlers+Reevaluate+Words+Learned+From+an+Unreliable+Speaker&rft.jtitle=Open+mind+%28Cambridge%2C+Mass.%29&rft.au=Isabelle+Dautriche&rft.au=Louise+Goupil&rft.au=Kenny+Smith&rft.au=Hugh+Rabagliati&rft.date=2021-02-01&rft.pub=The+MIT+Press&rft.eissn=2470-2986&rft.volume=5&rft_id=info:doi/10.1162%2Fopmi_a_00038&rft.externalDBID=DOA&rft.externalDocID=oai_doaj_org_article_263ea142621c4ebd9613308a656393bb
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2470-2986&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2470-2986&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2470-2986&client=summon