Socioeconomic status and functional brain development - associations in early infancy

Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6–9‐month‐olds from areas of East London with h...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inDevelopmental science Vol. 16; no. 5; pp. 676 - 687
Main Authors Tomalski, Przemyslaw, Moore, Derek G., Ribeiro, Helena, Axelsson, Emma L., Murphy, Elizabeth, Karmiloff-Smith, Annette, Johnson, Mark H., Kushnerenko, Elena
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published England Blackwell Publishing Ltd 01.09.2013
Wiley-Blackwell
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6–9‐month‐olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between‐subject comparisons of infants from low‐ and high‐income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low‐income homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region‐selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low‐income families. Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6–9‐months‐olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between‐subject comparisons of infants from low‐ and high‐income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low‐income homes. Our result show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties.
AbstractList Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6-9-month-olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between-subject comparisons of infants from low- and high-income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low-income homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region-selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low-income families.Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6-9-month-olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between-subject comparisons of infants from low- and high-income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low-income homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region-selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low-income families.
Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6-9-month-olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between-subject comparisons of infants from low- and high-income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low-income homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region-selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low-income families. Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6-9-months-olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between-subject comparisons of infants from low- and high-income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low-income homes. Our result show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties.
Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6-9-month-olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between-subject comparisons of infants from low- and high-income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low-income homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region-selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low-income families. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6–9‐month‐olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between‐subject comparisons of infants from low‐ and high‐income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low‐income homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region‐selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low‐income families.
Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6–9‐month‐olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between‐subject comparisons of infants from low‐ and high‐income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low‐income homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region‐selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low‐income families. Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6–9‐months‐olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between‐subject comparisons of infants from low‐ and high‐income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low‐income homes. Our result show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early risk for language and attention difficulties.
Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown. In this study we measured resting baseline EEG activity in the gamma frequency range in awake 6-9-month-olds from areas of East London with high socioeconomic deprivation. Between-subjects comparisons of infants from low- and high-income families revealed significantly lower frontal gamma power in infants from low-income homes homes. Similar power differences were found when comparing infants according to maternal occupation, with lower occupational status groups yielding lower power. Infant sleep, maternal education, length of gestation, and birth weight, as well as smoke exposure and bilingualism, did not explain these differences. Our results show that the effects of socioeconomic disparities on brain activity can already be detected in early infancy, potentially pointing to very early for language and attention difficulties. This is the first study to reveal region-selective differences in functional brain development associated with early infancy in low-income families.
Author Johnson, Mark H.
Kushnerenko, Elena
Murphy, Elizabeth
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette
Axelsson, Emma L.
Moore, Derek G.
Tomalski, Przemyslaw
Ribeiro, Helena
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Przemyslaw
  surname: Tomalski
  fullname: Tomalski, Przemyslaw
  email: tomalski@mac.com
  organization: Institute for Research in Child Development, School of Psychology, University of East London, UK
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Derek G.
  surname: Moore
  fullname: Moore, Derek G.
  organization: Institute for Research in Child Development, School of Psychology, University of East London, UK
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Helena
  surname: Ribeiro
  fullname: Ribeiro, Helena
  organization: Institute for Research in Child Development, School of Psychology, University of East London, UK
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Emma L.
  surname: Axelsson
  fullname: Axelsson, Emma L.
  organization: Institute for Research in Child Development, School of Psychology, University of East London, UK
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Elizabeth
  surname: Murphy
  fullname: Murphy, Elizabeth
  organization: Institute for Research in Child Development, School of Psychology, University of East London, UK
– sequence: 6
  givenname: Annette
  surname: Karmiloff-Smith
  fullname: Karmiloff-Smith, Annette
  organization: Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
– sequence: 7
  givenname: Mark H.
  surname: Johnson
  fullname: Johnson, Mark H.
  organization: Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck University of London, London, UK
– sequence: 8
  givenname: Elena
  surname: Kushnerenko
  fullname: Kushnerenko, Elena
  organization: Institute for Research in Child Development, School of Psychology, University of East London, UK
BackLink http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1026003$$DView record in ERIC
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24033573$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
https://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-353626$$DView record from Swedish Publication Index
BookMark eNqNks1v1DAQxSNURD_gwh0UiQuqSPG3k2O13RZoBUilBXGxHHuC3CbxYieU_e9xmnYPFUL4YI_0fm808pvdbKv3PWTZc4wOcDpvLURzgAmS1aNsBzMhCylKuZVqKmghOf-2ne3GeIUQYhThJ9k2YYhSLulOdnHujfNgfO87Z_I46GGMue5t3oy9GZzvdZvXQbs-t_ALWr_qoB_yItcxJqeeiJgnFXRo16lodG_WT7PHjW4jPLt797KL4-WXxbvi7NPJ-8XhWWFYKauihsqmEbWWjTDccEabkoEoS1bXNbDKSgbW0NpWmBtUGmQs1LaRmBpKbLr2sjdz33gDq7FWq-A6HdbKa6eO3OWh8uGHGkdFORVEJPz1jK-C_zlCHFTnooG21T34MSrMaEUoZpL8D0qJKAniCX31AL3yY0j_NlGkREjwiiXq5R011h3Yzaj3USTgxQxAcGYjLz9gRARCk74_6yb4GAM0GwYjNe2BmvZA3e5BgtED2LjhNqwhZdn-3YJny41rYf2P5upoeb649xSzx8UBfm88OlwrIank6uvHE3XKqu_kVHxWl_QPOlrS0w
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12817
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ridd_2020_103638
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep04740
crossref_primary_10_1001_jamanetworkopen_2019_11027
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_22265
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2017_03_007
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21336
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2016_03_004
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_12547
crossref_primary_10_1111_jir_12516
crossref_primary_10_1177_1745691620931461
crossref_primary_10_3389_fped_2022_909069
crossref_primary_10_1002_ctm2_70032
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2016_11_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pediatrneurol_2014_11_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jpsychires_2021_06_002
crossref_primary_10_1515_revneuro_2023_0163
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41155_016_0016_x
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12494
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_22393
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2024_101468
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_22534
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2015_08_008
crossref_primary_10_1080_09297049_2021_1879766
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14733
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_00061
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhz066
crossref_primary_10_1002_jnr_25308
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41390_022_01933_z
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2115649119
crossref_primary_10_1038_pr_2015_202
crossref_primary_10_1075_dujal_19032_van
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2019_101344
crossref_primary_10_4236_ce_2020_113029
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_socscimed_2018_07_025
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2020_100780
crossref_primary_10_1177_0142723716639502
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2022_101806
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_22128
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_22249
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijpsycho_2018_11_004
crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12262
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0215304
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2015_12_028
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2022_101163
crossref_primary_10_1044_persp2_SIG12_23
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdep_12069
crossref_primary_10_1016_S2352_4642_17_30024_X
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsych_2022_11_023
crossref_primary_10_1186_s12916_022_02680_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2024_03_004
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2022_101125
crossref_primary_10_3390_ijerph18041592
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2024_105462
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2024_101445
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10212_022_00607_y
crossref_primary_10_1177_2372732217720699
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pse_2015_08_003
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21540
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_sleepe_2022_100047
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21303
crossref_primary_10_1542_peds_2015_3075
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_acap_2016_01_010
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci14060560
crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_1914
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_appdev_2015_12_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bpsc_2019_05_009
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nicl_2017_12_012
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nicl_2017_06_015
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_devpsych_121020_032354
crossref_primary_10_1177_2167702614521188
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12344
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2017_08_034
crossref_primary_10_1002_ajhb_23328
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_clinpsy_072319_024242
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13765
crossref_primary_10_3390_jintelligence5020018
crossref_primary_10_1128_msystems_01438_21
crossref_primary_10_1093_sleep_zsy024
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21413
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12976
crossref_primary_10_3390_ctn7030024
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2022_101265
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2021_101665
crossref_primary_10_1001_jamanetworkopen_2024_45863
crossref_primary_10_1080_87565641_2021_1981904
crossref_primary_10_5604_01_3001_0013_6363
crossref_primary_10_1089_brain_2023_0077
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12691
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2023_101260
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_85236_z
crossref_primary_10_1542_peds_2016_0137
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2019_04_006
crossref_primary_10_1590_2237_6089_2017_0008
crossref_primary_10_1080_09297049_2019_1648641
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2016_04_007
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10803_013_1966_9
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2021_101555
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsycho_2023_108683
crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_70005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dr_2015_07_001
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci14040392
crossref_primary_10_1044_2019_PERSP_19_00073
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dr_2018_02_003
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2025_101539
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2019_101384
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2021_100988
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21870
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2021_101126
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2021_104600
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdep_12305
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0267665
crossref_primary_10_1080_87565641_2022_2108425
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_39242_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_appdev_2021_101291
crossref_primary_10_1080_02109395_2014_922264
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_14784
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10803_021_05024_6
crossref_primary_10_1080_03004430_2016_1159559
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0254106
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2023_101842
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2019_103619
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2017_04_007
crossref_primary_10_1038_nn_3983
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2021_101041
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2023_102406
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2024_101404
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2021_105351
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jhealeco_2021_102431
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2021_118298
crossref_primary_10_1542_peds_2020_049702
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41398_022_02019_4
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2024_105653
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0305000920000501
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae223
crossref_primary_10_1111_mono_12354
crossref_primary_10_1186_s12939_021_01589_x
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12701
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_soc_060116_053339
crossref_primary_10_1002_dev_21867
crossref_primary_10_1177_0898264318783489
crossref_primary_10_1177_2470547018808295
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_isci_2023_106987
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2018_00814
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_64498_3
crossref_primary_10_1111_jcpp_13429
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pedn_2019_12_015
crossref_primary_10_1177_0016986217738566
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2021_100981
crossref_primary_10_1080_03004430_2019_1582034
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2018_08_001
Cites_doi 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00343-7
10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233
10.1016/j.clinph.2007.09.136
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02140.x
10.1162/jocn.2009.21101
10.1017/S0954579408000412
10.1037/a0016738
10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.655
10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.022
10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01471.x
10.1016/j.clinph.2011.01.007
10.1212/WNL.57.11.2045
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.003
10.1073/pnas.0507626102
10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.002
10.1038/nrn2897
10.1097/00001756-200108280-00021
10.1007/s00787-004-0367-1
10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00524-7
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00807.x
10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01657.x
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x
10.1542/peds.113.6.e570
10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00163-3
10.1017/S0954579401003133
10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.29
10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.293
10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70271-5
10.1038/35094565
10.1017/S0954579497001260
10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.029
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.021
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01507.x
10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.034
10.1023/A:1021399605526
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00993.x
10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.049
10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003
10.1162/0898929042304723
10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.072
10.1016/j.bbr.2011.01.048
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.007
10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00168-5
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.037
10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.04.004
10.1097/00004703-199602000-00002
10.1017/S1355617710001335
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Copyright Wiley Subscription Services, Inc. Sep 2013
Copyright_xml – notice: 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
– notice: 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
– notice: Copyright Wiley Subscription Services, Inc. Sep 2013
DBID BSCLL
AAYXX
CITATION
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7TK
7X8
ADTPV
AOWAS
DF2
DOI 10.1111/desc.12079
DatabaseName Istex
CrossRef
ERIC
ERIC (Ovid)
ERIC
ERIC
ERIC (Legacy Platform)
ERIC( SilverPlatter )
ERIC
ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)
Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)
ERIC
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
SwePub
SwePub Articles
SWEPUB Uppsala universitet
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
ERIC
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
CrossRef

ERIC
MEDLINE

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: ERI
  name: ERIC
  url: https://eric.ed.gov/
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1467-7687
ERIC EJ1026003
EndPage 687
ExternalDocumentID oai_DiVA_org_uu_353626
3055269111
24033573
EJ1026003
10_1111_desc_12079
DESC12079
ark_67375_WNG_K49Z2K6P_V
Genre article
Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
Feature
GeographicLocations United Kingdom (London)
London
GeographicLocations_xml – name: United Kingdom (London)
– name: London
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: UEL School of Psychology
– fundername: UEL Promising Researcher Grant
– fundername: UK Medical Research Council
  funderid: G0701484
– fundername: Eranda Foundation
– fundername: Medical Research Council
  grantid: G0701484
GroupedDBID ---
.3N
.GA
.Y3
05W
0R~
10A
1OC
29F
31~
33P
36B
4.4
50Y
50Z
51W
51Y
52M
52O
52Q
52S
52T
52U
52V
52W
53G
5GY
5HH
5LA
5VS
66C
702
7PT
8-0
8-1
8-3
8-4
8-5
8UM
930
A01
A04
AABNI
AAESR
AAHHS
AAHSB
AAONW
AAOUF
AASGY
AAXRX
AAZKR
ABCQN
ABCUV
ABDBF
ABEML
ABIVO
ABPVW
ABQWH
ABSOO
ABXGK
ACAHQ
ACBKW
ACBWZ
ACCFJ
ACCZN
ACFBH
ACGFS
ACGOF
ACHQT
ACMXC
ACPOU
ACSCC
ACXQS
ADBBV
ADBTR
ADEMA
ADEOM
ADIZJ
ADKYN
ADMGS
ADXAS
ADZMN
ADZOD
AEEZP
AEIGN
AEIMD
AEQDE
AEUQT
AEUYR
AFBPY
AFEBI
AFFNX
AFFPM
AFGKR
AFKFF
AFPWT
AFZJQ
AHBTC
AIACR
AIFKG
AIURR
AIWBW
AJBDE
ALAGY
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALUQN
AMBMR
AMYDB
ASPBG
ASTYK
AVWKF
AZBYB
AZFZN
AZVAB
BAFTC
BDRZF
BFHJK
BMXJE
BNVMJ
BQESF
BROTX
BRXPI
BSCLL
BY8
CAG
COF
CS3
D-7
D-C
D-D
DCZOG
DPXWK
DR2
DRFUL
DRMAN
DRSSH
EAD
EAP
EBD
EBS
EJD
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPS
EST
ESX
F00
F01
F5P
FEDTE
FUBAC
G-S
G.N
G50
GODZA
HGLYW
HVGLF
HZI
HZ~
IHE
IX1
J0M
K48
KBYEO
LATKE
LC2
LC4
LEEKS
LH4
LITHE
LOXES
LP6
LP7
LUTES
LW6
LYRES
MEWTI
MK4
MRFUL
MRMAN
MRSSH
MSFUL
MSMAN
MSSSH
MXFUL
MXMAN
MXSSH
N04
N06
N9A
NF~
O66
O9-
OIG
OVD
P2P
P2W
P2Y
P4C
PQQKQ
Q.N
Q11
QB0
R.K
ROL
RX1
SUPJJ
SV3
TEORI
TUS
UB1
UPT
UQL
W8V
W99
WBKPD
WHDPE
WIH
WII
WIJ
WOHZO
WQZ
WRC
WSUWO
WXSBR
XG1
ZZTAW
~IA
~WP
AAHQN
AAIPD
AAMNL
AANHP
AAYCA
ACRPL
ACUHS
ACYXJ
ADNMO
AFWVQ
AFYRF
ALVPJ
AAYXX
AEYWJ
AGHNM
AGQPQ
CITATION
7SW
AAMMB
AEFGJ
AGXDD
AIDQK
AIDYY
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7TK
7X8
ADTPV
AOWAS
DF2
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c4879-be9d363aa7f6c5c543f84e6884bbbe49d74edc3bd915c08c0cdebdf713c32d3c3
IEDL.DBID DR2
ISSN 1363-755X
1467-7687
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 06:33:45 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 20:37:55 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 16:00:33 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 08:03:13 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 05:56:32 EDT 2025
Tue Sep 02 19:36:20 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 02:51:56 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:12:35 EDT 2025
Wed Jan 22 17:03:53 EST 2025
Wed Oct 30 09:54:04 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess false
IsPeerReviewed false
IsScholarly true
Issue 5
Language English
License http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor
2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4879-be9d363aa7f6c5c543f84e6884bbbe49d74edc3bd915c08c0cdebdf713c32d3c3
Notes UK Medical Research Council - No. G0701484
ark:/67375/WNG-K49Z2K6P-V
Data S1. Supporting Methods. Data S2. Supporting Results. Table S1. Additional demographic data on infant childcare arrangements, finances and housing for the entire sample, and participants split by household income.
Eranda Foundation
ArticleID:DESC12079
UEL Promising Researcher Grant
UEL School of Psychology
istex:B414E622F5BDA6B9A347F450A5262F73DA529920
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ObjectType-Article-2
PMID 24033573
PQID 1428006594
PQPubID 2030115
PageCount 12
ParticipantIDs swepub_primary_oai_DiVA_org_uu_353626
proquest_miscellaneous_1439231472
proquest_miscellaneous_1433268205
proquest_journals_1428006594
pubmed_primary_24033573
eric_primary_EJ1026003
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12079
crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_desc_12079
wiley_primary_10_1111_desc_12079_DESC12079
istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_K49Z2K6P_V
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate September 2013
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2013-09-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 09
  year: 2013
  text: September 2013
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace England
PublicationPlace_xml – name: England
– name: Oxford
PublicationTitle Developmental science
PublicationTitleAlternate Dev Sci
PublicationYear 2013
Publisher Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Wiley-Blackwell
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
Publisher_xml – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd
– name: Wiley-Blackwell
– name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
References Bayley, N. (1993). Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd edn.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Rao, H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J.M., Brodsky, N.L., Korczykowski, M., Avants, B.B., Gee, J.C., Wang, J., Hurt, H., Detre, J.A., & Farah, M.J. (2010). Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: evidence from brain morphology in humans. NeuroImage, 49 (1), 1144-1150.
Sadeh, A. (2004). A brief screening questionnaire for infant sleep problems: validation and findings for an internet sample. Pediatrics, 113 (6), e570-577.
Marshall, P.J., Bar-Haim, Y., & Fox, N.A. (2002). Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113 (8), 1199-1208.
Farah, M.J., Shera, D.M., Savage, J.H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J.M., Brodsky, N.L., Malmud, E.K., & Hurt, H. (2006). Childhood poverty: specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110 (1), 166-174.
Jones, N.A., Field, T., Fox, N.A., Lundy, B., & Davalos, M. (1997). EEG activation in 1-month-old infants of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 9 (03), 491-505.
McArdle, P., Prosser, J., & Kolvin, I. (2004). Prevalence of psychiatric disorder: with and without psychosocial impairment. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13 (6), 347-353.
Kishiyama, M.M., Boyce, W.T., Jimenez, A.M., Perry, L.M., & Knight, R.T. (2009). Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21 (6), 1106-1115.
Marshall, P.J., Reeb, B.C., Fox, N.A., Nelson, C.A., 3rd, & Zeanah, C.H. (2008). Effects of early intervention on EEG power and coherence in previously institutionalized children in Romania. Development and Psychopathology, 20 (3), 861-880.
Jones, N.A., Field, T., & Davalos, M. (2000). Right frontal EEG asymmetry and lack of empathy in preschool children of depressed mothers. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 30 (3), 189-204.
Lupien, S.J., King, S., Meaney, M.J., & McEwen, B.S. (2001). Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 653-676.
Fouts, H.N., Roopnarine, J.L., & Lamb, M.E. (2007). Social experiences and daily routines of African American infants in different socioeconomic contexts. Journal of Family Psychology, 21 (4), 655-664.
El-Sheikh, M., Hinnant, J.B., Kelly, R.J., & Erath, S. (2010). Maternal psychological control and child internalizing symptoms: vulnerability and protective factors across bioregulatory and ecological domains. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51 (2), 188-198.
Yordanova, J., Banaschewski, T., Kolev, V., Woerner, W., & Rothenberger, A. (2001). Abnormal early stages of task stimulus processing in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence from event-related gamma oscillations. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112 (6), 1096-1108.
Raizada, R.D.S., Richards, T.L., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P.K. (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage, 40 (3), 1392-1401.
Gou, Z., Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A.A. (2011). Resting frontal gamma power at 16, 24 and 36 months predicts individual differences in language and cognition at 4 and 5 years. Behavioural Brain Research, 220 (2), 263-270.
Corsi-Cabrera, M., Sanchez, A.I., del-Rio-Portilla, Y., Villanueva, Y., & Perez-Garci, E. (2003). Effect of 38 h of total sleep deprivation on the waking EEG in women: sex differences. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50 (3), 213-224.
Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-Jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., & Boyce, W.T. (2011). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: the roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17 (1), 120-132.
El-Sheikh, M., Buckhalt, J.A., Keller, P.S., Cummings, E.M., & Acebo, C. (2007). Child emotional insecurity and academic achievement: the role of sleep disruptions. Journal of Family Psychology, 21 (1), 29-38.
Ray, S., Niebur, E., Hsiao, S.S., Sinai, A., & Crone, N.E. (2008). High-frequency gamma activity (80-150 Hz) is increased in human cortex during selective attention. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119 (1), 116-133.
Van den Bergh, B.R., Mulder, E.J., Mennes, M., & Glover, V. (2005). Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29 (2), 237-258.
Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Hajos, M., Dupuy, F.E., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2011). EEG coherence and symptom profiles of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122 (7), 1327-1332.
Welsh, J.A., Nix, R.L., Blair, C., Bierman, K.L., & Nelson, K.E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102 (1), 43-53.
Lozoff, B., Askew, G.L., & Wolf, A.W. (1996). Cosleeping and early childhood sleep problems: effects of ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17 (1), 9-15.
Bradley, R.H., & Corwyn, R.F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371-399.
McLennan, D., Barnes, H., Noble, M., Davies, J., Garratt, E., & Dibben, C. (2011). The English indices of deprivation 2010: Technical report. London: Department for Communities and Local Government.
Jones, N.A., Field, T., & Almeida, A. (2009). Right frontal EEG asymmetry and behavioral inhibition in infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 32 (3), 298-304.
Orekhova, E.V., Stroganova, T.A., Nygren, G., Tsetlin, M.M., Posikera, I.N., Gillberg, C., & Elam, M. (2007). Excess of high frequency electroencephalogram oscillations in boys with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 62 (9), 1022-1029.
Gross, D.W., & Gotman, J. (1999). Correlation of high-frequency oscillations with the sleep-wake cycle and cognitive activity in humans. Neuroscience, 94 (4), 1005-1018.
Shackman, A.J., McMenamin, B.W., Maxwell, J.S., Greischar, L.L., & Davidson, R.J. (2010). Identifying robust and sensitive frequency bands for interrogating neural oscillations. NeuroImage, 51 (4), 1319-1333.
Engel, A.K., Fries, P., & Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2 (10), 704-716.
Marshall, P.J., & Fox, N.A. (2004). A comparison of the electroencephalogram between institutionalized and community children in Romania. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16 (8), 1327-1338.
Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: an event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science, 12 (4), 634-646.
Hackman, D.A., Farah, M.J., & Meaney, M.J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11 (9), 651-659.
Grice, S.J., Spratling, M.W., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Halit, H., Csibra, G., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M.H. (2001). Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. NeuroReport, 12 (12), 2697-2700.
Moore, D.G., & George, R. (2011). ACORNS: a tool for the visualisation and modelling of atypical development. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 55 (10), 956-972.
Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Hajos, M., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., & Dupuy, F.E. (2010). Resting-state EEG gamma activity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121 (11), 1871-1877.
Kaufman, J., Csibra, G., & Johnson, M.H. (2005). Oscillatory activity in the infant brain reflects object maintenance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102 (42), 15271-15274.
Horsfield, G. (2010). Family spending. A report on the 2009 Living Costs and Food Survey. London: Office for National Statistics.
Sweatt, J.D. (2009). Experience-dependent epigenetic modifications in the central nervous system. Biological Psychiatry, 65 (3), 191-197.
UK Office for National Statistics (2010). Standard Occupational Classification 2010. Volume 3: The National Statistics Socio-economic Classification User Manual. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Noble, K.G., Norman, M.F., & Farah, M.J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8 (1), 74-87.
Buckhalt, J.A., El-Sheikh, M., & Keller, P. (2007). Children's sleep and cognitive functioning: race and socioeconomic status as moderators of effects. Child Develpoment, 78 (1), 213-231.
Adams, N., Barton, A., Bray, S., Johnson, G., & Matejic, P. (2010). Households below average income: An analysis of the income distribution 1994/95 - 2008/09. London: Department for Work and Pensions.
Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Csibra, G., Holmboe, K., Garwood, H., Tucker, L., Krljes, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Charman, T., Baird, G., & Johnson, M.H. (2009). Neural correlates of eye gaze processing in the infant broader autism phenotype. Biological Psychiatry, 65 (1), 31-38.
Hackman, D.A., & Farah, M.J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (2), 65-73.
Crone, N.E., Hao, L., Hart, J., Jr, Boatman, D., Lesser, R.P., Irizarry, R., & Gordon, B. (2001). Electrocorticographic gamma activity during word production in spoken and sign language. Neurology, 57 (11), 2045-2053.
Bernier, A., Carlson, S.M., Bordeleau, S., & Carrier, J. (2010). Relations between physiological and cognitive regulatory systems: infant sleep regulation and subsequent executive functioning. Child Development, 81 (6), 1739-1752.
Benasich, A.A., Gou, Z., Choudhury, N., & Harris, K.D. (2008). Early cognitive and language skills are linked to resting frontal gamma power across the first 3 yea
2010; 11
2002; 53
2002; 113
2010; 102
1975
2011; 55
2011; 17
2003; 50
2007; 78
2005; 29
1997; 9
2009; 12
2009; 13
2001
2005; 102
2008; 119
2008; 22
2006; 1110
2007; 62
1999; 94
2008; 20
2007; 21
2001; 12
2001; 57
2001; 13
2010; 4
2011; 122
2008; 195
1996; 17
2009; 65
2009; 21
2011
2010
2010; 121
2005; 41
2007
1993
2010; 81
2001; 112
2010; 49
2009; 32
2004; 113
2004; 16
2005; 8
2000; 30
2004; 13
2001; 2
2008; 40
2010; 51
2011; 220
1992; 63
e_1_2_7_3_1
Raizada R.D.S. (e_1_2_7_45_1) 2010; 4
e_1_2_7_9_1
e_1_2_7_7_1
e_1_2_7_19_1
e_1_2_7_17_1
e_1_2_7_15_1
e_1_2_7_41_1
e_1_2_7_13_1
e_1_2_7_43_1
e_1_2_7_11_1
e_1_2_7_47_1
e_1_2_7_26_1
UK Office for National Statistics (e_1_2_7_54_1) 2010
e_1_2_7_49_1
e_1_2_7_50_1
e_1_2_7_25_1
e_1_2_7_31_1
e_1_2_7_52_1
e_1_2_7_23_1
e_1_2_7_33_1
e_1_2_7_21_1
e_1_2_7_35_1
e_1_2_7_56_1
e_1_2_7_39_1
e_1_2_7_6_1
e_1_2_7_4_1
e_1_2_7_8_1
e_1_2_7_18_1
e_1_2_7_16_1
e_1_2_7_42_1
e_1_2_7_12_1
e_1_2_7_44_1
e_1_2_7_10_1
e_1_2_7_46_1
e_1_2_7_48_1
e_1_2_7_27_1
Bayley N. (e_1_2_7_5_1) 1993
e_1_2_7_29_1
Adams N. (e_1_2_7_2_1) 2010
McLennan D. (e_1_2_7_37_1) 2011
Marshall P.J. (e_1_2_7_40_1) 2007
Duncan G.J. (e_1_2_7_14_1) 2001
e_1_2_7_51_1
e_1_2_7_30_1
e_1_2_7_53_1
e_1_2_7_24_1
e_1_2_7_32_1
e_1_2_7_55_1
e_1_2_7_22_1
e_1_2_7_34_1
e_1_2_7_57_1
Horsfield G. (e_1_2_7_28_1) 2010
e_1_2_7_20_1
e_1_2_7_36_1
e_1_2_7_38_1
References_xml – reference: Engel, A.K., Fries, P., & Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2 (10), 704-716.
– reference: Sweatt, J.D. (2009). Experience-dependent epigenetic modifications in the central nervous system. Biological Psychiatry, 65 (3), 191-197.
– reference: Hackman, D.A., Farah, M.J., & Meaney, M.J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11 (9), 651-659.
– reference: Bayley, N. (1993). Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd edn.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
– reference: El-Sheikh, M., Buckhalt, J.A., Keller, P.S., Cummings, E.M., & Acebo, C. (2007). Child emotional insecurity and academic achievement: the role of sleep disruptions. Journal of Family Psychology, 21 (1), 29-38.
– reference: McLennan, D., Barnes, H., Noble, M., Davies, J., Garratt, E., & Dibben, C. (2011). The English indices of deprivation 2010: Technical report. London: Department for Communities and Local Government.
– reference: Adams, N., Barton, A., Bray, S., Johnson, G., & Matejic, P. (2010). Households below average income: An analysis of the income distribution 1994/95 - 2008/09. London: Department for Work and Pensions.
– reference: Welsh, J.A., Nix, R.L., Blair, C., Bierman, K.L., & Nelson, K.E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102 (1), 43-53.
– reference: Lozoff, B., Askew, G.L., & Wolf, A.W. (1996). Cosleeping and early childhood sleep problems: effects of ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17 (1), 9-15.
– reference: Marshall, P.J., Reeb, B.C., Fox, N.A., Nelson, C.A., 3rd, & Zeanah, C.H. (2008). Effects of early intervention on EEG power and coherence in previously institutionalized children in Romania. Development and Psychopathology, 20 (3), 861-880.
– reference: Rao, H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J.M., Brodsky, N.L., Korczykowski, M., Avants, B.B., Gee, J.C., Wang, J., Hurt, H., Detre, J.A., & Farah, M.J. (2010). Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: evidence from brain morphology in humans. NeuroImage, 49 (1), 1144-1150.
– reference: D'Angiulli, A., Herdman, A., Stapells, D., & Hertzman, C. (2008). Children's event-related potentials of auditory selective attention vary with their socioeconomic status. Neuropsychology, 22 (3), 293-300.
– reference: Horsfield, G. (2010). Family spending. A report on the 2009 Living Costs and Food Survey. London: Office for National Statistics.
– reference: Corsi-Cabrera, M., Sanchez, A.I., del-Rio-Portilla, Y., Villanueva, Y., & Perez-Garci, E. (2003). Effect of 38 h of total sleep deprivation on the waking EEG in women: sex differences. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50 (3), 213-224.
– reference: Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Hajos, M., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., & Dupuy, F.E. (2010). Resting-state EEG gamma activity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121 (11), 1871-1877.
– reference: Kishiyama, M.M., Boyce, W.T., Jimenez, A.M., Perry, L.M., & Knight, R.T. (2009). Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21 (6), 1106-1115.
– reference: UK Office for National Statistics (2010). Standard Occupational Classification 2010. Volume 3: The National Statistics Socio-economic Classification User Manual. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
– reference: Farah, M.J., Shera, D.M., Savage, J.H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J.M., Brodsky, N.L., Malmud, E.K., & Hurt, H. (2006). Childhood poverty: specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research, 1110 (1), 166-174.
– reference: Van den Bergh, B.R., Mulder, E.J., Mennes, M., & Glover, V. (2005). Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29 (2), 237-258.
– reference: Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Hajos, M., Dupuy, F.E., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2011). EEG coherence and symptom profiles of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122 (7), 1327-1332.
– reference: Jones, N.A., Field, T., & Almeida, A. (2009). Right frontal EEG asymmetry and behavioral inhibition in infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 32 (3), 298-304.
– reference: Bernier, A., Carlson, S.M., Bordeleau, S., & Carrier, J. (2010). Relations between physiological and cognitive regulatory systems: infant sleep regulation and subsequent executive functioning. Child Development, 81 (6), 1739-1752.
– reference: Hackman, D.A., & Farah, M.J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (2), 65-73.
– reference: Buckhalt, J.A., El-Sheikh, M., & Keller, P. (2007). Children's sleep and cognitive functioning: race and socioeconomic status as moderators of effects. Child Develpoment, 78 (1), 213-231.
– reference: Kaufman, J., Csibra, G., & Johnson, M.H. (2005). Oscillatory activity in the infant brain reflects object maintenance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 102 (42), 15271-15274.
– reference: Raizada, R.D.S., Richards, T.L., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P.K. (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage, 40 (3), 1392-1401.
– reference: Shackman, A.J., McMenamin, B.W., Maxwell, J.S., Greischar, L.L., & Davidson, R.J. (2010). Identifying robust and sensitive frequency bands for interrogating neural oscillations. NeuroImage, 51 (4), 1319-1333.
– reference: Lupien, S.J., King, S., Meaney, M.J., & McEwen, B.S. (2001). Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 653-676.
– reference: Yordanova, J., Banaschewski, T., Kolev, V., Woerner, W., & Rothenberger, A. (2001). Abnormal early stages of task stimulus processing in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence from event-related gamma oscillations. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112 (6), 1096-1108.
– reference: McArdle, P., Prosser, J., & Kolvin, I. (2004). Prevalence of psychiatric disorder: with and without psychosocial impairment. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13 (6), 347-353.
– reference: Marshall, P.J., & Fox, N.A. (2004). A comparison of the electroencephalogram between institutionalized and community children in Romania. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16 (8), 1327-1338.
– reference: Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-Jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., & Boyce, W.T. (2011). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: the roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17 (1), 120-132.
– reference: Bradley, R.H., & Corwyn, R.F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 371-399.
– reference: Moore, D.G., & George, R. (2011). ACORNS: a tool for the visualisation and modelling of atypical development. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 55 (10), 956-972.
– reference: Sadeh, A. (2004). A brief screening questionnaire for infant sleep problems: validation and findings for an internet sample. Pediatrics, 113 (6), e570-577.
– reference: Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: an event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science, 12 (4), 634-646.
– reference: Grice, S.J., Halit, H., Farroni, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., & Johnson, M.H. (2005). Neural correlates of eye-gaze detection in young children with autism. Cortex, 41 (3), 342-353.
– reference: Jones, N.A., Field, T., Fox, N.A., Lundy, B., & Davalos, M. (1997). EEG activation in 1-month-old infants of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 9 (03), 491-505.
– reference: Gou, Z., Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A.A. (2011). Resting frontal gamma power at 16, 24 and 36 months predicts individual differences in language and cognition at 4 and 5 years. Behavioural Brain Research, 220 (2), 263-270.
– reference: Grice, S.J., Spratling, M.W., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Halit, H., Csibra, G., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M.H. (2001). Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. NeuroReport, 12 (12), 2697-2700.
– reference: Gross, D.W., & Gotman, J. (1999). Correlation of high-frequency oscillations with the sleep-wake cycle and cognitive activity in humans. Neuroscience, 94 (4), 1005-1018.
– reference: Crone, N.E., Hao, L., Hart, J., Jr, Boatman, D., Lesser, R.P., Irizarry, R., & Gordon, B. (2001). Electrocorticographic gamma activity during word production in spoken and sign language. Neurology, 57 (11), 2045-2053.
– reference: Dawson, G., Klinger, L.G., Panagiotides, H., Hill, D., & Spieker, S. (1992). Frontal lobe activity and affective behavior of infants of mothers with depressive symptoms. Child Development, 63 (3), 725-737.
– reference: Ray, S., Niebur, E., Hsiao, S.S., Sinai, A., & Crone, N.E. (2008). High-frequency gamma activity (80-150 Hz) is increased in human cortex during selective attention. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119 (1), 116-133.
– reference: Benasich, A.A., Gou, Z., Choudhury, N., & Harris, K.D. (2008). Early cognitive and language skills are linked to resting frontal gamma power across the first 3 years. Behavioural Brain Research, 195 (2), 215-222.
– reference: Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Csibra, G., Holmboe, K., Garwood, H., Tucker, L., Krljes, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Charman, T., Baird, G., & Johnson, M.H. (2009). Neural correlates of eye gaze processing in the infant broader autism phenotype. Biological Psychiatry, 65 (1), 31-38.
– reference: Noble, K.G., Norman, M.F., & Farah, M.J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8 (1), 74-87.
– reference: Fouts, H.N., Roopnarine, J.L., & Lamb, M.E. (2007). Social experiences and daily routines of African American infants in different socioeconomic contexts. Journal of Family Psychology, 21 (4), 655-664.
– reference: Jones, N.A., Field, T., & Davalos, M. (2000). Right frontal EEG asymmetry and lack of empathy in preschool children of depressed mothers. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 30 (3), 189-204.
– reference: El-Sheikh, M., Hinnant, J.B., Kelly, R.J., & Erath, S. (2010). Maternal psychological control and child internalizing symptoms: vulnerability and protective factors across bioregulatory and ecological domains. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51 (2), 188-198.
– reference: Orekhova, E.V., Stroganova, T.A., Nygren, G., Tsetlin, M.M., Posikera, I.N., Gillberg, C., & Elam, M. (2007). Excess of high frequency electroencephalogram oscillations in boys with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 62 (9), 1022-1029.
– reference: Raizada, R.D.S., & Kishiyama, M.M. (2010). Effects of socioeconomic status on brain development, and how cognitive neuroscience may contribute to leveling the playing field. [Review]. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 12.
– reference: Marshall, P.J., Bar-Haim, Y., & Fox, N.A. (2002). Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113 (8), 1199-1208.
– year: 2011
– volume: 220
  start-page: 263
  issue: 2
  year: 2011
  end-page: 270
  article-title: Resting frontal gamma power at 16, 24 and 36 months predicts individual differences in language and cognition at 4 and 5 years
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
– volume: 119
  start-page: 116
  issue: 1
  year: 2008
  end-page: 133
  article-title: High‐frequency gamma activity (80–150 Hz) is increased in human cortex during selective attention
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 78
  start-page: 213
  issue: 1
  year: 2007
  end-page: 231
  article-title: Children's sleep and cognitive functioning: race and socioeconomic status as moderators of effects
  publication-title: Child Develpoment
– volume: 22
  start-page: 293
  issue: 3
  year: 2008
  end-page: 300
  article-title: Children's event‐related potentials of auditory selective attention vary with their socioeconomic status
  publication-title: Neuropsychology
– volume: 113
  start-page: 1199
  issue: 8
  year: 2002
  end-page: 1208
  article-title: Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 40
  start-page: 1392
  issue: 3
  year: 2008
  end-page: 1401
  article-title: Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 49
  start-page: 1144
  issue: 1
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1150
  article-title: Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: evidence from brain morphology in humans
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 51
  start-page: 1319
  issue: 4
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1333
  article-title: Identifying robust and sensitive frequency bands for interrogating neural oscillations
  publication-title: NeuroImage
– volume: 53
  start-page: 371
  year: 2002
  end-page: 399
  article-title: Socioeconomic status and child development
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
– volume: 65
  start-page: 191
  issue: 3
  year: 2009
  end-page: 197
  article-title: Experience‐dependent epigenetic modifications in the central nervous system
  publication-title: Biological Psychiatry
– volume: 63
  start-page: 725
  issue: 3
  year: 1992
  end-page: 737
  article-title: Frontal lobe activity and affective behavior of infants of mothers with depressive symptoms
  publication-title: Child Development
– year: 1975
– volume: 21
  start-page: 1106
  issue: 6
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1115
  article-title: Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 32
  start-page: 298
  issue: 3
  year: 2009
  end-page: 304
  article-title: Right frontal EEG asymmetry and behavioral inhibition in infants of depressed mothers
  publication-title: Infant Behavior and Development
– volume: 4
  start-page: 12
  year: 2010
  article-title: Effects of socioeconomic status on brain development, and how cognitive neuroscience may contribute to leveling the playing field. [Review]
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– volume: 16
  start-page: 1327
  issue: 8
  year: 2004
  end-page: 1338
  article-title: A comparison of the electroencephalogram between institutionalized and community children in Romania
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 81
  start-page: 1739
  issue: 6
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1752
  article-title: Relations between physiological and cognitive regulatory systems: infant sleep regulation and subsequent executive functioning
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 94
  start-page: 1005
  issue: 4
  year: 1999
  end-page: 1018
  article-title: Correlation of high‐frequency oscillations with the sleep–wake cycle and cognitive activity in humans
  publication-title: Neuroscience
– volume: 55
  start-page: 956
  issue: 10
  year: 2011
  end-page: 972
  article-title: ACORNS: a tool for the visualisation and modelling of atypical development
  publication-title: Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
– volume: 1110
  start-page: 166
  issue: 1
  year: 2006
  end-page: 174
  article-title: Childhood poverty: specific associations with neurocognitive development
  publication-title: Brain Research
– volume: 2
  start-page: 704
  issue: 10
  year: 2001
  end-page: 716
  article-title: Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top‐down processing
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
– volume: 8
  start-page: 74
  issue: 1
  year: 2005
  end-page: 87
  article-title: Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 113
  start-page: e570
  issue: 6
  year: 2004
  end-page: 577
  article-title: A brief screening questionnaire for infant sleep problems: validation and findings for an internet sample
  publication-title: Pediatrics
– start-page: 83
  year: 2001
  end-page: 106
– volume: 20
  start-page: 861
  issue: 3
  year: 2008
  end-page: 880
  article-title: Effects of early intervention on EEG power and coherence in previously institutionalized children in Romania
  publication-title: Development and Psychopathology
– volume: 13
  start-page: 65
  issue: 2
  year: 2009
  end-page: 73
  article-title: Socioeconomic status and the developing brain
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
– volume: 62
  start-page: 1022
  issue: 9
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1029
  article-title: Excess of high frequency electroencephalogram oscillations in boys with autism
  publication-title: Biological Psychiatry
– year: 1993
– volume: 21
  start-page: 655
  issue: 4
  year: 2007
  end-page: 664
  article-title: Social experiences and daily routines of African American infants in different socioeconomic contexts
  publication-title: Journal of Family Psychology
– volume: 11
  start-page: 651
  issue: 9
  year: 2010
  end-page: 659
  article-title: Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research
  publication-title: Nature Reviews Neuroscience
– volume: 9
  start-page: 491
  issue: 03
  year: 1997
  end-page: 505
  article-title: EEG activation in 1‐month‐old infants of depressed mothers
  publication-title: Development and Psychopathology
– volume: 29
  start-page: 237
  issue: 2
  year: 2005
  end-page: 258
  article-title: Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review
  publication-title: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
– start-page: 227
  year: 2007
  end-page: 250
– volume: 13
  start-page: 347
  issue: 6
  year: 2004
  end-page: 353
  article-title: Prevalence of psychiatric disorder: with and without psychosocial impairment
  publication-title: European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
– volume: 121
  start-page: 1871
  issue: 11
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1877
  article-title: Resting‐state EEG gamma activity in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 51
  start-page: 188
  issue: 2
  year: 2010
  end-page: 198
  article-title: Maternal psychological control and child internalizing symptoms: vulnerability and protective factors across bioregulatory and ecological domains
  publication-title: Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
– volume: 12
  start-page: 2697
  issue: 12
  year: 2001
  end-page: 2700
  article-title: Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome
  publication-title: NeuroReport
– volume: 21
  start-page: 29
  issue: 1
  year: 2007
  end-page: 38
  article-title: Child emotional insecurity and academic achievement: the role of sleep disruptions
  publication-title: Journal of Family Psychology
– volume: 122
  start-page: 1327
  issue: 7
  year: 2011
  end-page: 1332
  article-title: EEG coherence and symptom profiles of children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 41
  start-page: 342
  issue: 3
  year: 2005
  end-page: 353
  article-title: Neural correlates of eye‐gaze detection in young children with autism
  publication-title: Cortex
– volume: 17
  start-page: 120
  issue: 1
  year: 2011
  end-page: 132
  article-title: Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: the roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood
  publication-title: Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society
– volume: 13
  start-page: 653
  year: 2001
  end-page: 676
  article-title: Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status
  publication-title: Development and Psychopathology
– year: 2010
– volume: 50
  start-page: 213
  issue: 3
  year: 2003
  end-page: 224
  article-title: Effect of 38 h of total sleep deprivation on the waking EEG in women: sex differences
  publication-title: International Journal of Psychophysiology
– volume: 12
  start-page: 634
  issue: 4
  year: 2009
  end-page: 646
  article-title: Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: an event‐related brain potential study
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 30
  start-page: 189
  issue: 3
  year: 2000
  end-page: 204
  article-title: Right frontal EEG asymmetry and lack of empathy in preschool children of depressed mothers
  publication-title: Child Psychiatry and Human Development
– volume: 57
  start-page: 2045
  issue: 11
  year: 2001
  end-page: 2053
  article-title: Electrocorticographic gamma activity during word production in spoken and sign language
  publication-title: Neurology
– volume: 65
  start-page: 31
  issue: 1
  year: 2009
  end-page: 38
  article-title: Neural correlates of eye gaze processing in the infant broader autism phenotype
  publication-title: Biological Psychiatry
– volume: 112
  start-page: 1096
  issue: 6
  year: 2001
  end-page: 1108
  article-title: Abnormal early stages of task stimulus processing in children with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence from event‐related gamma oscillations
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 17
  start-page: 9
  issue: 1
  year: 1996
  end-page: 15
  article-title: Cosleeping and early childhood sleep problems: effects of ethnicity and socioeconomic status
  publication-title: Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics
– volume: 102
  start-page: 15271
  issue: 42
  year: 2005
  end-page: 15274
  article-title: Oscillatory activity in the infant brain reflects object maintenance
  publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA
– volume: 102
  start-page: 43
  issue: 1
  year: 2010
  end-page: 53
  article-title: The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low‐income families
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
– volume: 195
  start-page: 215
  issue: 2
  year: 2008
  end-page: 222
  article-title: Early cognitive and language skills are linked to resting frontal gamma power across the first 3 years
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
– ident: e_1_2_7_24_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00343-7
– ident: e_1_2_7_8_1
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233
– ident: e_1_2_7_48_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.09.136
– ident: e_1_2_7_17_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02140.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_33_1
  doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21101
– ident: e_1_2_7_41_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000412
– ident: e_1_2_7_56_1
  doi: 10.1037/a0016738
– ident: e_1_2_7_20_1
  doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.655
– ident: e_1_2_7_4_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.04.022
– volume-title: Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development
  year: 1993
  ident: e_1_2_7_5_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_42_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01471.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_3_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.01.007
– volume-title: Standard Occupational Classification 2010. Volume 3: The National Statistics Socio‐economic Classification User Manual
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_2_7_54_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_11_1
  doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.11.2045
– ident: e_1_2_7_47_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.003
– ident: e_1_2_7_27_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_32_1
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507626102
– ident: e_1_2_7_53_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.002
– start-page: 83
  volume-title: Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development
  year: 2001
  ident: e_1_2_7_14_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_26_1
  doi: 10.1038/nrn2897
– ident: e_1_2_7_23_1
  doi: 10.1097/00001756-200108280-00021
– volume-title: Family spending. A report on the 2009 Living Costs and Food Survey
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_2_7_28_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_36_1
  doi: 10.1007/s00787-004-0367-1
– ident: e_1_2_7_57_1
  doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00524-7
– ident: e_1_2_7_52_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00807.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_13_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01657.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_43_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_49_1
  doi: 10.1542/peds.113.6.e570
– ident: e_1_2_7_38_1
  doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00163-3
– ident: e_1_2_7_35_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0954579401003133
– start-page: 227
  volume-title: Infant EEG and event‐related potentials
  year: 2007
  ident: e_1_2_7_40_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_16_1
  doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.29
– ident: e_1_2_7_12_1
  doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.293
– ident: e_1_2_7_22_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70271-5
– ident: e_1_2_7_18_1
  doi: 10.1038/35094565
– ident: e_1_2_7_31_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0954579497001260
– ident: e_1_2_7_44_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.029
– ident: e_1_2_7_46_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.021
– ident: e_1_2_7_7_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01507.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_15_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.034
– ident: e_1_2_7_30_1
  doi: 10.1023/A:1021399605526
– volume: 4
  start-page: 12
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_2_7_45_1
  article-title: Effects of socioeconomic status on brain development, and how cognitive neuroscience may contribute to leveling the playing field. [Review]
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– ident: e_1_2_7_9_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00993.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_6_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.049
– volume-title: The English indices of deprivation 2010: Technical report
  year: 2011
  ident: e_1_2_7_37_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_25_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003
– ident: e_1_2_7_39_1
  doi: 10.1162/0898929042304723
– ident: e_1_2_7_19_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.072
– ident: e_1_2_7_21_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.01.048
– ident: e_1_2_7_55_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.007
– volume-title: Households below average income: An analysis of the income distribution 1994/95 – 2008/09
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_2_7_2_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_10_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00168-5
– ident: e_1_2_7_51_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.037
– ident: e_1_2_7_29_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.04.004
– ident: e_1_2_7_34_1
  doi: 10.1097/00004703-199602000-00002
– ident: e_1_2_7_50_1
  doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001335
SSID ssj0004301
Score 2.4822652
Snippet Socioeconomic status (SES) impacts on both structural and functional brain development in childhood, but how early its effects can be demonstrated is unknown....
SourceID swepub
proquest
pubmed
eric
crossref
wiley
istex
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 676
SubjectTerms Analysis of Variance
At Risk Persons
Body Weight
Brain
Brain - growth & development
Brain Hemisphere Functions
Child Development
Child Development - physiology
Economically Disadvantaged
Electroencephalography
Employment Level
England (London)
Female
Foreign Countries
Humans
Infant
Infants
London
Male
Occupations
Parent Influence
Sleep
Sleep - physiology
Smoking
Social Class
Socioeconomic factors
Socioeconomic Influences
Socioeconomic Status
Surveys and Questionnaires
Title Socioeconomic status and functional brain development - associations in early infancy
URI https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-K49Z2K6P-V/fulltext.pdf
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fdesc.12079
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1026003
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24033573
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1428006594
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1433268205
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1439231472
https://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-353626
Volume 16
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1baxUxEB5KfemL1kt17YWIF1DYw-4m2Qv4UtpTS4tF1LYHQUJuK6Vlj5w9C9Yn_4P_0F9iJns5VkpBX5ZAZiGZzCRfkpkvAM-YKRLJchOWmeV4zRiF0qo8lLFSbskoVJligvPbo3T_mB1M-GQJXve5MC0_xHDghp7h52t0cKnqP5zc2FqP4iTKMHsPg7UQEb1fcEcxGnVJVzTMOJ903KQYxrP49cpq1AU830L9frsOcQ50oleRrF-K9u7A574TbQTK-aiZq5H-_he_4__2chVudxiVbLdGdReWbHUPVoap8vI-nPo8F9slNRPMSmpqIitDcJ1sjxeJwtcniFlEJZFfP34SubCHmrh6iwzLrlDiNP8AjvfGH3f2w-6JhlC7nU4RKlsYp2ApszLVXHNGy5zZNM-ZUsqywmTMGk2VKWKuo1xH2lhlSrcz1jQx7rMGy9W0so-A6CRWtHQTEKUOoqH9pEliSxZHuUISmgBe9kMldMdfjs9oXIh-H4O6El5XATwdZL-2rB3XSq3hiA8S44MYGdYiGsALbwNDjZydYwBcxsXp0RtxyIpPyWH6TpwEsNEbieh8vxbIYYfIrmABPBmqndfiVYys7LRBGepws0Nf_EYZRN8sSwJ42Brg0CBkUaQ8cy193lrkUIN04btnJ9tiOvsimkZQjoRDAbzyVnaDNsTu-MOOLz3-F-F1WEn8CyEYdrcBy_NZYzcdTpurLe-PW_5E4jfVQTm2
linkProvider Wiley-Blackwell
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9QwEB5Be6AXKI9CoIARD4lKWSWxncexarcs3XaFoI8VFyt-pKqKsmizkYBT_0P_YX8JHiebpaiqBJfIkidSMp6xP9sz3wC8YTqLcpZqv0gMx2vGwM-NTP08lNIuGZksYkxw3h_Fg0O2O-bjNjYHc2EafojuwA09w83X6OB4IP2Hl2tTqV4YBUl2G5axpLfbUX1esEcxGrRpV9RPOB-37KQYyLN498p61IY8L6OGf1yHOTtC0atY1i1GO_eaiquV4zDEGJSzXj2TPfXrL4bH__7PVbjbwlSy2djVfbhlygew0s2WPx_CsUt1MW1eM8HEpLoieakJLpXNCSORWICC6EVgErk8vyD5wiQqYvsNkizbRoEz_SM43OkfbA38tkqDr-xmJ_OlybTVcJ4nRay44owWKTNxmjIppWGZTpjRikqdhVwFqQqUNlIXdnOsaKTtYw2WyklpngBRUShpYecgSi1KQxOKo8gULAxSiTw0Hryfj5VQLYU5VtL4JuZbGdSVcLry4HUn-70h7rhWag2HvJPo74ZIshZQD945I-h68ukZxsAlXByPPoghy75Gw_iTOPJgfW4lonX_SiCNHYK7jHnwquu2jou3MXlpJjXKUAudLQDjN8ogAGdJ5MHjxgK7D0IiRcoT-6VvG5PsepAxfPv0aFNMpieirgXlyDnkwYYzsxu0Ibb7X7Zc6-m_CL-EO4OD_T2x93E0fAYrkSsYglF467A0m9bmuYVtM_nCOedv4s480A
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1ba9RAFD7UFqQv9VqNVh3xAhayJJmZJAO-lO6utatLUdsuggyZS4pUsmV3A-qT_8F_6C9xTm5rpRT0JQTmBGZOvjPzTXLONwBPmRFRxlLj54nl-Jsx8DOrUj8LlXJLhlB5jAXOb8fx3iHbn_DJCrxsa2FqfYjugxtGRjVfY4CfmfyPIDd2rnthFCTiCqyxOEgR0_13S_EoRoOm6or6CeeTRpwU83iWz55bjpqM5zV08NeLKGenJ3qeylZr0fAafGpHUaegnPbKherp738JPP7vMK_DRkNSyU6NqhuwYoubsN7Nld9uwXFV6GKbqmaCZUnlnGSFIbhQ1t8XicLjJ4hZpiWRXz9-kmwJiDlx7RYllt1NjvP8bTgcDj7s7vnNGQ2-dlsd4SsrjHNwliV5rLnmjOYps3GaMqWUZcIkzBpNlREh10GqA22sMrnbGmsaGXfZhNViWti7QHQUKpq7GYhSx9EQQHEU2ZyFQapQhcaDF-2rkroRMMdzNL7IdiODvpKVrzx40tme1bIdF1pt4hvvLAb7IUqsBdSD5xUGupZsdooZcAmXx-NXcsTEx2gUH8gjD7ZakMgm-OcSReyQ2gnmweOu2YUt_ovJCjst0YY64uzoF7_UBuk3SyIP7tQA7DqEMoqUJ66nz2pEdi2oF97_fLQjp7MTWZaSclQc8mC7Qtkl3pD9wfvd6u7evxg_gqsH_aF883o8ug_rUXVaCKbgbcHqYlbaB46zLdTDKjR_AyG9O4g
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Socioeconomic+status+and+functional+brain+development+-+associations+in+early+infancy&rft.jtitle=Developmental+science&rft.au=Tomalski%2C+Przemyslaw&rft.au=Moore%2C+Derek+G.&rft.au=Ribeiro%2C+Helena&rft.au=Axelsson%2C+Emma+L.&rft.date=2013-09-01&rft.pub=Blackwell+Publishing+Ltd&rft.issn=1363-755X&rft.eissn=1467-7687&rft.volume=16&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=676&rft.epage=687&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fdesc.12079&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=ark_67375_WNG_K49Z2K6P_V
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon