Examining the Influence of Cognitive Load and Environmental Conditions on Autonomic Nervous System Response in Military Aircrew: A Hypoxia–Normoxia Study

Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of differen...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBiology (Basel, Switzerland) Vol. 13; no. 5; p. 343
Main Authors Wittels, Harrison L., Wittels, S. Howard, Wishon, Michael J., Vogl, Jonathan, St. Onge, Paul, McDonald, Samantha M., Temme, Leonard A.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Switzerland MDPI AG 01.05.2024
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects (n = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O2) and normoxic (21.0% O2) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O2 saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter MonitorTM (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey’s test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (−rMSSD: −0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: −13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (−2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, p < 0.0001; rMSSD: −2.2 ms, 95% CI: −4.2, −0.2, p = 0.03; SDNN: −14.3 ms, 95% CI: −18.4, −10.1, p < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, p = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (−13.3 ms, 95% CI, −17.5, −8.9, p < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.
AbstractList Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects (n = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O2) and normoxic (21.0% O2) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O2 saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter MonitorTM (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey's test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (-rMSSD: -0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: -13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (-2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, p < 0.0001; rMSSD: -2.2 ms, 95% CI: -4.2, -0.2, p = 0.03; SDNN: -14.3 ms, 95% CI: -18.4, -10.1, p < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, p = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (-13.3 ms, 95% CI, -17.5, -8.9, p < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects (n = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O2) and normoxic (21.0% O2) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O2 saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter MonitorTM (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey's test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (-rMSSD: -0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: -13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (-2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, p < 0.0001; rMSSD: -2.2 ms, 95% CI: -4.2, -0.2, p = 0.03; SDNN: -14.3 ms, 95% CI: -18.4, -10.1, p < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, p = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (-13.3 ms, 95% CI, -17.5, -8.9, p < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.
Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects ( = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O ) and normoxic (21.0% O ) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter Monitor (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey's test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (-rMSSD: -0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: -13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (-2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, < 0.0001; rMSSD: -2.2 ms, 95% CI: -4.2, -0.2, = 0.03; SDNN: -14.3 ms, 95% CI: -18.4, -10.1, < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (-13.3 ms, 95% CI, -17.5, -8.9, < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.
During combat missions, military aircrew rely on their ability to make accurate life-or-death decisions within infinitesimal timeframes. Research shows that performing more challenging tasks (e.g., high cognitive load) requires a greater supply of oxygen to the brain, a process regulated by the autonomic nervous system. However, the influence of prolonged exposure to performing low and high cognitive loads, specifically related to flight operations, is unclear. Thus, the current study evaluated the response of the autonomic nervous system to sustained performance of tasks requiring low or high cognitive functions under two different environmental conditions: hypoxia (i.e., reduced oxygen content) and normoxia (i.e., standard oxygen content). Twenty military and civilian adults (19 to 45 years old) participated in the study, and each performed a series of low- and high-cognitive-load assessments for nearly two hours. All subjects performed these tests separately under hypoxic (14.0% oxygen) and normoxic (21.0% oxygen) conditions. Autonomic nervous system function was measured using a monitoring device placed on the upper right arm that collected heart rate and heart rate variability data. Our study found that prolonged exposure to high cognitive loads induced heightened activity of the autonomic nervous system, with subjects eliciting higher heart rates and lower heart rate variability relative to low cognitive loads. Additionally, the influence of high cognitive load was greater in the hypoxic environment. Accounting for the response of the autonomic nervous system to cognitive loads is important, as this stress likely compounds to the existing fight-or-flight response occurring during pre-flight and in-flight operations. Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects (n = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O[sub.2]) and normoxic (21.0% O[sub.2]) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O[sub.2] saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter Monitor[sup.TM] (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey’s test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (−rMSSD: −0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: −13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (−2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, p < 0.0001; rMSSD: −2.2 ms, 95% CI: −4.2, −0.2, p = 0.03; SDNN: −14.3 ms, 95% CI: −18.4, −10.1, p < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, p = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (−13.3 ms, 95% CI, −17.5, −8.9, p < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.
Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects (n = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O2) and normoxic (21.0% O2) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O2 saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter MonitorTM (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey’s test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (−rMSSD: −0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: −13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (−2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, p < 0.0001; rMSSD: −2.2 ms, 95% CI: −4.2, −0.2, p = 0.03; SDNN: −14.3 ms, 95% CI: −18.4, −10.1, p < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, p = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (−13.3 ms, 95% CI, −17.5, −8.9, p < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.
During combat missions, military aircrew rely on their ability to make accurate life-or-death decisions within infinitesimal timeframes. Research shows that performing more challenging tasks (e.g., high cognitive load) requires a greater supply of oxygen to the brain, a process regulated by the autonomic nervous system. However, the influence of prolonged exposure to performing low and high cognitive loads, specifically related to flight operations, is unclear. Thus, the current study evaluated the response of the autonomic nervous system to sustained performance of tasks requiring low or high cognitive functions under two different environmental conditions: hypoxia (i.e., reduced oxygen content) and normoxia (i.e., standard oxygen content). Twenty military and civilian adults (19 to 45 years old) participated in the study, and each performed a series of low- and high-cognitive-load assessments for nearly two hours. All subjects performed these tests separately under hypoxic (14.0% oxygen) and normoxic (21.0% oxygen) conditions. Autonomic nervous system function was measured using a monitoring device placed on the upper right arm that collected heart rate and heart rate variability data. Our study found that prolonged exposure to high cognitive loads induced heightened activity of the autonomic nervous system, with subjects eliciting higher heart rates and lower heart rate variability relative to low cognitive loads. Additionally, the influence of high cognitive load was greater in the hypoxic environment. Accounting for the response of the autonomic nervous system to cognitive loads is important, as this stress likely compounds to the existing fight-or-flight response occurring during pre-flight and in-flight operations.
Simple SummaryDuring combat missions, military aircrew rely on their ability to make accurate life-or-death decisions within infinitesimal timeframes. Research shows that performing more challenging tasks (e.g., high cognitive load) requires a greater supply of oxygen to the brain, a process regulated by the autonomic nervous system. However, the influence of prolonged exposure to performing low and high cognitive loads, specifically related to flight operations, is unclear. Thus, the current study evaluated the response of the autonomic nervous system to sustained performance of tasks requiring low or high cognitive functions under two different environmental conditions: hypoxia (i.e., reduced oxygen content) and normoxia (i.e., standard oxygen content). Twenty military and civilian adults (19 to 45 years old) participated in the study, and each performed a series of low- and high-cognitive-load assessments for nearly two hours. All subjects performed these tests separately under hypoxic (14.0% oxygen) and normoxic (21.0% oxygen) conditions. Autonomic nervous system function was measured using a monitoring device placed on the upper right arm that collected heart rate and heart rate variability data. Our study found that prolonged exposure to high cognitive loads induced heightened activity of the autonomic nervous system, with subjects eliciting higher heart rates and lower heart rate variability relative to low cognitive loads. Additionally, the influence of high cognitive load was greater in the hypoxic environment. Accounting for the response of the autonomic nervous system to cognitive loads is important, as this stress likely compounds to the existing fight-or-flight response occurring during pre-flight and in-flight operations.AbstractExecuting flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects (n = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O2) and normoxic (21.0% O2) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O2 saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter MonitorTM (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey’s test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (−rMSSD: −0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: −13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (−2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, p < 0.0001; rMSSD: −2.2 ms, 95% CI: −4.2, −0.2, p = 0.03; SDNN: −14.3 ms, 95% CI: −18.4, −10.1, p < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, p = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (−13.3 ms, 95% CI, −17.5, −8.9, p < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.
Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is crucial for regulating the supply of oxygen (O2) to the brain, but it is unclear how sustained cognitive loads of different complexities may affect this regulation. Therefore, in the current study, ANS responses to low and high cognitive loads in hypoxic and normoxic conditions were evaluated. The present analysis used data from a previously conducted, two-factor experimental design. Healthy subjects (n = 24) aged 19 to 45 years and located near Fort Novosel, AL, participated in the parent study. Over two, 2-h trials, subjects were exposed to hypoxic (14.0% O₂) and normoxic (21.0% O₂) air while simultaneously performing one, 15-min and one, 10-min simulation incorporating low- and high-cognitive aviation-related tasks, respectively. The tests were alternated across five, 27-min epochs; however, only epochs 2 through 4 were used in the analyses. Heart rate (HR), HR variability (HRV), and arterial O₂ saturation were continuously measured using the Warfighter Monitorᵀᴹ (Tiger Tech Solutions, Inc., Miami, FL, USA), a previously validated armband device equipped with electrocardiographic and pulse oximetry capabilities. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) regression models were performed to compare ANS responses between the low- and high-cognitive-load assessments under hypoxic and normoxic conditions. Pairwise comparisons corrected for familywise error were performed using Tukey’s test within and between high and low cognitive loads under each environmental condition. Across epochs 2 through 4, in both the hypoxic condition and the normoxic condition, the high-cognitive-load assessment (MATB-II) elicited heightened ANS activity, reflected by increased HR (+2.4 ± 6.9 bpm) and decreased HRV (−rMSSD: −0.4 ± 2.7 ms and SDNN: −13.6 ± 14.6 ms). Conversely, low cognitive load (ADVT) induced an improvement in ANS activity, with reduced HR (−2.6 ± 6.3 bpm) and increased HRV (rMSSD: +1.8 ± 6.0 ms and SDNN: vs. +0.7 ± 6.3 ms). Similar observations were found for the normoxic condition, albeit to a lower degree. These within-group ANS responses were significantly different between high and low cognitive loads (HR: +5.0 bpm, 95% CI: 2.1, 7.9, p < 0.0001; rMSSD: −2.2 ms, 95% CI: −4.2, −0.2, p = 0.03; SDNN: −14.3 ms, 95% CI: −18.4, −10.1, p < 0.0001) under the hypoxic condition. For normoxia, significant differences in ANS response were only observed for HR (+4.3 bpm, 95% CI: 1.2, 7.4, p = 0.002). Lastly, only high cognitive loads elicited significant differences between hypoxic and normoxic conditions but just for SDNN (−13.3 ms, 95% CI, −17.5, −8.9, p < 0.0001). Our study observations suggest that compared to low cognitive loads, performing high-cognitive-load tasks significantly alters ANS activity, especially under hypoxic conditions. Accounting for this response is critical, as military personnel during flight operations sustain exposure to high cognitive loads of unpredictable duration and frequency. Additionally, this is likely compounded by the increased ANS activity consequent to pre-flight activities and anticipation of combat-related outcomes.
Audience Academic
Author Temme, Leonard A.
Vogl, Jonathan
St. Onge, Paul
Wishon, Michael J.
Wittels, S. Howard
Wittels, Harrison L.
McDonald, Samantha M.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Harrison L.
  orcidid: 0000-0002-1244-3699
  surname: Wittels
  fullname: Wittels, Harrison L.
– sequence: 2
  givenname: S. Howard
  surname: Wittels
  fullname: Wittels, S. Howard
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Michael J.
  surname: Wishon
  fullname: Wishon, Michael J.
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Jonathan
  surname: Vogl
  fullname: Vogl, Jonathan
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Paul
  surname: St. Onge
  fullname: St. Onge, Paul
– sequence: 6
  givenname: Samantha M.
  surname: McDonald
  fullname: McDonald, Samantha M.
– sequence: 7
  givenname: Leonard A.
  orcidid: 0000-0002-4279-7834
  surname: Temme
  fullname: Temme, Leonard A.
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38785825$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFksFuEzEQhi1UREvomRuyxIVLWnu9Xnu5RVGgkUKRKJxXXns2uNq1g-0NzY134Mjb8SQ4pBSokLAPHlnf_GPP_I_RkfMOEHpKyRljNTlvre_9ekcZ4YSV7AE6KYiop0IwcfRHfIxOY7wmeQlSVKx6hI6ZFJLLgp-gb4sbNVhn3Rqnj4CXrutHcBqw7_Dcr51Ndgt45ZXByhm8cFsbvBvAJdVnwJkMeBexd3g2Ju_8YDW-hLD1Y8RXu5hgwO8gbjID2Dr8xvY2qbDDMxt0gM8v8Qxf7Db-xqrvX75e-jDsQ3yVRrN7gh52qo9wentO0IdXi_fzi-nq7evlfLaa6lJWaQoSWsoYNcxUNW9bVZpCdrIkWgtaalFIEIp1QFRVcaFK0rG20BKglsoQU7AJWh50jVfXzSbYIT-w8co2Py98WDcqJKt7aKhoTRYrBOWsbIvcRc00KXnLJaOU7bVeHLQ2wX8aIaZmsFFD3ysHuSUNy4k8j6vm_0dJRZioqvy3CXp-D732Y3C5KZnidTaDpPVvaq3yU63rfApK70WbmcgFJWXlvuzZP6i8DeTZZYd1Nt__lfDstvjYDmDuGvTLRBk4PwA6-BgDdHcIJc3eqc09p7IfuO_b3A
Cites_doi 10.3389/fphys.2021.665821
10.1155/2020/5640784
10.3389/fcvm.2022.931219
10.3389/fpubh.2017.00265
10.3390/biology12111398
10.52082/jssm.2022.260
10.1155/2016/8146809
10.1016/j.rmed.2013.02.004
10.1093/milmed/usad025
10.17744/mehc.31.2.161u820r2255t667
10.3758/s13428-020-01364-w
10.31234/osf.io/5hqp2
10.1177/1541931214581046
10.1093/milmed/usab219
10.3389/fphys.2019.00565
10.1016/0304-3940(93)90094-2
10.1080/00140139.2023.2194592
10.21236/ADA457239
10.3138/jmvfh.2018-0055
10.1016/j.jelectrocard.2021.06.003
10.3357/ASEM.1989.2007
10.1186/1744-9081-7-17
10.1016/j.autneu.2005.09.005
10.1007/s41465-019-00145-4
10.1177/154193129203601811
10.1093/milmed/149.2.89
10.30773/pi.2017.08.17
10.3357/AMHP.5022.2018
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright COPYRIGHT 2024 MDPI AG
2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Copyright_xml – notice: COPYRIGHT 2024 MDPI AG
– notice: 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
NPM
7QP
7TK
8FD
8FE
8FH
ABUWG
AFKRA
AZQEC
BBNVY
BENPR
BHPHI
CCPQU
COVID
DWQXO
FR3
GNUQQ
HCIFZ
LK8
M7P
P64
PHGZM
PHGZT
PIMPY
PKEHL
PQEST
PQGLB
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
RC3
7X8
7S9
L.6
DOA
DOI 10.3390/biology13050343
DatabaseName CrossRef
PubMed
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Technology Research Database
ProQuest SciTech Collection
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
ProQuest Central Essentials
Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central
Natural Science Collection
ProQuest One
Coronavirus Research Database
ProQuest Central Korea
Engineering Research Database
ProQuest Central Student
SciTech Premium Collection
Biological Sciences
Biological Science Database
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
ProQuest Central Premium
ProQuest One Academic (New)
ProQuest Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central China
Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
AGRICOLA
AGRICOLA - Academic
DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
PubMed
Publicly Available Content Database
ProQuest Central Student
Technology Research Database
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
SciTech Premium Collection
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Central China
ProQuest Central
ProQuest One Applied & Life Sciences
Genetics Abstracts
Natural Science Collection
ProQuest Central Korea
Biological Science Collection
ProQuest Central (New)
ProQuest Biological Science Collection
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
Coronavirus Research Database
Biological Science Database
ProQuest SciTech Collection
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
Engineering Research Database
ProQuest One Academic
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
ProQuest One Academic (New)
MEDLINE - Academic
AGRICOLA
AGRICOLA - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed

CrossRef

Publicly Available Content Database

AGRICOLA
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: DOA
  name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: BENPR
  name: ProQuest Central
  url: https://www.proquest.com/central
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Biology
EISSN 2079-7737
ExternalDocumentID oai_doaj_org_article_17bd7a3271534b2785c3c045b5831132
A795381345
38785825
10_3390_biology13050343
Genre Journal Article
GeographicLocations United States--US
GeographicLocations_xml – name: United States--US
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Small Business Technology Transfer - Army Research Office
  grantid: W911NF22C0029
– fundername: Military Operational Research Mishap Prevention Program
  grantid: 20G2030.1
GroupedDBID 2XV
53G
5VS
8FE
8FH
AADQD
AAFWJ
AAHBH
AAYXX
ADBBV
AFKRA
AFPKN
AFZYC
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
BBNVY
BCNDV
BENPR
BHPHI
CCPQU
CITATION
EBD
GROUPED_DOAJ
HCIFZ
HYE
IAO
IHR
ITC
KQ8
LK8
M48
M7P
MODMG
M~E
OK1
PGMZT
PHGZM
PHGZT
PIMPY
PROAC
RPM
NPM
PMFND
7QP
7TK
8FD
ABUWG
AZQEC
COVID
DWQXO
FR3
GNUQQ
P64
PKEHL
PQEST
PQGLB
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
RC3
7X8
7S9
L.6
PUEGO
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c486t-e8eb1331d3d695bba4d28f840cc714c728e7a3fe0a6657a40f3b2c8ee98ad0d23
IEDL.DBID BENPR
ISSN 2079-7737
IngestDate Wed Aug 27 01:29:58 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 23:21:53 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 13:51:13 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 11:45:40 EDT 2025
Tue Jun 17 22:13:30 EDT 2025
Tue Jun 10 21:08:31 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 06:56:57 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:29:09 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 5
Keywords hypoxia
aviators
cognitive load
autonomic nervous system
military
normoxia
sympathetic drive
Language English
License https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c486t-e8eb1331d3d695bba4d28f840cc714c728e7a3fe0a6657a40f3b2c8ee98ad0d23
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0002-1244-3699
0000-0002-4279-7834
OpenAccessLink https://www.proquest.com/docview/3059390819?pq-origsite=%requestingapplication%
PMID 38785825
PQID 3059390819
PQPubID 2032427
ParticipantIDs doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_17bd7a3271534b2785c3c045b5831132
proquest_miscellaneous_3153550395
proquest_miscellaneous_3060376613
proquest_journals_3059390819
gale_infotracmisc_A795381345
gale_infotracacademiconefile_A795381345
pubmed_primary_38785825
crossref_primary_10_3390_biology13050343
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2024-05-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2024-05-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 05
  year: 2024
  text: 2024-05-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace Switzerland
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Switzerland
– name: Basel
PublicationTitle Biology (Basel, Switzerland)
PublicationTitleAlternate Biology (Basel)
PublicationYear 2024
Publisher MDPI AG
Publisher_xml – name: MDPI AG
References Peck (ref_21) 2021; 67
Hoshi (ref_5) 1993; 150
Potter (ref_28) 2009; 31
ref_14
McDougall (ref_33) 2005; 123
ref_13
ref_11
Benarroch (ref_6) 2020; 26
Peck (ref_20) 2023; 188
Chan (ref_19) 2013; 107
Grassmann (ref_3) 2016; 2016
Kim (ref_31) 2018; 15
ref_18
ref_16
Wang (ref_1) 2020; 2020
Bowers (ref_2) 2014; 58
Scholey (ref_4) 2020; 4
Bourdillon (ref_17) 2022; 21
Cegarra (ref_15) 2020; 52
Shaw (ref_9) 2021; 12
Bouak (ref_7) 2018; 89
Bouak (ref_24) 2019; 5
Joseph (ref_27) 2007; 51
Garbi (ref_26) 2023; 188
Delliaux (ref_34) 2019; 10
ref_23
Mizuno (ref_22) 2011; 7
Li (ref_32) 2022; 9
Backs (ref_12) 1992; 36
Sampson (ref_29) 1984; 149
Ernst (ref_30) 2017; 5
ref_8
Smith (ref_25) 2007; 78
Shaw (ref_10) 2023; 66
References_xml – volume: 12
  start-page: 665821
  year: 2021
  ident: ref_9
  article-title: Hypoxic Hypoxia and Brain Function in Military Aviation: Basic Physiology and Applied Perspectives
  publication-title: Front. Physiol.
  doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.665821
– volume: 2020
  start-page: 5640784
  year: 2020
  ident: ref_1
  article-title: Cognitive Load Identification of Pilots Based on Physiological-Psychological Characteristics in Complex Environments
  publication-title: J. Adv. Transp.
  doi: 10.1155/2020/5640784
– volume: 9
  start-page: 931219
  year: 2022
  ident: ref_32
  article-title: The Mechanism of Cardiac Sympathetic Activity Assessment Methods: Current Knowledge
  publication-title: Front. Cardiovasc. Med.
  doi: 10.3389/fcvm.2022.931219
– volume: 5
  start-page: 265
  year: 2017
  ident: ref_30
  article-title: Hidden Signals-The History and Methods of Heart Rate Variability
  publication-title: Front Public Health
  doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00265
– volume: 51
  start-page: 1
  year: 2007
  ident: ref_27
  article-title: An Overview of Psychological Factors and Interventions in Air Combat Operations
  publication-title: Indian J. Aerosp. Med.
– ident: ref_11
  doi: 10.3390/biology12111398
– volume: 21
  start-page: 260
  year: 2022
  ident: ref_17
  article-title: RMSSD Is More Sensitive to Artifacts Than Frequency-Domain Parameters: Implication in Athletes’ Monitoring
  publication-title: J. Sports Sci. Med.
  doi: 10.52082/jssm.2022.260
– volume: 2016
  start-page: 8146809
  year: 2016
  ident: ref_3
  article-title: Respiratory Changes in Response to Cognitive Load: A Systematic Review
  publication-title: Neural Plast.
  doi: 10.1155/2016/8146809
– volume: 107
  start-page: 789
  year: 2013
  ident: ref_19
  article-title: Pulse Oximetry: Understanding Its Basic Principles Facilitates Appreciation of Its Limitations
  publication-title: Respir. Med.
  doi: 10.1016/j.rmed.2013.02.004
– volume: 188
  start-page: e2661
  year: 2023
  ident: ref_20
  article-title: COVID-19-Induced Changes in Photoplethysmography
  publication-title: Mil. Med.
  doi: 10.1093/milmed/usad025
– ident: ref_14
– volume: 26
  start-page: 12
  year: 2020
  ident: ref_6
  article-title: Physiology and Pathophysiology of the Autonomic Nervous System
  publication-title: Continuum
– ident: ref_18
– volume: 31
  start-page: 137
  year: 2009
  ident: ref_28
  article-title: Combat Stress Reactions During Military Deployments: Evaluation of the Effectiveness of Combat Stress Control Treatment
  publication-title: J. Ment. Health Couns.
  doi: 10.17744/mehc.31.2.161u820r2255t667
– volume: 52
  start-page: 1980
  year: 2020
  ident: ref_15
  article-title: OpenMATB: A Multi-Attribute Task Battery Promoting Task Customization, Software Extensibility and Experiment Replicability
  publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01364-w
– ident: ref_16
  doi: 10.31234/osf.io/5hqp2
– volume: 58
  start-page: 220
  year: 2014
  ident: ref_2
  article-title: The Effects of Workload Transitions in a Multitasking Environment
  publication-title: Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet.
  doi: 10.1177/1541931214581046
– ident: ref_23
– volume: 188
  start-page: e301
  year: 2023
  ident: ref_26
  article-title: Predictors of Emotional Distress in Combat Military Flight Engineers
  publication-title: Mil. Med.
  doi: 10.1093/milmed/usab219
– volume: 10
  start-page: 565
  year: 2019
  ident: ref_34
  article-title: Mental Workload Alters Heart Rate Variability, Lowering Non-Linear Dynamics
  publication-title: Front. Physiol.
  doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00565
– volume: 150
  start-page: 5
  year: 1993
  ident: ref_5
  article-title: Detection of Dynamic Changes in Cerebral Oxygenation Coupled to Neuronal Function during Mental Work in Man
  publication-title: Neurosci. Lett.
  doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90094-2
– volume: 66
  start-page: 2242
  year: 2023
  ident: ref_10
  article-title: Integrating Physiological Monitoring Systems in Military Aviation: A Brief Narrative Review of Its Importance, Opportunities, and Risks
  publication-title: Ergonomics
  doi: 10.1080/00140139.2023.2194592
– ident: ref_8
– ident: ref_13
  doi: 10.21236/ADA457239
– volume: 5
  start-page: 40
  year: 2019
  ident: ref_24
  article-title: Performance and Health Effects of Mild Hypoxic Hypoxia in Simulated 6-Hour Exposures between 2,438 and 3,048 Metres
  publication-title: J. Mil. Veteran Fam. Health
  doi: 10.3138/jmvfh.2018-0055
– volume: 67
  start-page: 136
  year: 2021
  ident: ref_21
  article-title: Single Limb Electrocardiogram Using Vector Mapping: Evaluation and Validation of a Novel Medical Device
  publication-title: J. Electrocardiol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jelectrocard.2021.06.003
– volume: 78
  start-page: 979
  year: 2007
  ident: ref_25
  article-title: Acute Hypoxia and Related Symptoms on Mild Exertion at Simulated Altitudes below 3048 m
  publication-title: Aviat. Space Environ. Med.
  doi: 10.3357/ASEM.1989.2007
– volume: 7
  start-page: 17
  year: 2011
  ident: ref_22
  article-title: Mental Fatigue Caused by Prolonged Cognitive Load Associated with Sympathetic Hyperactivity
  publication-title: Behav. Brain Funct.
  doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-17
– volume: 123
  start-page: 1
  year: 2005
  ident: ref_33
  article-title: Central Autonomic Integration of Psychological Stressors: Focus on Cardiovascular Modulation
  publication-title: Auton. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2005.09.005
– volume: 4
  start-page: 94
  year: 2020
  ident: ref_4
  article-title: Oxygen Administration and Acute Human Cognitive Enhancement: Higher Cognitive Demand Leads to a More Rapid Decay of Transient Hyperoxia
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Enhanc.
  doi: 10.1007/s41465-019-00145-4
– volume: 36
  start-page: 1413
  year: 1992
  ident: ref_12
  article-title: Multimodal Measures of Mental Workload during Dual-Task Performance: Energetic Demands of Cognitive Processes
  publication-title: Proc. Hum. Factors Soc. Annu. Meet.
  doi: 10.1177/154193129203601811
– volume: 149
  start-page: 89
  year: 1984
  ident: ref_29
  article-title: Anxiety As a Factor in the Incidence of Combat Cold Injury: A Review
  publication-title: Mil. Med.
  doi: 10.1093/milmed/149.2.89
– volume: 15
  start-page: 235
  year: 2018
  ident: ref_31
  article-title: Stress and Heart Rate Variability: A Meta-Analysis and Review of the Literature
  publication-title: Psychiatry Investig.
  doi: 10.30773/pi.2017.08.17
– volume: 89
  start-page: 526
  year: 2018
  ident: ref_7
  article-title: Acute Mild Hypoxic Hypoxia Effects on Cognitive and Simulated Aircraft Pilot Performance
  publication-title: Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform.
  doi: 10.3357/AMHP.5022.2018
SSID ssj0000702636
Score 2.3243809
Snippet Executing flight operations demand that military personnel continuously perform tasks that utilize low- and high-order cognitive functions. The autonomic...
During combat missions, military aircrew rely on their ability to make accurate life-or-death decisions within infinitesimal timeframes. Research shows that...
Simple SummaryDuring combat missions, military aircrew rely on their ability to make accurate life-or-death decisions within infinitesimal timeframes. Research...
SourceID doaj
proquest
gale
pubmed
crossref
SourceType Open Website
Aggregation Database
Index Database
StartPage 343
SubjectTerms air
Altitude
analysis of variance
Anemia
Automation
Autonomic nervous system
Aviation
brain
cognition
Cognitive ability
Cognitive load
Defense industry
Defensive behavior
electrocardiography
Environmental conditions
Environmental quality
Flight
Heart beat
Heart rate
Hypoxia
military
Military aspects
Military personnel
Nervous system
normoxia
Oxygen
Regression analysis
Sea level
sympathetic drive
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  dbid: DOA
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Li9RAEG5kQfAivp11lRIEvYRN-pF0vMVhllHcOYgLewv9CszBZJnN4M7N_-DRf-cvsSqdGXdW0Iu3kHSS7q5Hf0VXf8XYK5tbJwVXiffKJ2iJPDGoGUluZMhUE1QxnOI_XeTzM_nhXJ1fK_VFOWGRHjhO3HFWWF8YwQs0TWl5oZUTDnGIVVpQlXTyvrjmXQumBh9cYGxB-5LE5SMwrj8eOY3QZatUSLG3DA1s_X_65BtIc1hxTu6xuyNUhCp28T67FdoH7HYsHrl5yH7MrsyXobwDIIiD99tqI9A1MN0mBcHHzngwrYfZ7xNt-NFpR1vVpHLQtVCt-3g8GRboOrr1JUQic_gUM2gDLFs4Hfi8VxuoliuEml_fQgXzzUV3tTQ_v31fIPalS6DExM0jdnYy-zydJ2OphcRJnfdJ0Oizhci88HmprDXSc91g8OdckUlXcB1QCE1IDe3UGJk2wnKnQyi18ann4jE7aLs2PGVQll7pMrXCOyUb4UrllDAWYYHVubZqwt5sZ76-iIwaNUYiJKT6hpAm7B1JZteMqLCHG6gg9agg9b8UZMJek1xrMth-ZZwZzx1gb4n6qq6KEp1-JiT27GivJRqa23-81Yx6NPTLWlBJxJJw1YS93D2mNyl5rQ0oMmyTp-jHETj9pQ32HoNFUeJvnkSt2w1baBwURvKH_2M6nrE7HHFZzNk8Ygf9ah2eI67q7YvBhH4B4yUf-A
  priority: 102
  providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals
– databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access
  dbid: M48
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Li9RAEG50RfAiPtdZVylB0Es0SXenO4JIHGYZxZmDOLC30K8sA5rsZjM4c_M_ePTf-UuszmOWWXVvIekkldSjv4-uriLkuU60YTTmgbXcBuiJcaDQMoJEMRfxwnHR7uKfzZPpgn085scX7YD6H3j-T2rn-0kt6q-v1mebd-jwbz3jRMr-ui9XhNGYh5TR6-QGTkvCe-msx_ptWBZIN_xS5f5_7tuZmdoC_n-H6Uvgs52Eju6Q2z16hKxT911yzZX3yM2un-TmPvk1WatvbccHQFwHH4YGJFAVMB7yhOBTpSyo0sLkYpMbPnRc-dVrb4VQlZCtmm7HMswxmlSrc-hqm8PnLqnWwbKEWVviu95AtqwRfX5_AxlMN6fVeql-__g5RzjsD8HnKm4ekMXR5Mt4GvTdFwLDZNIETmIYpzSy1CYp11oxG8sC-aAxImJGxNIJRQsXKr94o1hYUB0b6VwqlQ1tTB-SvbIq3SMCaWq5TENNreGsoCblhlOlESlomUjNR-Tl8Ofz067IRo7kxCspv6SkEXnvNbMd5qtjtyeq-iTvnS2PhLYoWywwnDMdC8kNNYhdNZc0Qvo9Ii-8XnNvVU2tjOq3IqC0vhpWnokU54GIMpTscGck-p7ZvTxYRj6Ybk59l8TUQ60Reba97O_0-WylQ5XhmCTE0I5Y6ooxKD3yR5ria_Y7q9t-NpX4UUjuD64W4DG5FSMI6xI0D8leU6_cEwRRjX7aOscfRFIdTA
  priority: 102
  providerName: Scholars Portal
Title Examining the Influence of Cognitive Load and Environmental Conditions on Autonomic Nervous System Response in Military Aircrew: A Hypoxia–Normoxia Study
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38785825
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3059390819
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3060376613
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3153550395
https://doaj.org/article/17bd7a3271534b2785c3c045b5831132
Volume 13
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV3NattAEF7ahEIvpf91moYpFNqLiKTdlVa9FMU4uKU2JTSQm9g_BR8qubJM4lvfoce-XZ-ks9LKwSnkYoy1skeemW-_2Z2dIeSdSpRmNOaBMdwE6IlxINEygkQyG_HS8rQ7xT-bJ9Nz9uWCX_gFt5VPqxwwsQNqU2u3Rn5MXe-5zE1gn5Y_A9c1yu2u-hYa98k-QrDA4Gv_ZDL_drZdZUGDjhO3P-lq-lC8_9jXNkLo5iFldGc66qr2_4_NtxhnN_OcPiaPPGWEvNfxE3LPVk_Jg76J5OYZ-TO5lj-6Ng-AZA4-D11HoC5hPCQHwddaGpCVgcnNyTb80nHttqyd6UFdQb5u-2PKMEcIqdcr6Auaw1mfSWthUcGsq-vdbCBfNEg5rz5CDtPNsr5eyL-_fs-RA7u34BIUN8_J-enk-3ga-JYLgWYiaQMrELspjQw1ScaVkszEosQgUOs0YjqNhU0lLW0o3Y6NZGFJVayFtZmQJjQxfUH2qrqyrwhkmeEiCxU1mrOS6oxrTqVCeqBEIhQfkQ_DP18s-8oaBUYkTknFLSWNyInTzHaYK4ndfVA3l4X3sCJKlUHZ4hQxnKk4FVxTjYRVcUEjjLlH5L3Ta-Ect22klv78AUrrSmAVeZoh-EeUoWSHOyPR4fTu5cEyCu_wq-LGPEfk7fayu9MlsVUWVYZjkhDxHAnUHWNQegwaaYY_87K3uu1jU4EPhRH9wd0CvCYPY2RefVbmIdlrm7V9g8ypVUfePY66lQd8nTHxD688HJ4
linkProvider ProQuest
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Jb9NAFB5VrRBcEDuBAoMEgotVexZ7jIRQGlIlNIlQ1Uq9ubMF5YAdsqjNjf_Akf_Aj-KX8J6XVClSb71Z9sR-ztu-53kLIW9MbKzgTAbOSReAJrJAg2QEsRY-kmMvk7KKfziKeyfiy6k83SJ_mloYTKtsbGJpqF1h8Rv5HsfZcyk6sE_THwFOjcLd1WaERiUWh351DiHb_GP_M_D3LWMH3eNOL6inCgRWqHgReAXmifPIcRen0hgtHFNjiHOsTSJhE6Z8ovnYhxo3JbQIx9wwq7xPlXahw0YHYPJ3BIdQZpvs7HdHX4_WX3VAgViM-6HYQ4gDvXt1LyVwFTLkgm-4v3JKwP--4ArCLT3dwT1yt4aotF3J1H2y5fMH5FY1tHL1kPzuXujv5VgJCuCR9pspJ7QY006TjEQHhXZU5452Lyvp4KadArfIUdRpkdP2clGVRdMRmKxiOadVA3V6VGXuejrJ6bDsIz5b0fZkBhD3_ANt095qWlxM9N-fv0aAufGQYkLk6hE5uRFmPCbbeZH7p4SmqZMqDQ13Vooxt6m0kmsDcMSoWBnZIu-bfz6bVp08MoiAkEnZFSa1yD5yZr0MW3CXJ4rZt6zW6CxKjAPaWAI-QxiWKGm5BYBspOIRxPgt8g75mqGhWMy01XW9A1CLLbeydpKCs4m4AMp2N1aCgtvNy41kZLWBmWeX6tAir9eX8ZeYNJd7YBmsiUPwHwDYrlkD1EOQylN4zJNK6tavzRW8lGLy2fUEvCK3e8fDQTbojw6fkzsMUF-VEbpLthezpX8BqG1hXtaqQsnZTWvnP7MVWJY
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Jb9NAFB5VqUBcEDspBQYJBBcr9ozHHiMhlKaJEtpGVUWl3sxsRjnUDlnU5sZ_4Mg_4efwS3jPS6oUqbfeInvsPPtt3_O8hZC3OtIm5Ex41grrgSYyT4FkeJEKXSAyJ-Kyiv9oHA1Pwy9n4myL_GlqYTCtsrGJpaG2hcFv5B2Os-cSdGCdrE6LON4ffJ7-8HCCFO60NuM0KhE5cKsLCN_mn0b7wOt3jA36X3tDr54w4JlQRgvPSTBVnAeW2ygRWqvQMplBzGNMHIQmZtLFimfOV7hBoUI_45oZ6VwilfUtNj0A878dQ1Tkt8j2Xn98fLL-wgPHWIR7o9hPiAPtnbqvErgN4fOQb7jCcmLA_37hGtotvd7gAblfw1XareTrIdly-SNypxpguXpMfvcv1Xk5YoICkKSjZuIJLTLaaxKT6GGhLFW5pf2rqjq4aa_A7XIUe1rktLtcVCXSdAzmq1jOadVMnZ5UWbyOTnJ6VPYUn61odzIDuHvxkXbpcDUtLifq789fY8Df-JNicuTqCTm9FWY8Ja28yN1zQpPECpn4mlsjwoybRBjBlQZoomUktWiTD82bT6dVV48UoiFkUnqNSW2yh5xZL8N23OWBYvY9rbU7DWJtgTYWg_8INYulMNwAWNZC8gDi_TZ5j3xN0WgsZsqouvYBqMX2W2k3TsDxBDwEynY3VoKym83TjWSktbGZp1eq0SZv1qfxSkygyx2wDNZEPvgSAG83rAHqIWDlCfzNs0rq1o_NJTyUZGLnZgJek7uglenhaHzwgtxjAACr5NBd0lrMlu4lALiFflVrCiXfbls5_wGNGlzL
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Examining+the+Influence+of+Cognitive+Load+and+Environmental+Conditions+on+Autonomic+Nervous+System+Response+in+Military+Aircrew%3A+A+Hypoxia%E2%80%93Normoxia+Study&rft.jtitle=Biology+%28Basel%2C+Switzerland%29&rft.au=Wittels%2C+Harrison+L&rft.au=S+Howard+Wittels&rft.au=Wishon%2C+Michael+J&rft.au=Vogl%2C+Jonathan&rft.date=2024-05-01&rft.pub=MDPI+AG&rft.eissn=2079-7737&rft.volume=13&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=343&rft_id=info:doi/10.3390%2Fbiology13050343&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=2079-7737&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=2079-7737&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=2079-7737&client=summon