The what, why, and how of born-open data
Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and a...
Saved in:
Published in | Behavior research methods Vol. 48; no. 3; pp. 1062 - 1069 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
New York
Springer US
01.09.2016
Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1554-3528 1554-3528 |
DOI | 10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world’s largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called
born open
. In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world's largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open. In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication. Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world’s largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open . In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication. Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world's largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open. In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication.Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world's largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open. In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication. |
Author | Rouder, Jeffrey N. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Jeffrey N. surname: Rouder fullname: Rouder, Jeffrey N. email: rouderj@missouri.edu organization: University of Missouri |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26428912$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kMtKAzEUQINU7EM_wI0MuOmio7nJPDJLKb6g4KauQybJ2CnTpCZTiv16U6ZKKegmyeKc3MsZop6xRiN0DfiO5im790ATwmIMaYwziuPdGRpAmiYxTQnrHb37aOj9EmPKCCQXqE-y4BVABmg8X-houxDtJJxfk0gYFS3sNrJVVFpnYrvWJlKiFZfovBKN11eHe4Tenx7n05d49vb8On2YxTJhpI1VziDJQeUUZAaqCEMSQilgxQgrK8BlrmlWiEylEgtJqE5KDFQRxUopQdERGnf_rp393Gjf8lXtpW4aYbTdeA4M0ozhjKQBvT1Bl3bjTNguUISyIs8BAnVzoDblSiu-dvVKuC_-0yAA0AHSWe-drn4RwHzfmXedeejM9535Ljj5iSPrVrS1Na0TdfOvSTrThynmQ7ujpf-UvgGcJ42T |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0241496 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11192_023_04684_8 crossref_primary_10_1080_13662716_2020_1792274 crossref_primary_10_5334_jopd_60 crossref_primary_10_1080_03637751_2017_1394581 crossref_primary_10_15626_MP_2018_869 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_019_01259_5 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01713_8 crossref_primary_10_1186_s12874_023_01940_6 crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2315 crossref_primary_10_3390_publications10040039 crossref_primary_10_3390_psych3040053 crossref_primary_10_1525_collabra_158 crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2021_3052025 crossref_primary_10_1177_2515245920928007 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10869_022_09806_2 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_191375 crossref_primary_10_1177_21582440241234985 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0239283 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41393_018_0232_6 crossref_primary_10_1177_2515245918754826 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_023_02137_x crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_021_01776_2 crossref_primary_10_31200_makuubd_899741 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_020_01436_x crossref_primary_10_53841_bpscog_2025_1_10_36 crossref_primary_10_1186_s13104_022_06026_y crossref_primary_10_1177_2515245918801915 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01073_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijresmar_2024_12_005 crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2254 crossref_primary_10_1080_1177083X_2021_1922465 crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_020821_114157 crossref_primary_10_1080_02699931_2018_1429389 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_018_1109_1 crossref_primary_10_3390_mti6090071 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijinfomgt_2019_05_024 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2017_09_002 crossref_primary_10_38124_ijisrt_IJISRT24JUN013 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pdig_0000027 crossref_primary_10_20982_tqmp_19_2_p156 crossref_primary_10_1080_14659891_2018_1489905 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_023_02161_x crossref_primary_10_1525_collabra_28 |
Cites_doi | 10.1177/1745691612459058 10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x 10.1038/485298a 10.1177/1745691612465253 10.1177/0956797613504966 10.1037/a0021524 10.1037/a0019457 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.014 10.1371/journal.pone.0115253 10.1126/science.335.6076.1558 10.1080/08989621.2012.678688 10.1371/journal.pone.0026828 10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.004 doi:10.1037/a0021524 doi:10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x 10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.726 doi:10.1037/a0019457 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2015 Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Sep 2016 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2015 – notice: Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Sep 2016 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 4T- 7TK K9. 7X8 |
DOI | 10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Docstoc Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts Docstoc MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1554-3528 |
EndPage | 1069 |
ExternalDocumentID | 4196628161 26428912 10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z |
Genre | Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S Journal Article Feature |
GroupedDBID | --- -55 -5G -BR -DZ -EM -ET -~C -~X 0-V 06D 0R~ 0VY 199 1N0 203 23N 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 2VQ 30V 3V. 4.4 406 408 40E 53G 5GY 7X7 875 88E 8AO 8FI 8FJ 8G5 8TC 8UJ 95. 96X AAAVM AABHQ AACDK AAHNG AAIAL AAJBT AAJKR AAKPC AANZL AARTL AASML AATNV AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYQN AAYTO AAYZH AAZMS ABAKF ABDZT ABECU ABFTV ABHLI ABIVO ABJNI ABJOX ABJUD ABKCH ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABPPZ ABQBU ABQSL ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTMW ABUWG ABXPI ACAOD ACBXY ACDTI ACGFS ACHQT ACHSB ACHXU ACIWK ACKIV ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACNCT ACOKC ACPIV ACPRK ACZOJ ADBBV ADHHG ADHIR ADINQ ADKNI ADKPE ADURQ ADYFF ADZKW AEBTG AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEKMD AEMSY AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETCA AEVLU AEXYK AFBBN AFFNX AFKRA AFQWF AFRAH AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGJBK AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGRTI AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHAVH AHBYD AHMBA AHSBF AHYZX AIAKS AIGIU AIIXL AILAN AITGF AJBLW AJRNO AJZVZ ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AMYQR AOCGG ARALO ARMRJ ASPBG AVWKF AXYYD AYQZM AZFZN AZQEC B-. BAWUL BENPR BGNMA BPHCQ BVXVI C1A CAG CCPQU COF CSCUP DDRTE DIK DNIVK DPUIP DWQXO E3Z EBD EBLON EBS EIOEI EJD EMOBN ESBYG F5P FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FINBP FNLPD FRRFC FSGXE FYUFA GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNUQQ GNWQR GQ3 GQ6 GQ7 GUQSH H13 HF~ HMCUK HMJXF HRMNR HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IAO IHR IKXTQ INH IPY IRVIT ITC ITM IWAJR J-C JBSCW JZLTJ KOV LLZTM M1P M2M M2O M2R M4Y MVM N2Q N9A NB0 NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O9- O93 O9G O9J OHT OK1 P2P P9L PADUT PF- PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ PT4 R9I RIG ROL RPV RSV S16 S1Z S27 S3B SBS SBU SCLPG SHX SISQX SJYHP SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SV3 SZN T13 TN5 TR2 TSG TUC TUS U2A U9L UG4 UKHRP UOJIU UPT UTJUX UZXMN VFIZW VXZ W48 WH7 WK8 XJT XOL XSW Z7R Z7S Z7W Z81 Z83 Z88 Z8N Z92 ZMTXR ZOVNA ZUP AAPKM AAYXX ABBRH ABDBE ABFSG ACSTC ADHKG AEZWR AFDZB AFHIU AFOHR AGQPQ AHPBZ AHWEU AIXLP ATHPR AYFIA CITATION PHGZM PHGZT CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 4T- 7TK ABRTQ K9. 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-d781471d731c61d9912423310d828bf10b7e369a6d5c0ac23e4b013d2d8bcc1d3 |
IEDL.DBID | AGYKE |
ISSN | 1554-3528 |
IngestDate | Thu Sep 04 16:35:38 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 09:23:42 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:40:52 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:05:51 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:03:30 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 21 02:37:01 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 3 |
Keywords | Open science Data integrity Data sharing Open data |
Language | English |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c482t-d781471d731c61d9912423310d828bf10b7e369a6d5c0ac23e4b013d2d8bcc1d3 |
Notes | SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 14 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z.pdf |
PMID | 26428912 |
PQID | 1823897711 |
PQPubID | 976348 |
PageCount | 8 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_1815680625 proquest_journals_1823897711 pubmed_primary_26428912 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z crossref_citationtrail_10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z springer_journals_10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 20160900 2016-9-00 2016-09-00 20160901 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 9 year: 2016 text: 20160900 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | New York |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: New York – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Behavior research methods |
PublicationTitleAbbrev | Behav Res |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Behav Res Methods |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | Springer US Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer US – name: Springer Nature B.V |
References | Klein, Van de Sompel, Sanderson, Shankar, Balakireva, Zhou, Tobin (CR5) 2014; 9 Morey, Romeijn, Rouder (CR8) 2013; 66 Nosek, Spies, Motyl (CR9) 2012; 7 CR4 Storm, Tressoldi, Di Risio (CR13) 2010; 136 CR7 CR18 CR17 CR16 CR15 Cumming (CR3) 2014; 25 CR12 CR11 Yong (CR19) 2012; 485 Pashler, Wagenmakers (CR10) 2012; 7 Carpenter (CR2) 2012; 335 Bem (CR1) 2011; 100 Vines (CR14) 2014; 24 Krawczyk, Reuben (CR6) 2012; 19 630_CR16 M Krawczyk (630_CR6) 2012; 19 630_CR17 630_CR18 RD Morey (630_CR8) 2013; 66 DJ Bem (630_CR1) 2011; 100 S Carpenter (630_CR2) 2012; 335 M Klein (630_CR5) 2014; 9 BA Nosek (630_CR9) 2012; 7 E Yong (630_CR19) 2012; 485 630_CR4 630_CR7 G Cumming (630_CR3) 2014; 25 630_CR11 630_CR12 L Storm (630_CR13) 2010; 136 THea Vines (630_CR14) 2014; 24 H Pashler (630_CR10) 2012; 7 630_CR15 |
References_xml | – ident: CR18 – ident: CR4 – volume: 7 start-page: 615 year: 2012 end-page: 631 ident: CR9 article-title: Scientific utopia: II. Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/1745691612459058 – ident: CR15 – volume: 66 start-page: 68 year: 2013 end-page: 75 ident: CR8 article-title: The humble Bayesian: Model checking from a fully Bayesian perspective publication-title: British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x – ident: CR16 – volume: 485 start-page: 298 year: 2012 end-page: 300 ident: CR19 article-title: Replication studies: Bad copy publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/485298a – ident: CR12 – ident: CR17 – ident: CR11 – volume: 7 start-page: 528 year: 2012 end-page: 530 ident: CR10 article-title: Editors’ introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: A crisis of confidence? publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/1745691612465253 – volume: 25 start-page: 7 year: 2014 end-page: 29 ident: CR3 article-title: The new statistics: Why and how publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966 – volume: 100 start-page: 407 year: 2011 end-page: 425 ident: CR1 article-title: Feeling the future: experimental evidence for anomalous retroactive influences on cognition and affect publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology doi: 10.1037/a0021524 – ident: CR7 – volume: 136 start-page: 471 year: 2010 end-page: 485 ident: CR13 article-title: Meta-analysis of free-response studies, 1992-2008: Assessing the noise reduction model in parapsychology publication-title: Psychological Bulletin doi: 10.1037/a0019457 – volume: 24 start-page: 94 year: 2014 end-page: 97 ident: CR14 article-title: The availability of research data declines rapidly with article age publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.014 – volume: 9 start-page: e115253 year: 2014 ident: CR5 article-title: Scholarly context not found: One in five articles suffers from reference rot publication-title: PLoS One doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115253 – volume: 19 start-page: 175 year: 2012 end-page: 186 ident: CR6 article-title: (Un) available upon request: field experiment on researchers’ willingness to share supplementary materials publication-title: Accountability in Research – volume: 335 start-page: 1558 year: 2012 end-page: 1561 ident: CR2 article-title: Psychology’s bold initiative publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.335.6076.1558 – ident: 630_CR4 – volume: 19 start-page: 175 year: 2012 ident: 630_CR6 publication-title: Accountability in Research doi: 10.1080/08989621.2012.678688 – volume: 24 start-page: 94 year: 2014 ident: 630_CR14 publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.014 – ident: 630_CR17 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026828 – ident: 630_CR7 – ident: 630_CR15 – volume: 335 start-page: 1558 year: 2012 ident: 630_CR2 publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.335.6076.1558 – volume: 7 start-page: 615 year: 2012 ident: 630_CR9 publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/1745691612459058 – ident: 630_CR16 doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.004 – volume: 100 start-page: 407 year: 2011 ident: 630_CR1 publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology doi: doi:10.1037/a0021524 – volume: 25 start-page: 7 year: 2014 ident: 630_CR3 publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966 – volume: 7 start-page: 528 year: 2012 ident: 630_CR10 publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/1745691612465253 – volume: 66 start-page: 68 year: 2013 ident: 630_CR8 publication-title: British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology doi: doi:10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x – ident: 630_CR18 doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.726 – volume: 136 start-page: 471 year: 2010 ident: 630_CR13 publication-title: Psychological Bulletin doi: doi:10.1037/a0019457 – ident: 630_CR11 – ident: 630_CR12 – volume: 485 start-page: 298 year: 2012 ident: 630_CR19 publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/485298a – volume: 9 start-page: e115253 year: 2014 ident: 630_CR5 publication-title: PLoS One doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115253 |
SSID | ssj0038214 |
Score | 2.3717966 |
Snippet | Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref springer |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 1062 |
SubjectTerms | Academic publications Behavioral Science and Psychology Behavioral sciences Cognitive Psychology Data Interpretation, Statistical Databases, Factual Information Dissemination Internet Open access Psychology Publishing Research Personnel Researchers Scientific Misconduct - statistics & numerical data Software |
Title | The what, why, and how of born-open data |
URI | https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26428912 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1823897711 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1815680625 |
Volume | 48 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3dT8IwEL8IvPDi9weKZCY-GKW4tvsoj8SARKNPkODTsrVbSDSbkRECf71XthEVNeFle9h17e6u19_trleAS0fZwuEuI21cv4mufkeEY1OimM3dyFQBX-5be3p2-kPrYWSP8n3ckyLbvQhJLi219isR1N5OKLd0NWVqE10niixKULGpaIsyVDr3L4_dwgBzwaiVBTB_b_h9CVrDlWsx0eVS09uBQTHILMPktTVNg5Zc_KjfuOFX7MJ2Dj2NTqYre7AVxvtQXVnA-QFcodIYs7GfNvE6bxp-rIxxMjOSyEBViYk-asvQOaWHMOx1B3d9kh-lQKQlWEqUrmzlUuVyKh2qEBRqHIVyUOhxBRE1AzfkTttH0UnTl4yHlv5BqpgSgZRU8SMox0kcnoCBVsC3A8rwLi1mhW0_ElbkShZEgkYmrYFZcNeTeZ1xfdzFm4f-huaBl_HAQx54mgfeogbXqybvWZGN_4jrhci8fL5NPPSSEHm5LsXuL1aPcabo8Icfh8lU06CvKkx0-GpwnIl61RvTbhgypQY3hdi-vPyvoZxuRH0GVURbTpagVody-jENzxHRpEEj1-AGlIas8wlyqOoe |
linkProvider | Springer Nature |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LS8NAEB60HvQivo1WjeBBtIvZzWPTYxFL1banFnpbkt2EHiQRm1LaX-9sXihVwUsCySYbvtnH92V2ZwBuPOX6ns0ZaeP8TXT0O-J7LiWKuTaPLRXa-b61wdDrjZ2XiTsp93HPqtXulUsyH6m1rkRS-zCjtqOjKVOX6DhRZLUJW8gFfJ22YMw61fCLF6hTuC9_fuz7BLTGKtc8ovlE092D3ZIhmp3CpPuwESUHsFMPVMtDuEXbmotpkLXwuGyZQaLMabow09hEiyZEZ8Qy9dLPIxh3n0aPPVJmPCDS8VlGlA5AxaniNpUeVcjdNN1BuBQKozCmVsgj22sHiLC0AsnsyNH_MRVTfiglVfYxNJI0iU7BxM4auCFleJYOc6J2EPtOzCULY5_GFjXAqmAQsgwHrrNSvAmUBRo5USAnEDmhkRMrA-7qR96LWBh_FW5W2IqyW8wEihkkSJxTrP66vo0NWnspgiRK57oMSkrfQl1mwElhk7o2ptUSgmLAfWWkLy__7VPO_lX6CrZ7o0Ff9J-Hr-ewgwTJK9aUNaGRfcyjCyQhWXiZN7pPyH_PAQ |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LS8NAEB60gngR31arRvAg6mJ2N68ei1rqq3iw0NuS7CZ4kKTYlNL-emeapChVwUtyyG42fLOPbzK73wCcecYNPOkL1sT1m5H6HQs8lzMjXOkntonk7Nzac9fr9JyHvtsv85wOq93uVUiyONNAKk1pfj0wCQ1xiQT3esilQ8rK3GWkGcWmy7CCszGnjt4TrWoqloHgThHK_Lna98VogWEuREdni057A9ZLtmi1CvNuwlKcbsHafNKabMM52tkav4X5FV4nV1aYGustG1tZYqF1U0bZsSzaBroDvfbd602HldkPmHYCkTNDYlQ-N77k2uMGeRxRH4TOoJMUJdyO_Fh6zRDR1naohYwd-qdphAkirbmRu1BLszTeBwsHbuhGXOBdO8KJm2ESOImvRZQEPLF5HewKBqVLaXDKUPGu0EUg5FSBnELkFCGnpnW4mFcZFLoYfxVuVNiqcogMFTo2SJZ8n2Pzp_PH2LkpYhGmcTaiMuheBjb6aHXYK2wyb02Q54Sg1OGyMtKXl__2KQf_Kn0Cqy-3bfV03308hDXkSl6xvawBtfxjFB8hH8mj41mf-wQL1NM9 |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+what%2C+why%2C+and+how+of+born-open+data&rft.jtitle=Behavior+research+methods&rft.au=Rouder%2C+Jeffrey+N&rft.date=2016-09-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.eissn=1554-3528&rft.volume=48&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=1062&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13428-015-0630-z&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK&rft.externalDocID=4196628161 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon |