The what, why, and how of born-open data

Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and a...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inBehavior research methods Vol. 48; no. 3; pp. 1062 - 1069
Main Author Rouder, Jeffrey N.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published New York Springer US 01.09.2016
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1554-3528
1554-3528
DOI10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world’s largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open . In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication.
AbstractList Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world's largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open. In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication.
Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world’s largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open . In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication.
Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world's largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open. In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication.Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not available on demand. There are many reasons researchers do not open their data, and one is technical. It is often time consuming to prepare and archive data. In response, my laboratory has automated the process such that our data are archived the night they are created without any human approval or action. All data are versioned, logged, time stamped, and uploaded including aborted runs and data from pilot subjects. The archive is GitHub, github.com, the world's largest collection of open-source materials. Data archived in this manner are called born open. In this paper, I discuss the benefits of born-open data and provide a brief technical overview of the process. I also address some of the common concerns about opening data before publication.
Author Rouder, Jeffrey N.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Jeffrey N.
  surname: Rouder
  fullname: Rouder, Jeffrey N.
  email: rouderj@missouri.edu
  organization: University of Missouri
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26428912$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kMtKAzEUQINU7EM_wI0MuOmio7nJPDJLKb6g4KauQybJ2CnTpCZTiv16U6ZKKegmyeKc3MsZop6xRiN0DfiO5im790ATwmIMaYwziuPdGRpAmiYxTQnrHb37aOj9EmPKCCQXqE-y4BVABmg8X-houxDtJJxfk0gYFS3sNrJVVFpnYrvWJlKiFZfovBKN11eHe4Tenx7n05d49vb8On2YxTJhpI1VziDJQeUUZAaqCEMSQilgxQgrK8BlrmlWiEylEgtJqE5KDFQRxUopQdERGnf_rp393Gjf8lXtpW4aYbTdeA4M0ozhjKQBvT1Bl3bjTNguUISyIs8BAnVzoDblSiu-dvVKuC_-0yAA0AHSWe-drn4RwHzfmXedeejM9535Ljj5iSPrVrS1Na0TdfOvSTrThynmQ7ujpf-UvgGcJ42T
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0241496
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11192_023_04684_8
crossref_primary_10_1080_13662716_2020_1792274
crossref_primary_10_5334_jopd_60
crossref_primary_10_1080_03637751_2017_1394581
crossref_primary_10_15626_MP_2018_869
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_019_01259_5
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01713_8
crossref_primary_10_1186_s12874_023_01940_6
crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2315
crossref_primary_10_3390_publications10040039
crossref_primary_10_3390_psych3040053
crossref_primary_10_1525_collabra_158
crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2021_3052025
crossref_primary_10_1177_2515245920928007
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10869_022_09806_2
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_191375
crossref_primary_10_1177_21582440241234985
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0239283
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41393_018_0232_6
crossref_primary_10_1177_2515245918754826
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_023_02137_x
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_021_01776_2
crossref_primary_10_31200_makuubd_899741
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_020_01436_x
crossref_primary_10_53841_bpscog_2025_1_10_36
crossref_primary_10_1186_s13104_022_06026_y
crossref_primary_10_1177_2515245918801915
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_020_01073_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijresmar_2024_12_005
crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2254
crossref_primary_10_1080_1177083X_2021_1922465
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_psych_020821_114157
crossref_primary_10_1080_02699931_2018_1429389
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_018_1109_1
crossref_primary_10_3390_mti6090071
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ijinfomgt_2019_05_024
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmp_2017_09_002
crossref_primary_10_38124_ijisrt_IJISRT24JUN013
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pdig_0000027
crossref_primary_10_20982_tqmp_19_2_p156
crossref_primary_10_1080_14659891_2018_1489905
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_023_02161_x
crossref_primary_10_1525_collabra_28
Cites_doi 10.1177/1745691612459058
10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x
10.1038/485298a
10.1177/1745691612465253
10.1177/0956797613504966
10.1037/a0021524
10.1037/a0019457
10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.014
10.1371/journal.pone.0115253
10.1126/science.335.6076.1558
10.1080/08989621.2012.678688
10.1371/journal.pone.0026828
10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.004
doi:10.1037/a0021524
doi:10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x
10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.726
doi:10.1037/a0019457
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2015
Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Sep 2016
Copyright_xml – notice: Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2015
– notice: Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Sep 2016
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
4T-
7TK
K9.
7X8
DOI 10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Docstoc
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Neurosciences Abstracts
Docstoc
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE

ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1554-3528
EndPage 1069
ExternalDocumentID 4196628161
26428912
10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Journal Article
Feature
GroupedDBID ---
-55
-5G
-BR
-DZ
-EM
-ET
-~C
-~X
0-V
06D
0R~
0VY
199
1N0
203
23N
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2VQ
30V
3V.
4.4
406
408
40E
53G
5GY
7X7
875
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8G5
8TC
8UJ
95.
96X
AAAVM
AABHQ
AACDK
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AAKPC
AANZL
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
AAZMS
ABAKF
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABJUD
ABKCH
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABPPZ
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABUWG
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACBXY
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIWK
ACKIV
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACPIV
ACPRK
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETCA
AEVLU
AEXYK
AFBBN
AFFNX
AFKRA
AFQWF
AFRAH
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHMBA
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARALO
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
AZQEC
B-.
BAWUL
BENPR
BGNMA
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C1A
CAG
CCPQU
COF
CSCUP
DDRTE
DIK
DNIVK
DPUIP
DWQXO
E3Z
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMOBN
ESBYG
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FYUFA
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ3
GQ6
GQ7
GUQSH
H13
HF~
HMCUK
HMJXF
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
IAO
IHR
IKXTQ
INH
IPY
IRVIT
ITC
ITM
IWAJR
J-C
JBSCW
JZLTJ
KOV
LLZTM
M1P
M2M
M2O
M2R
M4Y
MVM
N2Q
N9A
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9G
O9J
OHT
OK1
P2P
P9L
PADUT
PF-
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT4
R9I
RIG
ROL
RPV
RSV
S16
S1Z
S27
S3B
SBS
SBU
SCLPG
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TN5
TR2
TSG
TUC
TUS
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UOJIU
UPT
UTJUX
UZXMN
VFIZW
VXZ
W48
WH7
WK8
XJT
XOL
XSW
Z7R
Z7S
Z7W
Z81
Z83
Z88
Z8N
Z92
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
ZUP
AAPKM
AAYXX
ABBRH
ABDBE
ABFSG
ACSTC
ADHKG
AEZWR
AFDZB
AFHIU
AFOHR
AGQPQ
AHPBZ
AHWEU
AIXLP
ATHPR
AYFIA
CITATION
PHGZM
PHGZT
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
4T-
7TK
ABRTQ
K9.
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-d781471d731c61d9912423310d828bf10b7e369a6d5c0ac23e4b013d2d8bcc1d3
IEDL.DBID AGYKE
ISSN 1554-3528
IngestDate Thu Sep 04 16:35:38 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 09:23:42 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:40:52 EST 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:05:51 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:03:30 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 21 02:37:01 EST 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 3
Keywords Open science
Data integrity
Data sharing
Open data
Language English
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c482t-d781471d731c61d9912423310d828bf10b7e369a6d5c0ac23e4b013d2d8bcc1d3
Notes SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z.pdf
PMID 26428912
PQID 1823897711
PQPubID 976348
PageCount 8
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_1815680625
proquest_journals_1823897711
pubmed_primary_26428912
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z
crossref_citationtrail_10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z
springer_journals_10_3758_s13428_015_0630_z
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 20160900
2016-9-00
2016-09-00
20160901
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2016-09-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 9
  year: 2016
  text: 20160900
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace New York
PublicationPlace_xml – name: New York
– name: United States
PublicationTitle Behavior research methods
PublicationTitleAbbrev Behav Res
PublicationTitleAlternate Behav Res Methods
PublicationYear 2016
Publisher Springer US
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer US
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References Klein, Van de Sompel, Sanderson, Shankar, Balakireva, Zhou, Tobin (CR5) 2014; 9
Morey, Romeijn, Rouder (CR8) 2013; 66
Nosek, Spies, Motyl (CR9) 2012; 7
CR4
Storm, Tressoldi, Di Risio (CR13) 2010; 136
CR7
CR18
CR17
CR16
CR15
Cumming (CR3) 2014; 25
CR12
CR11
Yong (CR19) 2012; 485
Pashler, Wagenmakers (CR10) 2012; 7
Carpenter (CR2) 2012; 335
Bem (CR1) 2011; 100
Vines (CR14) 2014; 24
Krawczyk, Reuben (CR6) 2012; 19
630_CR16
M Krawczyk (630_CR6) 2012; 19
630_CR17
630_CR18
RD Morey (630_CR8) 2013; 66
DJ Bem (630_CR1) 2011; 100
S Carpenter (630_CR2) 2012; 335
M Klein (630_CR5) 2014; 9
BA Nosek (630_CR9) 2012; 7
E Yong (630_CR19) 2012; 485
630_CR4
630_CR7
G Cumming (630_CR3) 2014; 25
630_CR11
630_CR12
L Storm (630_CR13) 2010; 136
THea Vines (630_CR14) 2014; 24
H Pashler (630_CR10) 2012; 7
630_CR15
References_xml – ident: CR18
– ident: CR4
– volume: 7
  start-page: 615
  year: 2012
  end-page: 631
  ident: CR9
  article-title: Scientific utopia: II. Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability
  publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/1745691612459058
– ident: CR15
– volume: 66
  start-page: 68
  year: 2013
  end-page: 75
  ident: CR8
  article-title: The humble Bayesian: Model checking from a fully Bayesian perspective
  publication-title: British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology
  doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x
– ident: CR16
– volume: 485
  start-page: 298
  year: 2012
  end-page: 300
  ident: CR19
  article-title: Replication studies: Bad copy
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/485298a
– ident: CR12
– ident: CR17
– ident: CR11
– volume: 7
  start-page: 528
  year: 2012
  end-page: 530
  ident: CR10
  article-title: Editors’ introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: A crisis of confidence?
  publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/1745691612465253
– volume: 25
  start-page: 7
  year: 2014
  end-page: 29
  ident: CR3
  article-title: The new statistics: Why and how
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966
– volume: 100
  start-page: 407
  year: 2011
  end-page: 425
  ident: CR1
  article-title: Feeling the future: experimental evidence for anomalous retroactive influences on cognition and affect
  publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/a0021524
– ident: CR7
– volume: 136
  start-page: 471
  year: 2010
  end-page: 485
  ident: CR13
  article-title: Meta-analysis of free-response studies, 1992-2008: Assessing the noise reduction model in parapsychology
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
  doi: 10.1037/a0019457
– volume: 24
  start-page: 94
  year: 2014
  end-page: 97
  ident: CR14
  article-title: The availability of research data declines rapidly with article age
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.014
– volume: 9
  start-page: e115253
  year: 2014
  ident: CR5
  article-title: Scholarly context not found: One in five articles suffers from reference rot
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115253
– volume: 19
  start-page: 175
  year: 2012
  end-page: 186
  ident: CR6
  article-title: (Un) available upon request: field experiment on researchers’ willingness to share supplementary materials
  publication-title: Accountability in Research
– volume: 335
  start-page: 1558
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1561
  ident: CR2
  article-title: Psychology’s bold initiative
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.335.6076.1558
– ident: 630_CR4
– volume: 19
  start-page: 175
  year: 2012
  ident: 630_CR6
  publication-title: Accountability in Research
  doi: 10.1080/08989621.2012.678688
– volume: 24
  start-page: 94
  year: 2014
  ident: 630_CR14
  publication-title: Current Biology
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.014
– ident: 630_CR17
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026828
– ident: 630_CR7
– ident: 630_CR15
– volume: 335
  start-page: 1558
  year: 2012
  ident: 630_CR2
  publication-title: Science
  doi: 10.1126/science.335.6076.1558
– volume: 7
  start-page: 615
  year: 2012
  ident: 630_CR9
  publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/1745691612459058
– ident: 630_CR16
  doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.004
– volume: 100
  start-page: 407
  year: 2011
  ident: 630_CR1
  publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
  doi: doi:10.1037/a0021524
– volume: 25
  start-page: 7
  year: 2014
  ident: 630_CR3
  publication-title: Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966
– volume: 7
  start-page: 528
  year: 2012
  ident: 630_CR10
  publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science
  doi: 10.1177/1745691612465253
– volume: 66
  start-page: 68
  year: 2013
  ident: 630_CR8
  publication-title: British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology
  doi: doi:10.1111/j.2044-8317.2012.02067.x
– ident: 630_CR18
  doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.726
– volume: 136
  start-page: 471
  year: 2010
  ident: 630_CR13
  publication-title: Psychological Bulletin
  doi: doi:10.1037/a0019457
– ident: 630_CR11
– ident: 630_CR12
– volume: 485
  start-page: 298
  year: 2012
  ident: 630_CR19
  publication-title: Nature
  doi: 10.1038/485298a
– volume: 9
  start-page: e115253
  year: 2014
  ident: 630_CR5
  publication-title: PLoS One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115253
SSID ssj0038214
Score 2.3717966
Snippet Although many researchers agree that scientific data should be open to scrutiny to ferret out poor analyses and outright fraud, most raw data sets are not...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
springer
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 1062
SubjectTerms Academic publications
Behavioral Science and Psychology
Behavioral sciences
Cognitive Psychology
Data Interpretation, Statistical
Databases, Factual
Information Dissemination
Internet
Open access
Psychology
Publishing
Research Personnel
Researchers
Scientific Misconduct - statistics & numerical data
Software
Title The what, why, and how of born-open data
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13428-015-0630-z
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26428912
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1823897711
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1815680625
Volume 48
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV3dT8IwEL8IvPDi9weKZCY-GKW4tvsoj8SARKNPkODTsrVbSDSbkRECf71XthEVNeFle9h17e6u19_trleAS0fZwuEuI21cv4mufkeEY1OimM3dyFQBX-5be3p2-kPrYWSP8n3ckyLbvQhJLi219isR1N5OKLd0NWVqE10niixKULGpaIsyVDr3L4_dwgBzwaiVBTB_b_h9CVrDlWsx0eVS09uBQTHILMPktTVNg5Zc_KjfuOFX7MJ2Dj2NTqYre7AVxvtQXVnA-QFcodIYs7GfNvE6bxp-rIxxMjOSyEBViYk-asvQOaWHMOx1B3d9kh-lQKQlWEqUrmzlUuVyKh2qEBRqHIVyUOhxBRE1AzfkTttH0UnTl4yHlv5BqpgSgZRU8SMox0kcnoCBVsC3A8rwLi1mhW0_ElbkShZEgkYmrYFZcNeTeZ1xfdzFm4f-huaBl_HAQx54mgfeogbXqybvWZGN_4jrhci8fL5NPPSSEHm5LsXuL1aPcabo8Icfh8lU06CvKkx0-GpwnIl61RvTbhgypQY3hdi-vPyvoZxuRH0GVURbTpagVody-jENzxHRpEEj1-AGlIas8wlyqOoe
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LS8NAEB60HvQivo1WjeBBtIvZzWPTYxFL1banFnpbkt2EHiQRm1LaX-9sXihVwUsCySYbvtnH92V2ZwBuPOX6ns0ZaeP8TXT0O-J7LiWKuTaPLRXa-b61wdDrjZ2XiTsp93HPqtXulUsyH6m1rkRS-zCjtqOjKVOX6DhRZLUJW8gFfJ22YMw61fCLF6hTuC9_fuz7BLTGKtc8ovlE092D3ZIhmp3CpPuwESUHsFMPVMtDuEXbmotpkLXwuGyZQaLMabow09hEiyZEZ8Qy9dLPIxh3n0aPPVJmPCDS8VlGlA5AxaniNpUeVcjdNN1BuBQKozCmVsgj22sHiLC0AsnsyNH_MRVTfiglVfYxNJI0iU7BxM4auCFleJYOc6J2EPtOzCULY5_GFjXAqmAQsgwHrrNSvAmUBRo5USAnEDmhkRMrA-7qR96LWBh_FW5W2IqyW8wEihkkSJxTrP66vo0NWnspgiRK57oMSkrfQl1mwElhk7o2ptUSgmLAfWWkLy__7VPO_lX6CrZ7o0Ff9J-Hr-ewgwTJK9aUNaGRfcyjCyQhWXiZN7pPyH_PAQ
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1LS8NAEB60gngR31arRvAg6mJ2N68ei1rqq3iw0NuS7CZ4kKTYlNL-emeapChVwUtyyG42fLOPbzK73wCcecYNPOkL1sT1m5H6HQs8lzMjXOkntonk7Nzac9fr9JyHvtsv85wOq93uVUiyONNAKk1pfj0wCQ1xiQT3esilQ8rK3GWkGcWmy7CCszGnjt4TrWoqloHgThHK_Lna98VogWEuREdni057A9ZLtmi1CvNuwlKcbsHafNKabMM52tkav4X5FV4nV1aYGustG1tZYqF1U0bZsSzaBroDvfbd602HldkPmHYCkTNDYlQ-N77k2uMGeRxRH4TOoJMUJdyO_Fh6zRDR1naohYwd-qdphAkirbmRu1BLszTeBwsHbuhGXOBdO8KJm2ESOImvRZQEPLF5HewKBqVLaXDKUPGu0EUg5FSBnELkFCGnpnW4mFcZFLoYfxVuVNiqcogMFTo2SJZ8n2Pzp_PH2LkpYhGmcTaiMuheBjb6aHXYK2wyb02Q54Sg1OGyMtKXl__2KQf_Kn0Cqy-3bfV03308hDXkSl6xvawBtfxjFB8hH8mj41mf-wQL1NM9
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+what%2C+why%2C+and+how+of+born-open+data&rft.jtitle=Behavior+research+methods&rft.au=Rouder%2C+Jeffrey+N&rft.date=2016-09-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.eissn=1554-3528&rft.volume=48&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=1062&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758%2Fs13428-015-0630-z&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK&rft.externalDocID=4196628161
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1554-3528&client=summon