Genetic Factors and Orofacial Motor Learning Selectively Influence Variability in Central Sulcus Morphology in Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes )
Captive chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by...
Saved in:
Published in | The Journal of neuroscience Vol. 37; no. 22; pp. 5475 - 5483 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Society for Neuroscience
31.05.2017
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Captive chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by chimpanzees and, here, we examined whether changes in cortical organization of the central sulcus (CS) were associated with AG sound production. MRI scans were collected from 240 chimpanzees, including 122 that reliably produced AG sounds and 118 that did not. For each subject, the depth of CS was quantified along the superior–inferior plane with specific interest in the inferior portion corresponding to the region of the motor cortex where the mouth and orofacial movements are controlled. Results indicated that CS depth in the inferior, but not superior, portion was significantly greater in chimpanzees that reliably produced AG sounds compared with those who did not. Quantitative genetic analyses indicated that overall CS surface area and depth were significantly heritable, particularly in the superior regions, but less so in the inferior and central portions. Further, heritability in CS depth was altered as a function of acquisition of AG sounds. The collective results suggest that learning to produce AG sounds resulted in region-specific cortical reorganization within the inferior portion of the CS, a finding previously undocumented in chimpanzees or any nonhuman primate.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Recent studies in chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) have shown that some can learn to produce novel sounds by configuring different orofacial movement patterns and these sounds are used in communicatively relevant contexts. Here, we examined the neuromorphological correlates in the production of these sounds in chimpanzees. We show that chimpanzees that have learned to produce these sounds show significant differences in central sulcus (CS) morphology, particularly in the inferior region. We further show that overall CS morphology and regions within the superior portion are significantly heritable, whereas central and inferior portions of the CS are not. The collective findings suggest chimpanzees exhibit cortical plasticity in regions of the brain that were central to the emergence of speech functions in humans. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Captive chimpanzees (
Pan troglodytes
) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by chimpanzees and, here, we examined whether changes in cortical organization of the central sulcus (CS) were associated with AG sound production. MRI scans were collected from 240 chimpanzees, including 122 that reliably produced AG sounds and 118 that did not. For each subject, the depth of CS was quantified along the superior–inferior plane with specific interest in the inferior portion corresponding to the region of the motor cortex where the mouth and orofacial movements are controlled. Results indicated that CS depth in the inferior, but not superior, portion was significantly greater in chimpanzees that reliably produced AG sounds compared with those who did not. Quantitative genetic analyses indicated that overall CS surface area and depth were significantly heritable, particularly in the superior regions, but less so in the inferior and central portions. Further, heritability in CS depth was altered as a function of acquisition of AG sounds. The collective results suggest that learning to produce AG sounds resulted in region-specific cortical reorganization within the inferior portion of the CS, a finding previously undocumented in chimpanzees or any nonhuman primate.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT
Recent studies in chimpanzees (
Pan troglodytes
) have shown that some can learn to produce novel sounds by configuring different orofacial movement patterns and these sounds are used in communicatively relevant contexts. Here, we examined the neuromorphological correlates in the production of these sounds in chimpanzees. We show that chimpanzees that have learned to produce these sounds show significant differences in central sulcus (CS) morphology, particularly in the inferior region. We further show that overall CS morphology and regions within the superior portion are significantly heritable, whereas central and inferior portions of the CS are not. The collective findings suggest chimpanzees exhibit cortical plasticity in regions of the brain that were central to the emergence of speech functions in humans. Captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by chimpanzees and, here, we examined whether changes in cortical organization of the central sulcus (CS) were associated with AG sound production. MRI scans were collected from 240 chimpanzees, including 122 that reliably produced AG sounds and 118 that did not. For each subject, the depth of CS was quantified along the superior-inferior plane with specific interest in the inferior portion corresponding to the region of the motor cortex where the mouth and orofacial movements are controlled. Results indicated that CS depth in the inferior, but not superior, portion was significantly greater in chimpanzees that reliably produced AG sounds compared with those who did not. Quantitative genetic analyses indicated that overall CS surface area and depth were significantly heritable, particularly in the superior regions, but less so in the inferior and central portions. Further, heritability in CS depth was altered as a function of acquisition of AG sounds. The collective results suggest that learning to produce AG sounds resulted in region-specific cortical reorganization within the inferior portion of the CS, a finding previously undocumented in chimpanzees or any nonhuman primate. Captive chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by chimpanzees and, here, we examined whether changes in cortical organization of the central sulcus (CS) were associated with AG sound production. MRI scans were collected from 240 chimpanzees, including 122 that reliably produced AG sounds and 118 that did not. For each subject, the depth of CS was quantified along the superior–inferior plane with specific interest in the inferior portion corresponding to the region of the motor cortex where the mouth and orofacial movements are controlled. Results indicated that CS depth in the inferior, but not superior, portion was significantly greater in chimpanzees that reliably produced AG sounds compared with those who did not. Quantitative genetic analyses indicated that overall CS surface area and depth were significantly heritable, particularly in the superior regions, but less so in the inferior and central portions. Further, heritability in CS depth was altered as a function of acquisition of AG sounds. The collective results suggest that learning to produce AG sounds resulted in region-specific cortical reorganization within the inferior portion of the CS, a finding previously undocumented in chimpanzees or any nonhuman primate. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Recent studies in chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) have shown that some can learn to produce novel sounds by configuring different orofacial movement patterns and these sounds are used in communicatively relevant contexts. Here, we examined the neuromorphological correlates in the production of these sounds in chimpanzees. We show that chimpanzees that have learned to produce these sounds show significant differences in central sulcus (CS) morphology, particularly in the inferior region. We further show that overall CS morphology and regions within the superior portion are significantly heritable, whereas central and inferior portions of the CS are not. The collective findings suggest chimpanzees exhibit cortical plasticity in regions of the brain that were central to the emergence of speech functions in humans. Captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by chimpanzees and, here, we examined whether changes in cortical organization of the central sulcus (CS) were associated with AG sound production. MRI scans were collected from 240 chimpanzees, including 122 that reliably produced AG sounds and 118 that did not. For each subject, the depth of CS was quantified along the superior-inferior plane with specific interest in the inferior portion corresponding to the region of the motor cortex where the mouth and orofacial movements are controlled. Results indicated that CS depth in the inferior, but not superior, portion was significantly greater in chimpanzees that reliably produced AG sounds compared with those who did not. Quantitative genetic analyses indicated that overall CS surface area and depth were significantly heritable, particularly in the superior regions, but less so in the inferior and central portions. Further, heritability in CS depth was altered as a function of acquisition of AG sounds. The collective results suggest that learning to produce AG sounds resulted in region-specific cortical reorganization within the inferior portion of the CS, a finding previously undocumented in chimpanzees or any nonhuman primate.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Recent studies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have shown that some can learn to produce novel sounds by configuring different orofacial movement patterns and these sounds are used in communicatively relevant contexts. Here, we examined the neuromorphological correlates in the production of these sounds in chimpanzees. We show that chimpanzees that have learned to produce these sounds show significant differences in central sulcus (CS) morphology, particularly in the inferior region. We further show that overall CS morphology and regions within the superior portion are significantly heritable, whereas central and inferior portions of the CS are not. The collective findings suggest chimpanzees exhibit cortical plasticity in regions of the brain that were central to the emergence of speech functions in humans.Captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by chimpanzees and, here, we examined whether changes in cortical organization of the central sulcus (CS) were associated with AG sound production. MRI scans were collected from 240 chimpanzees, including 122 that reliably produced AG sounds and 118 that did not. For each subject, the depth of CS was quantified along the superior-inferior plane with specific interest in the inferior portion corresponding to the region of the motor cortex where the mouth and orofacial movements are controlled. Results indicated that CS depth in the inferior, but not superior, portion was significantly greater in chimpanzees that reliably produced AG sounds compared with those who did not. Quantitative genetic analyses indicated that overall CS surface area and depth were significantly heritable, particularly in the superior regions, but less so in the inferior and central portions. Further, heritability in CS depth was altered as a function of acquisition of AG sounds. The collective results suggest that learning to produce AG sounds resulted in region-specific cortical reorganization within the inferior portion of the CS, a finding previously undocumented in chimpanzees or any nonhuman primate.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Recent studies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have shown that some can learn to produce novel sounds by configuring different orofacial movement patterns and these sounds are used in communicatively relevant contexts. Here, we examined the neuromorphological correlates in the production of these sounds in chimpanzees. We show that chimpanzees that have learned to produce these sounds show significant differences in central sulcus (CS) morphology, particularly in the inferior region. We further show that overall CS morphology and regions within the superior portion are significantly heritable, whereas central and inferior portions of the CS are not. The collective findings suggest chimpanzees exhibit cortical plasticity in regions of the brain that were central to the emergence of speech functions in humans. Captive chimpanzees ( ) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or drawing their attention to a desired object or food. There are significant individual differences in the use of AG sounds by chimpanzees and, here, we examined whether changes in cortical organization of the central sulcus (CS) were associated with AG sound production. MRI scans were collected from 240 chimpanzees, including 122 that reliably produced AG sounds and 118 that did not. For each subject, the depth of CS was quantified along the superior-inferior plane with specific interest in the inferior portion corresponding to the region of the motor cortex where the mouth and orofacial movements are controlled. Results indicated that CS depth in the inferior, but not superior, portion was significantly greater in chimpanzees that reliably produced AG sounds compared with those who did not. Quantitative genetic analyses indicated that overall CS surface area and depth were significantly heritable, particularly in the superior regions, but less so in the inferior and central portions. Further, heritability in CS depth was altered as a function of acquisition of AG sounds. The collective results suggest that learning to produce AG sounds resulted in region-specific cortical reorganization within the inferior portion of the CS, a finding previously undocumented in chimpanzees or any nonhuman primate. Recent studies in chimpanzees ( ) have shown that some can learn to produce novel sounds by configuring different orofacial movement patterns and these sounds are used in communicatively relevant contexts. Here, we examined the neuromorphological correlates in the production of these sounds in chimpanzees. We show that chimpanzees that have learned to produce these sounds show significant differences in central sulcus (CS) morphology, particularly in the inferior region. We further show that overall CS morphology and regions within the superior portion are significantly heritable, whereas central and inferior portions of the CS are not. The collective findings suggest chimpanzees exhibit cortical plasticity in regions of the brain that were central to the emergence of speech functions in humans. |
Author | Pope, Sarah Mahovetz, Lindsay Coulon, Oliver Meguerditchian, Adrien Schapiro, Steven J. Autrey, Michelle Davidek, Kendall Mareno, Mary Catherine Hopkins, William D. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: William D. surname: Hopkins fullname: Hopkins, William D. – sequence: 2 givenname: Oliver orcidid: 0000-0003-4752-1228 surname: Coulon fullname: Coulon, Oliver – sequence: 3 givenname: Adrien orcidid: 0000-0003-3754-6747 surname: Meguerditchian fullname: Meguerditchian, Adrien – sequence: 4 givenname: Michelle surname: Autrey fullname: Autrey, Michelle – sequence: 5 givenname: Kendall orcidid: 0000-0003-2674-115X surname: Davidek fullname: Davidek, Kendall – sequence: 6 givenname: Lindsay surname: Mahovetz fullname: Mahovetz, Lindsay – sequence: 7 givenname: Sarah surname: Pope fullname: Pope, Sarah – sequence: 8 givenname: Mary Catherine surname: Mareno fullname: Mareno, Mary Catherine – sequence: 9 givenname: Steven J. surname: Schapiro fullname: Schapiro, Steven J. |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28473646$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed https://hal.science/hal-01619542$$DView record in HAL |
BookMark | eNqFUstuEzEUtVARTQu_UFli0y6m-DWejISQqqiPoEAQoWwtj8eTuHLs1PZECh_BN-NR2gqyYWXr3vO4vj4n4Mh5pwE4w-gSl4R--Pz1-v77fDGZXhLOcIH5JUG4egVGuVsXhCF8BEaIVKjgrGLH4CTGB4RQlUFvwDEZs4pyxkfg9612OhkFb6RKPkQoXQvnwXdSGWnhF5-LcKZlcMYt4UJbrZLZaruDU9fZXjul4U8ZjGyMNWkHjYMT7VLI3EVvVR-zRNisvPXLfXNl1hvpfmkd4Tn8Jh1MwS-tb3cpVy7egtedtFG_ezpPwf3N9Y_JXTGb304nV7NCsYqnQtN2rBBi9bjEqmNS8S5fGl0S1ZUdxajhkmvCqVK6ViVrOlpyVammGretJB09BZ_2upu-WetW7UcWm2DWMuyEl0b823FmJZZ-K0qWt0_rLHCxF1gd0O6uZmKoIcxxXTKyxRl7_mQW_GOvYxJrE5W2Vjrt-yjwuOaIYUIH6PsD6IPvg8urEATVlNUVpVVGnf09_Yv_87dmwMc9QAUfY9CdUCbJZPzwGGMFRmJIkXhJkRhSJDAXQ4oynR_Qnx3-Q_wDvjzO6A |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhaa392 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_48760_7 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsbl_2021_0302 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_019_11347_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2025_121038 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae232 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhac183 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2019_01_001 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajpa_24590 crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_023_04953_5 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2020_1320 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_mri_2020_12_015 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41559_022_01689_z crossref_primary_10_1002_bies_201900102 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_55360_y crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_64051_y |
Cites_doi | 10.1093/cercor/bhp074 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.020 10.1075/is.5.2.03lie 10.1371/journal.pone.0018852 10.1075/celcr.12.12lea 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5153-08.2009 10.1038/srep03774 10.1007/978-3-319-02669-5_10 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.3.CO;2-P 10.1159/000362431 10.1073/pnas.1512646112 10.1075/ais.1.05hop 10.1002/hbm.20407 10.1126/science.1145463 10.1007/s100710050041 10.1109/TMI.2004.831204 10.1371/journal.pone.0026822 10.1086/301844 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0113 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09240.2003 10.1371/journal.pone.0028243 10.9734/JSRR/2015/14076 10.1002/hbm.20198 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1616-13.2013 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.069 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4394-09.2010 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01561.x 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.004 10.1109/MMBIA.2000.852374 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.07.002 10.1007/s10071-009-0242-z 10.1371/journal.pone.0002529 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.045 10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.049 10.1086/509092 10.1002/ajp.20304 10.1093/brain/60.4.389 10.1111/1467-7687.00254 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.003 10.1126/science.1093993 10.1007/s10329-015-0497-8 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2017 the authors 0270-6474/17/375476-09$15.00/0. Copyright Society for Neuroscience May 31, 2017 Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License Copyright © 2017 the authors 0270-6474/17/375476-09$15.00/0 2017 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2017 the authors 0270-6474/17/375476-09$15.00/0. – notice: Copyright Society for Neuroscience May 31, 2017 – notice: Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License – notice: Copyright © 2017 the authors 0270-6474/17/375476-09$15.00/0 2017 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QR 7TK 7U7 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 P64 7X8 1XC VOOES 5PM |
DOI | 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2641-16.2017 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Toxicology Abstracts Virology and AIDS Abstracts Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Engineering Research Database AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access) PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Virology and AIDS Abstracts Technology Research Database Toxicology Abstracts Animal Behavior Abstracts AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Engineering Research Database Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic Virology and AIDS Abstracts MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology Psychology |
EISSN | 1529-2401 |
EndPage | 5483 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC5452339 oai_HAL_hal_01619542v1 28473646 10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2641_16_2017 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .55 18M 2WC 34G 39C 53G 5GY 5RE 5VS AAFWJ AAJMC AAYXX ABBAR ABIVO ACGUR ACNCT ADBBV ADCOW ADHGD AENEX AFCFT AFOSN AFSQR AHWXS ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS BAWUL BTFSW CITATION CS3 DIK DU5 E3Z EBS EJD F5P GX1 H13 HYE H~9 KQ8 L7B OK1 P0W P2P QZG R.V RHI RPM TFN TR2 W8F WH7 WOQ X7M YBU YHG YKV YNH YSK CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QR 7TK 7U7 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 P64 7X8 .GJ 1CY 1XC 3O- ADXHL AETEA AFFNX AFHIN AI. MVM RIG VH1 VOOES XJT YYP ZGI ZXP 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c476t-e3d8c0049851cf4ac6f51cbe52cf5f310b6a6e263cce9c54bf356c7cb78dda2f3 |
ISSN | 0270-6474 1529-2401 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:17:09 EDT 2025 Wed Aug 13 07:42:03 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 06:57:49 EDT 2025 Mon Jun 30 16:50:03 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:57:40 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:06:19 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 03 08:43:37 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 22 |
Keywords | chimpanzee language vocal learning central sulcus orofacial movements heritability |
Language | English |
License | https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0 Copyright © 2017 the authors 0270-6474/17/375476-09$15.00/0. Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c476t-e3d8c0049851cf4ac6f51cbe52cf5f310b6a6e263cce9c54bf356c7cb78dda2f3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Author contributions: W.D.H. designed research; W.D.H., A.M., M.A., K.D., L.M., S.P., M.C.M., and S.J.S. performed research; O.C. contributed unpublished reagents/analytic tools; W.D.H. analyzed data; W.D.H., O.C., A.M., K.D., S.P., L.M., M.C.M., and S.J.S. wrote the paper. |
ORCID | 0000-0003-2674-115X 0000-0003-3754-6747 0000-0003-4752-1228 |
OpenAccessLink | https://hal.science/hal-01619542 |
PMID | 28473646 |
PQID | 2093497337 |
PQPubID | 2049535 |
PageCount | 9 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5452339 hal_primary_oai_HAL_hal_01619542v1 proquest_miscellaneous_1896041231 proquest_journals_2093497337 pubmed_primary_28473646 crossref_citationtrail_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2641_16_2017 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2641_16_2017 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2017-05-31 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2017-05-31 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 05 year: 2017 text: 2017-05-31 day: 31 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: Baltimore |
PublicationTitle | The Journal of neuroscience |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Neurosci |
PublicationYear | 2017 |
Publisher | Society for Neuroscience |
Publisher_xml | – name: Society for Neuroscience |
References | 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.26 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.27 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.24 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.46 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.25 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.47 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.22 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.44 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.23 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.45 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.20 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.42 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.21 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.43 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.40 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.41 Gaser (2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.13) 2003; 23 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.28 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.29 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.2 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.15 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.3 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.16 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.38 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.4 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.35 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.14 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.36 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.6 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.11 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.33 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.7 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.12 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.34 Penfield (2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.37) 1936; 60 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.8 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.31 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.9 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.10 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.32 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.30 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.1 Coulon (2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.5) 2006; 31 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.19 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.17 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.39 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.18 24068828 - J Neurosci. 2013 Sep 25;33(39):15618-25 25139259 - Brain Behav Evol. 2014;84(1):19-30 22073201 - PLoS One. 2011;6(11):e26822 20035879 - Neuroimage. 2010 Nov 15;53(3):1126-34 9545414 - Am J Hum Genet. 1998 May;62(5):1198-211 26627234 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015 Dec 1;112(48):14799-804 21533079 - PLoS One. 2011 Apr 20;6(4):e18852 14726578 - Science. 2004 Jan 16;303(5656):318-20 18308569 - Curr Biol. 2008 Mar 11;18(5):343-8 17683939 - Curr Biol. 2007 Aug 7;17(15):1345-8 26293777 - Sci Rep. 2015 Aug 21;5:13460 17389908 - Anim Behav. 2007 Feb;73(2):281-286 16161162 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2005 Nov;26(3):210-20 26552655 - Sci Rep. 2015 Nov 10;5:16442 15943676 - Psychol Sci. 2005 Jun;16(6):487-93 19504272 - Anim Cogn. 2010 Jan;13(1):33-40 26546459 - Primates. 2016 Jan;57(1):61-72 17823343 - Science. 2007 Sep 7;317(5843):1344-7 20176115 - Neuroimage. 2010 Nov 15;53(3):1103-8 14534258 - J Neurosci. 2003 Oct 8;23(27):9240-5 22438489 - Biol Lett. 2012 Aug 23;8(4):498-501 17437285 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2007 Jun;28(6):576-83 15338731 - IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2004 Aug;23(8):968-82 16967515 - Am J Primatol. 2006 Oct;68(10):978-92 18575610 - PLoS One. 2008 Jun 25;3(6):e2529 19433652 - Cereb Cortex. 2010 Jan;20(1):25-33 22205941 - PLoS One. 2011;6(12):e28243 24440967 - Sci Rep. 2014 Jan 20;4:3774 20203186 - J Neurosci. 2010 Mar 3;30(9):3271-5 27055947 - Neuropsychologia. 2016 Dec;93(Pt B):325-334 19261882 - J Neurosci. 2009 Mar 4;29(9):2867-75 22867751 - Brain Lang. 2012 Oct;123(1):75-9 |
References_xml | – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.30 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp074 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.41 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.020 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.31 doi: 10.1075/is.5.2.03lie – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.43 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018852 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.27 doi: 10.1075/celcr.12.12lea – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.9 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5153-08.2009 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.21 doi: 10.1038/srep03774 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.28 doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-02669-5_10 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.7 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5<178::AID-EVAN5>3.3.CO;2-P – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.22 doi: 10.1159/000362431 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.15 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1512646112 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.20 doi: 10.1075/ais.1.05hop – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.40 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20407 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.8 doi: 10.1126/science.1145463 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.45 doi: 10.1007/s100710050041 – volume: 31 start-page: S46 year: 2006 ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.5 article-title: Cortical localization via surface parameterization: a sulcus-based approach publication-title: Neuroimage – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.35 doi: 10.1109/TMI.2004.831204 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.4 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026822 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.1 doi: 10.1086/301844 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.44 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0113 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.19 – volume: 23 start-page: 9240 year: 2003 ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.13 article-title: Brain structures differ between musicians and non-muscians publication-title: J Neurosci doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09240.2003 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.6 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028243 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.14 doi: 10.9734/JSRR/2015/14076 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.25 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20198 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.36 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1616-13.2013 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.3 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.069 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.11 doi: 10.9734/JSRR/2015/14076 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.24 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4394-09.2010 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.2 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.17 doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01561.x – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.18 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.004 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.34 doi: 10.1109/MMBIA.2000.852374 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.46 doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.07.002 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.29 doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0242-z – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.33 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002529 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.26 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.045 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.42 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.049 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.12 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.32 doi: 10.1086/509092 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.38 doi: 10.1002/ajp.20304 – volume: 60 start-page: 389 year: 1936 ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.37 article-title: Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation publication-title: Brain doi: 10.1093/brain/60.4.389 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.16 doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00254 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.23 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.003 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.39 doi: 10.1126/science.1093993 – ident: 2023041803251852000_37.22.5475.47 doi: 10.1007/s10329-015-0497-8 – reference: 26552655 - Sci Rep. 2015 Nov 10;5:16442 – reference: 22073201 - PLoS One. 2011;6(11):e26822 – reference: 26627234 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015 Dec 1;112(48):14799-804 – reference: 24440967 - Sci Rep. 2014 Jan 20;4:3774 – reference: 19261882 - J Neurosci. 2009 Mar 4;29(9):2867-75 – reference: 14534258 - J Neurosci. 2003 Oct 8;23(27):9240-5 – reference: 25139259 - Brain Behav Evol. 2014;84(1):19-30 – reference: 20176115 - Neuroimage. 2010 Nov 15;53(3):1103-8 – reference: 14726578 - Science. 2004 Jan 16;303(5656):318-20 – reference: 27055947 - Neuropsychologia. 2016 Dec;93(Pt B):325-334 – reference: 22205941 - PLoS One. 2011;6(12):e28243 – reference: 16161162 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2005 Nov;26(3):210-20 – reference: 26546459 - Primates. 2016 Jan;57(1):61-72 – reference: 21533079 - PLoS One. 2011 Apr 20;6(4):e18852 – reference: 17437285 - Hum Brain Mapp. 2007 Jun;28(6):576-83 – reference: 19433652 - Cereb Cortex. 2010 Jan;20(1):25-33 – reference: 24068828 - J Neurosci. 2013 Sep 25;33(39):15618-25 – reference: 18308569 - Curr Biol. 2008 Mar 11;18(5):343-8 – reference: 15943676 - Psychol Sci. 2005 Jun;16(6):487-93 – reference: 9545414 - Am J Hum Genet. 1998 May;62(5):1198-211 – reference: 17389908 - Anim Behav. 2007 Feb;73(2):281-286 – reference: 22438489 - Biol Lett. 2012 Aug 23;8(4):498-501 – reference: 22867751 - Brain Lang. 2012 Oct;123(1):75-9 – reference: 18575610 - PLoS One. 2008 Jun 25;3(6):e2529 – reference: 19504272 - Anim Cogn. 2010 Jan;13(1):33-40 – reference: 20203186 - J Neurosci. 2010 Mar 3;30(9):3271-5 – reference: 26293777 - Sci Rep. 2015 Aug 21;5:13460 – reference: 17823343 - Science. 2007 Sep 7;317(5843):1344-7 – reference: 17683939 - Curr Biol. 2007 Aug 7;17(15):1345-8 – reference: 20035879 - Neuroimage. 2010 Nov 15;53(3):1126-34 – reference: 15338731 - IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2004 Aug;23(8):968-82 – reference: 16967515 - Am J Primatol. 2006 Oct;68(10):978-92 |
SSID | ssj0007017 |
Score | 2.3339655 |
Snippet | Captive chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means... Captive chimpanzees ( ) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of requesting or... Captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means of... Captive chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) have been shown to learn the use of novel attention-getting (AG) sounds to capture the attention of humans as a means... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral hal proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 5475 |
SubjectTerms | Acoustics Animal Communication Animals Chimpanzees Cognitive science Cortex (motor) Female Genetic analysis Genetic factors Genetic Variation - genetics Genetic Variation - physiology Heritability Magnetic resonance imaging Male Monkeys & apes Morphology Motor Cortex - anatomy & histology Motor Cortex - physiology Motor skill learning Motors Mouth - physiology Neuronal Plasticity - physiology Pan troglodytes Psychology Psychomotor Performance - physiology Quantitative genetics Sound production |
Title | Genetic Factors and Orofacial Motor Learning Selectively Influence Variability in Central Sulcus Morphology in Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28473646 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2093497337 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1896041231 https://hal.science/hal-01619542 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC5452339 |
Volume | 37 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1bb9MwFLa68bIXBBuXwkAGIQSa0rWJ4zSP1UrVXegmbUN7ixLXaSeFBLUJUvcb-NGcEztOOioxeImiuHFdna_nYp_zHUI-iEhyKcOexbvCtljIQwusYGx1Zew5gAewKFg7_HXCx9fs5Ma9aW21GllLRR51xN3GupL_kSo8A7lilew_SNZMCg_gHuQLV5AwXB8kY-SMRsLVkW6ag5vg56BYQ1Vym8HDikB1BlohUcotWYFW0K1JDr5BrKyoulUBoNrsBX2SiGIJU4AYFEsTDs6xpjK9k2qr9gJUQ77IZkk2XeWobfymp1vXnJXeboM3s4aSytAp0_zUts_BsGMORbJCZwScJ5g7YpAhZ4UEVOeYpK34D6bI1WyQW-QLtQ1fZbk2NzbAWOoz-Wpjs5G1Orm_RqUgbQ9CX6a6_HSkVuB2eWLUa2p4RSujkazKoLW-dpnq26JtP4Rvzka74pb8FicTTK-8PDrugBuJ_bUxNdCrLWmVPTAeXAYXw1Fwdjw5XR81jN7jwVkwB8Chx-27zP4JEfwjG0Id7MIxPD413oTXLbtGm9-qq9xhQYebl7PmYG3NMb33z9jpfgpww6e6ekIea3jQgUL2U9KS6S7ZG6Rhnn1f0Y-0TE8u4bdLdoypXu2RXxr5VCOfAvKpQT4tkU8r5NMG8qlBPm0gn96mVCOfKuTTGvnlYI18-glwTxu4__yMXI--XB2NLd1XxBLM47klnWlfYGgM0YaIWSh4DDeRdG0RuzHEOxEPubS5I4T0hcui2HG58ETk9afT0I6d52Q7zVL5klCXhT7EAB7EPTB3yKO4yzj3kcTOY6HbbxO3EkYgNOk-9n5JAgy-QYiBEWKAQgx6PEAhtsmhee-Hop356xvvEU_VhzdjrE32KygEWo8t4W3fYb7nODDHOzMMVgaPDsNUZsUy6PWRxAm8XJjihUKO-Sp0cB3OeJt4a5haW8v6SHo7L5nsIXyzHcd_9ZC1vyY7tZbYJ9v5opBvICDIo7flP-Y3MxsPdg |
linkProvider | Flying Publisher |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Genetic+Factors+and+Orofacial+Motor+Learning+Selectively+Influence+Variability+in+Central+Sulcus+Morphology+in+Chimpanzees+%28Pan+troglodytes%29&rft.jtitle=The+Journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Hopkins%2C+William+D.&rft.au=Coulon%2C+Oliver&rft.au=Meguerditchian%2C+Adrien&rft.au=Autrey%2C+Michelle&rft.date=2017-05-31&rft.pub=Society+for+Neuroscience&rft.issn=0270-6474&rft.eissn=1529-2401&rft.volume=37&rft.issue=22&rft.spage=5475&rft.epage=5483&rft_id=info:doi/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.2641-16.2017&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK&rft.externalDocID=oai_HAL_hal_01619542v1 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon |