Early visual deprivation alters multisensory processing in peripersonal space
Multisensory peripersonal space develops in a maturational process that is thought to be influenced by early sensory experience. We investigated the role of vision in the effective development of audiotactile interactions in peripersonal space. Early blind (EB), late blind (LB) and sighted control (...
Saved in:
Published in | Neuropsychologia Vol. 47; no. 14; pp. 3236 - 3243 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Kidlington
Elsevier Ltd
01.12.2009
Elsevier |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Multisensory peripersonal space develops in a maturational process that is thought to be influenced by early sensory experience. We investigated the role of vision in the effective development of audiotactile interactions in peripersonal space. Early blind (EB), late blind (LB) and sighted control (SC) participants were asked to lateralize auditory, tactile and audiotactile stimuli. The experiment was conducted with the hands uncrossed or crossed over the body midline in order to alter the relationship between personal and peripersonal spatial representations. First, we observed that the crossed posture results in a greater detrimental effect for tactile performance in sighted subjects but a greater deficit in auditory performance in early blind ones. This result is interpreted as evidence for a visually driven developmental process that automatically remaps tactile and proprioceptive spatial representation into an external framework. Second, we demonstrate that improved reaction times observed in the bimodal conditions in SC and LB exceeds that predicted by probability summation in both conditions of postures, indicating neural integration of different sensory information. In EB, nonlinear summation was obtained in the uncrossed but not in the crossed posture. We argue that the default use of an anatomically anchored reference system in EB prevents effective audiotactile interactions in the crossed posture due to the poorly aligned spatial coordinates of these two modalities in such conditions. Altogether, these results provide compelling evidence for the critical role of early vision in the development of multisensory perception and action control in peripersonal space. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Multisensory peripersonal space develops in a maturational process that is thought to be influenced by early sensory experience. We investigated the role of vision in the effective development of audiotactile interactions in peripersonal space. Early blind (EB), late blind (LB) and sighted control (SC) participants were asked to lateralize auditory, tactile and audiotactile stimuli. The experiment was conducted with the hands uncrossed or crossed over the body midline in order to alter the relationship between personal and peripersonal spatial representations. First, we observed that the crossed posture results in a greater detrimental effect for tactile performance in sighted subjects but a greater deficit in auditory performance in early blind ones. This result is interpreted as evidence for a visually driven developmental process that automatically remaps tactile and proprioceptive spatial representation into an external framework. Second, we demonstrate that improved reaction times observed in the bimodal conditions in SC and LB exceeds that predicted by probability summation in both conditions of postures, indicating neural integration of different sensory information. In EB, nonlinear summation was obtained in the uncrossed but not in the crossed posture. We argue that the default use of an anatomically anchored reference system in EB prevents effective audiotactile interactions in the crossed posture due to the poorly aligned spatial coordinates of these two modalities in such conditions. Altogether, these results provide compelling evidence for the critical role of early vision in the development of multisensory perception and action control in peripersonal space. (Contains 3 figures and 2 tables.) Multisensory peripersonal space develops in a maturational process that is thought to be influenced by early sensory experience. We investigated the role of vision in the effective development of audiotactile interactions in peripersonal space. Early blind (EB), late blind (LB) and sighted control (SC) participants were asked to lateralize auditory, tactile and audiotactile stimuli. The experiment was conducted with the hands uncrossed or crossed over the body midline in order to alter the relationship between personal and peripersonal spatial representations. First, we observed that the crossed posture results in a greater detrimental effect for tactile performance in sighted subjects but a greater deficit in auditory performance in early blind ones. This result is interpreted as evidence for a visually driven developmental process that automatically remaps tactile and proprioceptive spatial representation into an external framework. Second, we demonstrate that improved reaction times observed in the bimodal conditions in SC and LB exceeds that predicted by probability summation in both conditions of postures, indicating neural integration of different sensory information. In EB, nonlinear summation was obtained in the uncrossed but not in the crossed posture. We argue that the default use of an anatomically anchored reference system in EB prevents effective audiotactile interactions in the crossed posture due to the poorly aligned spatial coordinates of these two modalities in such conditions. Altogether, these results provide compelling evidence for the critical role of early vision in the development of multisensory perception and action control in peripersonal space.Multisensory peripersonal space develops in a maturational process that is thought to be influenced by early sensory experience. We investigated the role of vision in the effective development of audiotactile interactions in peripersonal space. Early blind (EB), late blind (LB) and sighted control (SC) participants were asked to lateralize auditory, tactile and audiotactile stimuli. The experiment was conducted with the hands uncrossed or crossed over the body midline in order to alter the relationship between personal and peripersonal spatial representations. First, we observed that the crossed posture results in a greater detrimental effect for tactile performance in sighted subjects but a greater deficit in auditory performance in early blind ones. This result is interpreted as evidence for a visually driven developmental process that automatically remaps tactile and proprioceptive spatial representation into an external framework. Second, we demonstrate that improved reaction times observed in the bimodal conditions in SC and LB exceeds that predicted by probability summation in both conditions of postures, indicating neural integration of different sensory information. In EB, nonlinear summation was obtained in the uncrossed but not in the crossed posture. We argue that the default use of an anatomically anchored reference system in EB prevents effective audiotactile interactions in the crossed posture due to the poorly aligned spatial coordinates of these two modalities in such conditions. Altogether, these results provide compelling evidence for the critical role of early vision in the development of multisensory perception and action control in peripersonal space. Multisensory peripersonal space develops in a maturational process that is thought to be influenced by early sensory experience. We investigated the role of vision in the effective development of audiotactile interactions in peripersonal space. Early blind (EB), late blind (LB) and sighted control (SC) participants were asked to lateralize auditory, tactile and audiotactile stimuli. The experiment was conducted with the hands uncrossed or crossed over the body midline in order to alter the relationship between personal and peripersonal spatial representations. First, we observed that the crossed posture results in a greater detrimental effect for tactile performance in sighted subjects but a greater deficit in auditory performance in early blind ones. This result is interpreted as evidence for a visually driven developmental process that automatically remaps tactile and proprioceptive spatial representation into an external framework. Second, we demonstrate that improved reaction times observed in the bimodal conditions in SC and LB exceeds that predicted by probability summation in both conditions of postures, indicating neural integration of different sensory information. In EB, nonlinear summation was obtained in the uncrossed but not in the crossed posture. We argue that the default use of an anatomically anchored reference system in EB prevents effective audiotactile interactions in the crossed posture due to the poorly aligned spatial coordinates of these two modalities in such conditions. Altogether, these results provide compelling evidence for the critical role of early vision in the development of multisensory perception and action control in peripersonal space. |
Author | Lassonde, Maryse Charbonneau, Geneviève Lepore, Franco Collignon, Olivier |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Olivier surname: Collignon fullname: Collignon, Olivier email: olivier.collignon@umontreal.ca organization: Université de Montréal, Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition (CERNEC), Montreal, Canada – sequence: 2 givenname: Geneviève surname: Charbonneau fullname: Charbonneau, Geneviève organization: Université de Montréal, Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition (CERNEC), Montreal, Canada – sequence: 3 givenname: Maryse surname: Lassonde fullname: Lassonde, Maryse organization: Université de Montréal, Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition (CERNEC), Montreal, Canada – sequence: 4 givenname: Franco surname: Lepore fullname: Lepore, Franco organization: Université de Montréal, Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition (CERNEC), Montreal, Canada |
BackLink | http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ862813$$DView record in ERIC http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=22173555$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19666035$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkU9vFCEYxompsdvqNzBmLtbTjC8wwMzFxNSt1dR40TOhzDuVDQsrzGyy317W3dakB9MDIeH58bx_njNyEmJAQi4oNBSofL9qAs4pbvLO_oo-3jnTMIC-AdUAE8_IgnaK11zQ9oQsAFhX856zU3KW8woAWsG6F-SU9lJK4GJBvi1N8rtq6_JsfDXgJrmtmVwMlfETplytZz-5jCHHtKs2KVrM2YW7yoVqg8mVk2MoX_PGWHxJno_GZ3x1vM_Jz6vlj8vr-ub75y-XH29q2yo-1YPqB-j7cWRKIJjeUAlK9YbdMkNHQFleqBS2h1EqBkj5KMTAeduplo9loHPy7uBbGvo9Y5702mWL3puAcc5acS5pKwEKefFfkpVCDDgt4JsjON-ucdBlEWuTdvp-VQV4ewRMtsaPyQTr8gPHGFVciD33-sCV7dgHefm1k6yjvMgfDrJNMeeE479KoPcR65V-HLHeR6xB6RJxMfj0yMC66W9kUzLOP93m-thmyWnrMOlsHQaLg0toJz1E91SrP-bW0Ec |
CODEN | NUPSA6 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2011_11_002 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_00291 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2013_03_004 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15315 crossref_primary_10_1002_ejp_876 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2023_114818 crossref_primary_10_7899_JCE_18_14 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2016_10_014 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2013_04_012 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_013_0559_1 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2014_07_012 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2018_01_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2011_11_006 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2014_03_058 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11682_023_00775_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2015_05_068 crossref_primary_10_1097_WCO_0b013e328348bdbf crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_01158_9 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_5547_12_2013 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2014_08_003 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2019_06_003 crossref_primary_10_1163_22134808_00002460 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797619866625 crossref_primary_10_1155_2012_304045 crossref_primary_10_1177_1073858410392239 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae324 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2011_10_019 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_isci_2024_109167 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2015_04_004 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2022_1050772 crossref_primary_10_1002_ejp_978 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnint_2014_00098 crossref_primary_10_1111_ane_12396 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2019_12_024 crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_12813 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2021_07_017 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2023_114361 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_43526_8 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2016_00612 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2020_06_008 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_45634_w crossref_primary_10_3390_genes15060660 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_isci_2022_104439 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci15030288 crossref_primary_10_1163_22134808_bja10018 crossref_primary_10_1177_03946320120250S103 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0189067 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_82350_6 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0107281 |
Cites_doi | 10.1152/jn.00021.2005 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00512.x 10.1007/s00221-008-1410-0 10.1111/1467-9280.00316 10.1152/jn.00587.2007 10.1073/pnas.96.18.10418 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.04.023 10.1038/22106 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0455-05.2005 10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.045 10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00007-3 10.3758/CABN.4.2.208 10.1016/j.cub.2003.12.054 10.1126/science.4048948 10.1111/j.2164-0947.1962.tb01433.x 10.1007/s00221-005-2327-5 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.11.003 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.004 10.1152/jn.1998.80.2.1006 10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.149 10.1111/1467-9280.00270 10.1007/s10548-009-0075-8 10.1007/s00221-008-1510-x 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00070-8 10.3758/BF03194823 10.1093/cercor/bhl077 10.1007/s002210100773 10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00031-4 10.1007/s10339-004-0017-z 10.1007/s00221-002-1261-z 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2535-04.2004 10.1038/nn1480 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.003 10.1073/pnas.0607158104 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.079 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.018 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.010 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.036 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00060-5 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90010-X 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.004 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00449-4 10.1126/science.290.5497.1782 10.1126/science.2063209 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00168-4 10.1126/science.277.5323.190 10.1152/jn.00497.2006 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06352.x 10.1162/08989290152001899 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.021 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3295-06.2006 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00118-X 10.1038/89559 10.3758/BF03193160 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03439.2003 10.1093/cercor/bhh197 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-07-02429.1997 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2009 Elsevier Ltd 2015 INIST-CNRS |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2009 Elsevier Ltd – notice: 2015 INIST-CNRS |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TK 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ERIC ERIC (Ovid) ERIC ERIC ERIC (Legacy Platform) ERIC( SilverPlatter ) ERIC ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform) Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) ERIC Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef ERIC MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | ERIC MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE Neurosciences Abstracts |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: ERI name: ERIC url: https://eric.ed.gov/ sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine |
EISSN | 1873-3514 |
ERIC | EJ862813 |
EndPage | 3243 |
ExternalDocumentID | 19666035 22173555 EJ862813 10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_07_025 S0028393209003194 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M --Z -~X .GJ .~1 0R~ 123 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29N 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 53G 5RE 5VS 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAGJA AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAXLA AAXUO AAYJJ ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABMAC ABOYX ABXDB ABYKQ ACCUC ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACIUM ACNCT ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS ADMUD AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AENEX AETEA AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ ASPBG AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC C45 CS3 D0S DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q GBLVA HMQ HMW HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IHE J1W KOM L7B M2V M3V M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OKEIE OVD OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSY SSZ T5K TEORI TN5 UHB UQL WH7 WUQ XJT XOL YQT ZGI ZKB ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADNMO ADXHL AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO EFKBS ERI PET REK WWN IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7TK 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c473t-d79d099ff275e0a9a160779a2b2a1f0e69a1165c90f6720e13f55d3348743f393 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0028-3932 1873-3514 |
IngestDate | Mon Jul 21 11:10:49 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 19:18:43 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:05:19 EDT 2025 Wed Apr 02 07:26:01 EDT 2025 Fri Aug 01 12:16:57 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:09:59 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 04:14:03 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:28:59 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | false |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 14 |
Keywords | Somatosensory Auditory Peripersonal space Multisensory Blindness Redundant signal effect (RSE) Human Vision disorder Stimulus redundancy Cognition Eye disease Hearing Sensory deprivation Vision Early Intermodal perception |
Language | English |
License | https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0 CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c473t-d79d099ff275e0a9a160779a2b2a1f0e69a1165c90f6720e13f55d3348743f393 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 |
PMID | 19666035 |
PQID | 21162031 |
PQPubID | 23462 |
PageCount | 8 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_733614600 proquest_miscellaneous_21162031 pubmed_primary_19666035 pascalfrancis_primary_22173555 eric_primary_EJ862813 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_07_025 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_07_025 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_07_025 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2009-12-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2009-12-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 12 year: 2009 text: 2009-12-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Kidlington |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Kidlington – name: England |
PublicationTitle | Neuropsychologia |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Neuropsychologia |
PublicationYear | 2009 |
Publisher | Elsevier Ltd Elsevier |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Ltd – name: Elsevier |
References | Shore, Spry, Spence (bib46) 2002; 14 Oldfield (bib36) 1971; 9 Röder, Rosler, Spence (bib41) 2004; 14 Wallace, Meredith, Stein (bib57) 1998; 80 Martuzzi, Murray, Michel, Thiran, Philippe, Maeder (bib30) 2007; 17 Kitazawa (bib23) 2002; 11 Werner-Reiss, Kelly, Trause, Underhill, Groh (bib61) 2003; 13 Eimer (bib11) 2004; 14 Sperdin, Cappe, Foxe, Murray (bib50) 2009 Hecht, Reiner, Karni (bib19) 2008; 189 Gallace, Soto-Faraco, Dalton, Kreukniet, Spence (bib15) 2008; 191 Avillac, Deneve, Olivier, Pouget, Duhamel (bib1) 2005; 8 Pavani, Spence, Driver (bib37) 2000; 11 Eimer (bib10) 2001; 39 Goldreich, Kanics (bib14) 2003; 23 Lloyd, Merat, McGlone, Spence (bib28) 2003; 65 Azanon, Soto-Faraco (bib2) 2008; 18 Nardini, Jones, Bedford, Braddick (bib34) 2008; 18 Wallace, Perrault, Hairston, Stein (bib59) 2004; 24 Lewkowicz (bib27) 2002; 14 Yamamoto, Kitazawa (bib62) 2001; 4 Murray, Molholm, Michel, Heslenfeld, Ritter, Javitt (bib33) 2005; 15 Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese (bib39) 1997; 277 Knudsen, Brainard (bib25) 1991; 253 Carriere, Royal, Perrault, Morrison, Vaughan, Stein (bib5) 2007; 98 Kujala, Lehtokoski, Alho, Kekoni, Naatanen (bib26) 1997; 96 Townsend, Ashby (bib53) 1983 Maravita, Spence, Driver (bib29) 2003; 13 Röder, Kusmierek, Spence, Schicke (bib42) 2007; 104 Champoux, Bacon, Lepore, Guillemot (bib6) 2008; 1191 Spence, Kingstone, Shore, Gazzaniga (bib48) 2001; 12 Miller (bib31) 1982; 14 Wallace (bib58) 2004; 5 Bremner, Holmes, Spence (bib3) 2008; 12 Graziano (bib17) 1999; 96 Röder, Focker, Hotting, Spence (bib43) 2008; 28 Röder, Teder-Salejarvi, Sterr, Rosler, Hillyard, Neville (bib40) 1999; 400 Mullette-Gillman, Cohen, Groh (bib32) 2005; 94 Kennett, Eimer, Spence, Driver (bib22) 2001; 13 Eimer, Cockburn, Smedley, Driver (bib12) 2001; 139 Graziano, Cooke, Taylor (bib18) 2000; 290 Raab (bib38) 1962; 24 Spence, Driver (bib49) 2004 Collignon, Girard, Gosselin, Roy, Saint-Amour, Lassonde (bib8) 2008; 1242 Holmes, Sanabria, Calvert, Spence (bib20) 2006; 1077 Wallace, Stein (bib56) 2007; 97 Ulrich, Miller, Schroter (bib54) 2007; 39 Hotting, Rosler, Röder (bib21) 2003; 148 Neil, Chee-Ruiter, Scheier, Lewkowicz, Shimojo (bib35) 2006; 9 Forster, Eardley, Eimer (bib13) 2007; 1131 Zampini, Torresan, Spence, Murray (bib63) 2007; 45 Collignon, Renier, Bruyer, Tranduy, Veraart (bib7) 2006; 1075 Sanabria, Soto-Faraco, Spence (bib44) 2005; 165 Soto-Faraco, Spence, Kingstone (bib47) 2004; 4 Knudsen, Knudsen (bib24) 1985; 230 Townsend, Ashby (bib52) 1978; vol. 3 Stein, Meredith (bib51) 1993 Collignon, Davare, Olivier, De Volder (bib9) 2009; 21 Schlack, Sterbing-D’Angelo, Hartung, Hoffmann, Bremmer (bib45) 2005; 25 Bremner, Mareschal, Lloyd-Fox, Spence (bib4) 2008; 137 Wallace, Stein (bib55) 1997; 17 Wallace, Carriere, Perrault, Vaughan, Stein (bib60) 2006; 26 Gori, Del Viva, Sandini, Burr (bib16) 2008; 18 Collignon (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib7) 2006; 1075 Yamamoto (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib62) 2001; 4 Eimer (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib10) 2001; 39 Ulrich (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib54) 2007; 39 Bremner (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib3) 2008; 12 Wallace (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib56) 2007; 97 Spence (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib48) 2001; 12 Sanabria (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib44) 2005; 165 Kitazawa (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib23) 2002; 11 Eimer (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib11) 2004; 14 Townsend (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib53) 1983 Röder (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib41) 2004; 14 Maravita (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib29) 2003; 13 Miller (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib31) 1982; 14 Bremner (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib4) 2008; 137 Mullette-Gillman (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib32) 2005; 94 Neil (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib35) 2006; 9 Carriere (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib5) 2007; 98 Goldreich (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib14) 2003; 23 Soto-Faraco (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib47) 2004; 4 Pavani (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib37) 2000; 11 Wallace (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib57) 1998; 80 Gallace (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib15) 2008; 191 Townsend (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib52) 1978; vol. 3 Avillac (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib1) 2005; 8 Röder (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib43) 2008; 28 Stein (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib51) 1993 Eimer (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib12) 2001; 139 Rizzolatti (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib39) 1997; 277 Hecht (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib19) 2008; 189 Knudsen (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib24) 1985; 230 Werner-Reiss (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib61) 2003; 13 Shore (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib46) 2002; 14 Kennett (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib22) 2001; 13 Wallace (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib60) 2006; 26 Röder (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib40) 1999; 400 Azanon (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib2) 2008; 18 Hotting (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib21) 2003; 148 Zampini (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib63) 2007; 45 Murray (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib33) 2005; 15 Lloyd (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib28) 2003; 65 Graziano (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib18) 2000; 290 Nardini (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib34) 2008; 18 Gori (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib16) 2008; 18 Champoux (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib6) 2008; 1191 Holmes (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib20) 2006; 1077 Martuzzi (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib30) 2007; 17 Sperdin (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib50) 2009 Knudsen (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib25) 1991; 253 Röder (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib42) 2007; 104 Raab (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib38) 1962; 24 Forster (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib13) 2007; 1131 Collignon (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib8) 2008; 1242 Lewkowicz (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib27) 2002; 14 Graziano (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib17) 1999; 96 Spence (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib49) 2004 Collignon (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib9) 2009; 21 Kujala (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib26) 1997; 96 Wallace (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib59) 2004; 24 Wallace (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib58) 2004; 5 Wallace (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib55) 1997; 17 Oldfield (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib36) 1971; 9 Schlack (10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib45) 2005; 25 |
References_xml | – volume: 39 start-page: 291 year: 2007 end-page: 302 ident: bib54 article-title: Testing the race model inequality: An algorithm and computer programs publication-title: Behavioral Researches Methods – volume: 1191 start-page: 84 year: 2008 end-page: 95 ident: bib6 article-title: Effects of early binocular enucleation on auditory and somatosensory coding in the superior colliculus of the rat publication-title: Brain Research – volume: 191 start-page: 171 year: 2008 end-page: 186 ident: bib15 article-title: Response requirements modulate tactile spatial congruency effects publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 18 start-page: 694 year: 2008 end-page: 698 ident: bib16 article-title: Young children do not integrate visual and haptic form information publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 5 start-page: 69 year: 2004 end-page: 83 ident: bib58 article-title: The development of multisensory processes publication-title: Cognitive Processing – volume: 98 start-page: 2858 year: 2007 end-page: 2867 ident: bib5 article-title: Visual deprivation alters the development of cortical multisensory integration publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology – volume: 139 start-page: 398 year: 2001 end-page: 411 ident: bib12 article-title: Cross-modal links in endogenous spatial attention are mediated by common external locations: Evidence from event-related brain potentials publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 13 start-page: 554 year: 2003 end-page: 562 ident: bib61 article-title: Eye position affects activity in primary auditory cortex of primates publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 400 start-page: 162 year: 1999 end-page: 166 ident: bib40 article-title: Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans publication-title: Nature – volume: 137 start-page: 149 year: 2008 end-page: 162 ident: bib4 article-title: Spatial localization of touch in the first year of life: Early influence of a visual spatial code and the development of remapping across changes in limb position publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology General – volume: 25 start-page: 4616 year: 2005 end-page: 4625 ident: bib45 article-title: Multisensory space representations in the macaque ventral intraparietal area publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 13 start-page: 462 year: 2001 end-page: 478 ident: bib22 article-title: Tactile-visual links in exogenous spatial attention under different postures: Convergent evidence from psychophysics and ERPs publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 39 start-page: 1292 year: 2001 end-page: 1303 ident: bib10 article-title: Crossmodal links in spatial attention between vision, audition, and touch: Evidence from event-related brain potentials publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 14 start-page: 153 year: 2002 end-page: 163 ident: bib46 article-title: Confusing the mind by crossing the hands publication-title: Brain Research. Cognitive. Brain Research – year: 1993 ident: bib51 article-title: The merging of the senses – volume: 18 start-page: 1044 year: 2008 end-page: 1049 ident: bib2 article-title: Changing reference frames during the encoding of tactile events publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 15 start-page: 963 year: 2005 end-page: 974 ident: bib33 article-title: Grabbing your ear: Rapid auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in low-level sensory cortices are not constrained by stimulus alignment publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – volume: 45 start-page: 1869 year: 2007 end-page: 1877 ident: bib63 article-title: Auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in front and rear space publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 1075 start-page: 175 year: 2006 end-page: 182 ident: bib7 article-title: Improved selective and divided spatial attention in early blind subjects publication-title: Brain Research – volume: 23 start-page: 3439 year: 2003 end-page: 3445 ident: bib14 article-title: Tactile acuity is enhanced in blindness publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 9 start-page: 97 year: 1971 end-page: 113 ident: bib36 article-title: The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 24 start-page: 9580 year: 2004 end-page: 9584 ident: bib59 article-title: Visual experience is necessary for the development of multisensory integration publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 1131 start-page: 149 year: 2007 end-page: 154 ident: bib13 article-title: Altered tactile spatial attention in the early blind publication-title: Brain Research – volume: 230 start-page: 545 year: 1985 end-page: 548 ident: bib24 article-title: Vision guides the adjustment of auditory localization in young barn owls publication-title: Science – volume: 148 start-page: 26 year: 2003 end-page: 37 ident: bib21 article-title: Crossmodal and intermodal attention modulate event-related brain potentials to tactile and auditory stimuli publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 4 start-page: 759 year: 2001 end-page: 765 ident: bib62 article-title: Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing publication-title: Nature Neuroscience – volume: 18 start-page: 689 year: 2008 end-page: 693 ident: bib34 article-title: Development of cue integration in human navigation publication-title: Current Biology – start-page: 2009 year: 2009 ident: bib50 article-title: Early, low-level auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions impact reaction time speed publication-title: Frontiers in Integrative Neurosciences – volume: 97 start-page: 921 year: 2007 end-page: 926 ident: bib56 article-title: Early experience determines how the senses will interact publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology – volume: 14 start-page: R115 year: 2004 end-page: R117 ident: bib11 article-title: Multisensory integration: How visual experience shapes spatial perception publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 165 start-page: 505 year: 2005 end-page: 514 ident: bib44 article-title: Spatiotemporal interactions between audition and touch depend on hand posture. Experimental publication-title: Brain Research – year: 1983 ident: bib53 article-title: Stochastic modelling of elementary psychological processes – volume: 96 start-page: 10418 year: 1999 end-page: 10421 ident: bib17 article-title: Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA – year: 2004 ident: bib49 article-title: Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention – volume: 253 start-page: 85 year: 1991 end-page: 87 ident: bib25 article-title: Visual instruction of the neural map of auditory space in the developing optic tectum publication-title: Science – volume: 1077 start-page: 108 year: 2006 end-page: 115 ident: bib20 article-title: Multisensory interactions follow the hands across the midline: Evidence from a non-spatial visual-tactile congruency task publication-title: Brain Research – volume: 26 start-page: 11844 year: 2006 end-page: 11849 ident: bib60 article-title: The development of cortical multisensory integration publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 11 start-page: 353 year: 2000 end-page: 359 ident: bib37 article-title: Visual capture of touch: Out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 4 start-page: 208 year: 2004 end-page: 217 ident: bib47 article-title: Congruency effects between auditory and tactile motion: Extending the phenomenon of cross-modal dynamic capture publication-title: Cognitive and Affective Behavioral Neuroscience – volume: 8 start-page: 941 year: 2005 end-page: 949 ident: bib1 article-title: Reference frames for representing visual and tactile locations in parietal cortex publication-title: Nature Neuroscience – volume: 94 start-page: 2331 year: 2005 end-page: 2352 ident: bib32 article-title: Eye-centered, head-centered, and complex coding of visual and auditory targets in the intraparietal sulcus publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology – volume: 28 start-page: 475 year: 2008 end-page: 483 ident: bib43 article-title: Spatial coordinate systems for tactile spatial attention depend on developmental vision: Evidence from event-related potentials in sighted and congenitally blind adult humans publication-title: European Journal of Neurosciences – volume: 11 start-page: 475 year: 2002 end-page: 477 ident: bib23 article-title: Where conscious sensation takes place publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition – volume: 65 start-page: 901 year: 2003 end-page: 924 ident: bib28 article-title: Crossmodal links between audition and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – volume: 290 start-page: 1782 year: 2000 end-page: 1786 ident: bib18 article-title: Coding the location of the arm by sight publication-title: Science – volume: 14 start-page: 41 year: 2002 end-page: 63 ident: bib27 article-title: Heterogeneity and heterochrony in the development of intersensory perception publication-title: Brain Research. Cognitive. Brain Research – volume: 14 start-page: 247 year: 1982 end-page: 279 ident: bib31 article-title: Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 24 start-page: 574 year: 1962 end-page: 590 ident: bib38 article-title: Statistical facilitation of simple reaction times publication-title: Transactions of the New York Academy of Science – volume: 14 start-page: 121 year: 2004 end-page: 124 ident: bib41 article-title: Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind publication-title: Current Biology – volume: 104 start-page: 4753 year: 2007 end-page: 4758 ident: bib42 article-title: Developmental vision determines the reference frame for the multisensory control of action publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA – volume: 189 start-page: 133 year: 2008 end-page: 143 ident: bib19 article-title: Multisensory enhancement: Gains in choice and in simple response times publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 277 start-page: 190 year: 1997 end-page: 191 ident: bib39 article-title: The space around us publication-title: Science – volume: 96 start-page: 75 year: 1997 end-page: 82 ident: bib26 article-title: Faster reaction times in the blind than sighted during bimodal divided attention publication-title: Acta Psychologica (Amst) – volume: 1242 start-page: 126 year: 2008 end-page: 135 ident: bib8 article-title: Audio-visual integration of emotion expression publication-title: Brain Research – volume: 12 start-page: 90 year: 2001 end-page: 93 ident: bib48 article-title: Representation of visuotactile space in the split brain publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 17 start-page: 2429 year: 1997 end-page: 2444 ident: bib55 article-title: Development of multisensory neurons and multisensory integration in cat superior colliculus publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 21 start-page: 232 year: 2009 end-page: 240 ident: bib9 article-title: Reorganisation of the right occipito-parietal stream for auditory spatial processing in early blind humans. A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation study publication-title: Brain Topography – volume: 17 start-page: 1672 year: 2007 end-page: 1679 ident: bib30 article-title: Multisensory interactions within human primary cortices revealed by BOLD dynamics publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – volume: 80 start-page: 1006 year: 1998 end-page: 1010 ident: bib57 article-title: Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus of the alert cat publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology – volume: 12 start-page: 298 year: 2008 end-page: 305 ident: bib3 article-title: Infants lost in (peripersonal) space? publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Science – volume: 9 start-page: 454 year: 2006 end-page: 464 ident: bib35 article-title: Development of multisensory spatial integration and perception in humans publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 13 start-page: R531 year: 2003 end-page: 539 ident: bib29 article-title: Multisensory integration and the body schema: Close to hand and within reach publication-title: Current Biology – volume: vol. 3 start-page: 199 year: 1978 end-page: 239 ident: bib52 article-title: Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems publication-title: Cognitive theory – volume: 94 start-page: 2331 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib32 article-title: Eye-centered, head-centered, and complex coding of visual and auditory targets in the intraparietal sulcus publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.00021.2005 – volume: 9 start-page: 454 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib35 article-title: Development of multisensory spatial integration and perception in humans publication-title: Developmental Science doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00512.x – volume: 189 start-page: 133 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib19 article-title: Multisensory enhancement: Gains in choice and in simple response times publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1410-0 – volume: 12 start-page: 90 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib48 article-title: Representation of visuotactile space in the split brain publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00316 – volume: 98 start-page: 2858 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib5 article-title: Visual deprivation alters the development of cortical multisensory integration publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.00587.2007 – volume: 96 start-page: 10418 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib17 article-title: Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.18.10418 – volume: 1242 start-page: 126 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib8 article-title: Audio-visual integration of emotion expression publication-title: Brain Research doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.04.023 – volume: 400 start-page: 162 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib40 article-title: Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/22106 – volume: 25 start-page: 4616 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib45 article-title: Multisensory space representations in the macaque ventral intraparietal area publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0455-05.2005 – volume: 18 start-page: 1044 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib2 article-title: Changing reference frames during the encoding of tactile events publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.045 – volume: 96 start-page: 75 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib26 article-title: Faster reaction times in the blind than sighted during bimodal divided attention publication-title: Acta Psychologica (Amst) doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00007-3 – volume: 4 start-page: 208 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib47 article-title: Congruency effects between auditory and tactile motion: Extending the phenomenon of cross-modal dynamic capture publication-title: Cognitive and Affective Behavioral Neuroscience doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.2.208 – volume: 14 start-page: 121 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib41 article-title: Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2003.12.054 – volume: 230 start-page: 545 year: 1985 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib24 article-title: Vision guides the adjustment of auditory localization in young barn owls publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.4048948 – volume: 24 start-page: 574 year: 1962 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib38 article-title: Statistical facilitation of simple reaction times publication-title: Transactions of the New York Academy of Science doi: 10.1111/j.2164-0947.1962.tb01433.x – volume: 165 start-page: 505 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib44 article-title: Spatiotemporal interactions between audition and touch depend on hand posture. Experimental publication-title: Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2327-5 – volume: 1191 start-page: 84 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib6 article-title: Effects of early binocular enucleation on auditory and somatosensory coding in the superior colliculus of the rat publication-title: Brain Research doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.11.003 – volume: 1131 start-page: 149 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib13 article-title: Altered tactile spatial attention in the early blind publication-title: Brain Research doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.004 – volume: 80 start-page: 1006 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib57 article-title: Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus of the alert cat publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.2.1006 – volume: 137 start-page: 149 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib4 article-title: Spatial localization of touch in the first year of life: Early influence of a visual spatial code and the development of remapping across changes in limb position publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology General doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.1.149 – volume: 11 start-page: 353 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib37 article-title: Visual capture of touch: Out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00270 – volume: 21 start-page: 232 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib9 article-title: Reorganisation of the right occipito-parietal stream for auditory spatial processing in early blind humans. A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation study publication-title: Brain Topography doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0075-8 – volume: 191 start-page: 171 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib15 article-title: Response requirements modulate tactile spatial congruency effects publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1510-x – volume: 9 start-page: 97 year: 1971 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib36 article-title: The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 – volume: 14 start-page: 153 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib46 article-title: Confusing the mind by crossing the hands publication-title: Brain Research. Cognitive. Brain Research doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00070-8 – volume: 65 start-page: 901 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib28 article-title: Crossmodal links between audition and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03194823 – volume: 17 start-page: 1672 issue: 7 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib30 article-title: Multisensory interactions within human primary cortices revealed by BOLD dynamics publication-title: Cerebral Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl077 – volume: 139 start-page: 398 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib12 article-title: Cross-modal links in endogenous spatial attention are mediated by common external locations: Evidence from event-related brain potentials publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s002210100773 – volume: 11 start-page: 475 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib23 article-title: Where conscious sensation takes place publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition doi: 10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00031-4 – volume: 5 start-page: 69 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib58 article-title: The development of multisensory processes publication-title: Cognitive Processing doi: 10.1007/s10339-004-0017-z – volume: 148 start-page: 26 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib21 article-title: Crossmodal and intermodal attention modulate event-related brain potentials to tactile and auditory stimuli publication-title: Experimental Brain Research doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1261-z – volume: 24 start-page: 9580 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib59 article-title: Visual experience is necessary for the development of multisensory integration publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2535-04.2004 – volume: 8 start-page: 941 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib1 article-title: Reference frames for representing visual and tactile locations in parietal cortex publication-title: Nature Neuroscience doi: 10.1038/nn1480 – volume: 12 start-page: 298 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib3 article-title: Infants lost in (peripersonal) space? publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Science doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.003 – volume: 104 start-page: 4753 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib42 article-title: Developmental vision determines the reference frame for the multisensory control of action publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607158104 – volume: 1075 start-page: 175 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib7 article-title: Improved selective and divided spatial attention in early blind subjects publication-title: Brain Research doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.079 – volume: 14 start-page: R115 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib11 article-title: Multisensory integration: How visual experience shapes spatial perception publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.018 – volume: 1077 start-page: 108 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib20 article-title: Multisensory interactions follow the hands across the midline: Evidence from a non-spatial visual-tactile congruency task publication-title: Brain Research doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.010 – volume: 18 start-page: 694 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib16 article-title: Young children do not integrate visual and haptic form information publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.036 – volume: 14 start-page: 41 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib27 article-title: Heterogeneity and heterochrony in the development of intersensory perception publication-title: Brain Research. Cognitive. Brain Research doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00060-5 – volume: 14 start-page: 247 year: 1982 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib31 article-title: Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90010-X – volume: 45 start-page: 1869 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib63 article-title: Auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in front and rear space publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.004 – volume: 13 start-page: R531 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib29 article-title: Multisensory integration and the body schema: Close to hand and within reach publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00449-4 – volume: 290 start-page: 1782 year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib18 article-title: Coding the location of the arm by sight publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.290.5497.1782 – volume: 253 start-page: 85 year: 1991 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib25 article-title: Visual instruction of the neural map of auditory space in the developing optic tectum publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.2063209 – volume: 13 start-page: 554 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib61 article-title: Eye position affects activity in primary auditory cortex of primates publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00168-4 – volume: 277 start-page: 190 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib39 article-title: The space around us publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.277.5323.190 – year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib49 – volume: 97 start-page: 921 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib56 article-title: Early experience determines how the senses will interact publication-title: Journal of Neurophysiology doi: 10.1152/jn.00497.2006 – volume: 28 start-page: 475 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib43 article-title: Spatial coordinate systems for tactile spatial attention depend on developmental vision: Evidence from event-related potentials in sighted and congenitally blind adult humans publication-title: European Journal of Neurosciences doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06352.x – volume: 13 start-page: 462 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib22 article-title: Tactile-visual links in exogenous spatial attention under different postures: Convergent evidence from psychophysics and ERPs publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience doi: 10.1162/08989290152001899 – volume: 18 start-page: 689 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib34 article-title: Development of cue integration in human navigation publication-title: Current Biology doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.021 – volume: 26 start-page: 11844 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib60 article-title: The development of cortical multisensory integration publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3295-06.2006 – volume: 39 start-page: 1292 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib10 article-title: Crossmodal links in spatial attention between vision, audition, and touch: Evidence from event-related brain potentials publication-title: Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00118-X – start-page: 2009 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib50 article-title: Early, low-level auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions impact reaction time speed publication-title: Frontiers in Integrative Neurosciences – volume: 4 start-page: 759 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib62 article-title: Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing publication-title: Nature Neuroscience doi: 10.1038/89559 – year: 1993 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib51 – year: 1983 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib53 – volume: vol. 3 start-page: 199 year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib52 article-title: Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems – volume: 39 start-page: 291 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib54 article-title: Testing the race model inequality: An algorithm and computer programs publication-title: Behavioral Researches Methods doi: 10.3758/BF03193160 – volume: 23 start-page: 3439 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib14 article-title: Tactile acuity is enhanced in blindness publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03439.2003 – volume: 15 start-page: 963 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib33 article-title: Grabbing your ear: Rapid auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in low-level sensory cortices are not constrained by stimulus alignment publication-title: Cerebral Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh197 – volume: 17 start-page: 2429 year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025_bib55 article-title: Development of multisensory neurons and multisensory integration in cat superior colliculus publication-title: Journal of Neuroscience doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-07-02429.1997 |
SSID | ssj0004528 |
Score | 2.208188 |
Snippet | Multisensory peripersonal space develops in a maturational process that is thought to be influenced by early sensory experience. We investigated the role of... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis eric crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 3236 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Age of Onset Attention - physiology Audition Auditory Biological and medical sciences Blindness Blindness - physiopathology Brain Hemisphere Functions Cognitive Processes Control Groups Developmental Stages Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Hearing - physiology Humans Male Medical sciences Middle Aged Models, Psychological Multisensory Ophthalmology Perception Perception - physiology Perceptual Masking - physiology Peripersonal space Physical Stimulation - methods Probability Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Reaction Time Reaction Time - physiology Redundant signal effect (RSE) Role Sensory Experience Somatosensory Spatial Ability Tactual Perception Touch - physiology Vision Vision disorders |
Title | Early visual deprivation alters multisensory processing in peripersonal space |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025 http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ862813 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19666035 https://www.proquest.com/docview/21162031 https://www.proquest.com/docview/733614600 |
Volume | 47 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3NS-wwEB9EQQQRn599z7cvB_FWTbNtsj14WETZp6wnBW8hbRNYkW6xu4IX_3Zn2nT94Ake3rUkLclMZn7TzPwG4DAXRSxlHoWFMzldM7rQcGnDzNiByOI4tY6Kk8fXcnQbX94ld0tw1tXCUFqlt_2tTW-stX9y4nfzpJpMqMYXXSPCD04_4zAWpwr2WJGWH79E7xnDBx0TM41ehaO3HK-GM7JaWBrj-SvVMafW2f92VD4ter0yNe6ia9tffI1PGz91sQkbHmCyYbuGH7Bkyy1YHfsr9G0YN5TG7GlSz3EYJcL6_masuTivWZNiWGN0O318ZlVbR4D-jU1KRqzIlUfvDE1Rbnfg9uL85mwU-p4KYR6r_iwsVFogKHROqMRykxoimFOpEZkwkeNW4pNIJnnKnVSC26jvkqSgcl2EGg43cBeWy2lp94EZg2ATv0QgLpYWQy_DeWZFZGXuUM4BnHYbqHNPOE59Lx50l1l2rz8LgLpipporjQIIQC3mVy31xrdnDjt56Q_KpNFPfPsdOyToxZfPLzH2G0T9AHofJL8YIDC4Q_CGE_90qqDxqNL9iyntdF5rjLWlQFUNgH0xgrgp0XVxHsBeq0RvK8e4VPJ-8vM_rO0XrAnfBoNHB7A8e5zb34itZlmvOTw9WBn-vRpd95q_Ca-NFCk0 |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1NT9wwEB0hkGilCgGlbWgLPgC3gOMkzubAAVHQ8rGcQOJmnMSWFlXZiOyCuPCn-gc7kzhLqUDiANfIjmOPPTMvnnkDsJGLIpIyD_zC6pyuGa2vuTR-pk1PZFGUGkvJyYMz2b-Iji_jyxn40-XCUFil0_2tTm-0tXuy41ZzpxoOKccXTSO6H5x-xiEWd5GVJ-b-DnFbvXv0C4W8KcThwfl-33elBfw8SsKxXyRpgb6RtSKJDdepJp61JNUiEzqw3Eh8Esg4T7mVieAmCG0cF5S1ihbXhsTAhHp_LkJ1QWUTth-CfynKex31M33ePGw9BpU1JJXVVLVpR5iZbHOq1f28ZXRx2J8qXaPYbFtv42WHuDGMh4uw4Dxattcu2hLMmHIZ5gfuzv4zDBoOZXY7rCfYjCJvXUE11tzU16yJaawRTo9u7lnVJi6gQWXDkhENc-XgAkPdl5sVuHiXlf4Cs-WoNN-AaY3eLY5EXmMkDWI9zXlmRGBkbnFjebDbLaDKHcM5Fdr4rbpQtmv1vwCoDGeqeKJQAB4k0_5Vy_Xx6p57nbzUk92r0DC9-h0rJOjpyAfHCDZ7QejB2hPJTxsIRJPoLWLH9W4rKNQNdOGjSzOa1ArBvRR4NjxgL7QgMky0lZx78LXdRI8zRyAseRivvsHc1uFD_3xwqk6Pzk6-w0fhanDw4AfMjm8m5ic6duNsrTlIDK7e--T-BaOJYdk |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Early+visual+deprivation+alters+multisensory+processing+in+peripersonal+space&rft.jtitle=Neuropsychologia&rft.au=Collignon%2C+Olivier&rft.au=Charbonneau%2C+Genevieve&rft.au=Lassonde%2C+Maryse&rft.au=Lepore%2C+Franco&rft.date=2009-12-01&rft.issn=0028-3932&rft.volume=47&rft.issue=14&rft.spage=3236&rft.epage=3243&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.neuropsychologia.2009.07.025&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0028-3932&client=summon |