The cognitive impact of interactive design features for learning complex materials in medical education

To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of func...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inComputers and education Vol. 71; pp. 198 - 205
Main Authors Song, Hyuksoon S., Pusic, Martin, Nick, Michael W., Sarpel, Umut, Plass, Jan L., Kalet, Adina L.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Elsevier Ltd 01.02.2014
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0360-1315
1873-782X
DOI10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017

Cover

Loading…
Abstract To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications. •Low cognitive interactivity lead to better performance in a transfer task.•Students in the high cognitive interactivity spend more time on the materials.•Cognitive effort that manipulates contents may cause extraneous cognitive load.•Instructional designers must consider cognitive load in interaction design.
AbstractList To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications.To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications.
To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications. •Low cognitive interactivity lead to better performance in a transfer task.•Students in the high cognitive interactivity spend more time on the materials.•Cognitive effort that manipulates contents may cause extraneous cognitive load.•Instructional designers must consider cognitive load in interaction design.
To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications.
To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model’s features may have overtaxed subjects’ cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications.
Author Song, Hyuksoon S.
Sarpel, Umut
Nick, Michael W.
Kalet, Adina L.
Pusic, Martin
Plass, Jan L.
AuthorAffiliation b School of Medicine, New York University, United States
c Mount Sinai School of Medicine, United States
d Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, United States
a School of Education, Georgian Court University, 900 Lakewood Ave., Lakewood, NJ 08701, United States
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: d Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, United States
– name: b School of Medicine, New York University, United States
– name: a School of Education, Georgian Court University, 900 Lakewood Ave., Lakewood, NJ 08701, United States
– name: c Mount Sinai School of Medicine, United States
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Hyuksoon S.
  surname: Song
  fullname: Song, Hyuksoon S.
  email: hyuksong@gmail.com
  organization: School of Education, Georgian Court University, 900 Lakewood Ave., Lakewood, NJ 08701, United States
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Martin
  surname: Pusic
  fullname: Pusic, Martin
  organization: School of Medicine, New York University, United States
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Michael W.
  surname: Nick
  fullname: Nick, Michael W.
  organization: School of Medicine, New York University, United States
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Umut
  surname: Sarpel
  fullname: Sarpel, Umut
  organization: Mount Sinai School of Medicine, United States
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Jan L.
  orcidid: 0000-0001-5161-6989
  surname: Plass
  fullname: Plass, Jan L.
  organization: Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, United States
– sequence: 6
  givenname: Adina L.
  surname: Kalet
  fullname: Kalet, Adina L.
  organization: School of Medicine, New York University, United States
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27274609$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFUdGO1CAUJWaNO7v6CRoefZkKhRYaE43ZuKvJJr6siW-E0kuXSQsj0In-vdQZN-rLvgAXzjn3cM8FOvPBA0IvKakooe2bXWXCvIdhqWpCWUW6ilDxBG2oFGwrZP3tDG0Ia8mWMtqco4uUdoQQ3vLmGTqvRS14S7oNGu_uAZswepfdAbCb99pkHCx2PkMs5_V2gORGjy3ovERI2IaIJ9DROz_i1cYEP_CsC8HpKRUqnmFwRk-4-DM6u-Cfo6e2vMGL036Jvl5_vLv6tL39cvP56sPt1vBW5LL2YJisNRskwGC5NL0VA--Y7TlQKyTpCkIT0ZNSiVITZizjunzMMMsu0buj7n7piwkDPkc9qX10s44_VdBO_fvi3b0aw0Fx2dVMNEXg9Ukghu8LpKxmlwxMk_YQlqSoZE0rGetW6Ku_ez00-TPdAmiOABNDShHsA4QStaaoduqUolpTVKRTJcXCe_sfz7j8e4zFspseZb8_sqHM-eAgqmQceFMiiWCyGoJ7ROEX9WHAGQ
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_021_09624_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jclinepi_2024_111399
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2024_102564
crossref_primary_10_7759_cureus_12059
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_intmar_2020_01_002
crossref_primary_10_1080_10494820_2021_1999274
crossref_primary_10_3389_feduc_2023_1134823
crossref_primary_10_1075_rcl_00126_sun
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_018_0092_9
crossref_primary_10_3390_informatics3040020
crossref_primary_10_1080_10410236_2019_1654172
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10639_022_10912_0
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_021_09607_8
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10209_021_00808_0
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbe2_184
crossref_primary_10_1080_07380569_2022_2037296
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11357_022_00711_3
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10956_018_9760_2
crossref_primary_10_1097_SIH_0000000000000254
crossref_primary_10_1111_jcal_12454
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10758_016_9297_5
crossref_primary_10_17718_tojde_803360
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2015_10_011
crossref_primary_10_1111_test_12324
crossref_primary_10_1177_0735633117706047
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2022_102974
crossref_primary_10_4085_120151
crossref_primary_10_1111_bjet_13397
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_athoracsur_2022_04_049
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2018_05_019
crossref_primary_10_1111_jonm_12919
crossref_primary_10_1002_ase_1987
crossref_primary_10_1007_s40670_019_00702_8
crossref_primary_10_1021_acs_jchemed_3c00498
crossref_primary_10_1088_1742_6596_2611_1_012013
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01191
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cpem_2016_05_002
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10639_022_11501_x
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11042_022_12296_2
crossref_primary_10_1097_j_pain_0000000000001146
crossref_primary_10_1002_ase_2172
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2014_03_041
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tria_2023_100247
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compind_2018_08_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2017_10_005
crossref_primary_10_1007_s40692_023_00268_w
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2017_12_002
Cites_doi 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.806
10.1007/s11251-004-7685-z
10.1023/A:1022193728205
10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.018
10.1016/j.compedu.2008.08.008
10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.003
10.1007/s12528-012-9060-4
10.1016/j.compedu.2011.11.006
10.1007/s11251-009-9096-7
10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00022-1
10.1007/s10648-007-9045-4
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.02.006
10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
10.1007/s10648-011-9173-8
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.008
10.1002/acp.1248
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.03.001
10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.015
10.1007/BF00897978
10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.021
10.1037/0022-0663.93.2.390
10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2
10.1007/s10648-007-9051-6
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.005
10.1002/acp.1762
10.1207/s1532690xci1203_1
10.1016/j.chb.2006.10.002
10.1037/a0026609
10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8
10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0
10.1007/BF03172968
10.3109/0142159X.2012.706727
10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.010
10.1007/s11251-009-9108-7
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.003
10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00018-X
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2013 Elsevier Ltd
Copyright_xml – notice: 2013 Elsevier Ltd
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
NPM
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017
DatabaseName CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic

PubMed

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Education
EISSN 1873-782X
EndPage 205
ExternalDocumentID PMC4892375
27274609
10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017
S0360131513002807
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NLM NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 LM009538
GroupedDBID --K
--M
-~X
.DC
.~1
0R~
1B1
1RT
1~.
1~5
29F
4.4
41~
457
4G.
5GY
5VS
6J9
6TJ
7-5
71M
8P~
9JO
AABNK
AACTN
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFJI
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKOC
AALRI
AAOAW
AAQFI
AAQXK
AAXUO
ABFNM
ABFRF
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABMMH
ABTAH
ABXDB
ABYKQ
ACDAQ
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACRLP
ACTDY
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADHUB
ADIYS
ADMUD
AEBSH
AEFWE
AEKER
AENEX
AFFNX
AFKWA
AFTJW
AGHFR
AGUBO
AGYEJ
AHHHB
AI.
AIEXJ
AIKHN
AITUG
AJBFU
AJOXV
AKYCK
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMFUW
AMRAJ
AOMHK
ASPBG
AVARZ
AVWKF
AXJTR
AZFZN
BKOJK
BLXMC
CS3
DU5
EBS
EFJIC
EFLBG
EJD
EO8
EO9
EP2
EP3
F5P
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
FNPLU
FYGXN
G-2
G-Q
GBLVA
HF~
HLZ
HMY
HVGLF
HZ~
IHE
J1W
KOM
LG9
M3Y
M41
MO0
N9A
NHB
O-L
O9-
OAUVE
OHT
OZT
P-8
P-9
P2P
PC.
PRBVW
Q38
R2-
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SBC
SDF
SDG
SDP
SES
SEW
SPCBC
SSB
SSO
SSS
SSZ
T5K
UNMZH
VH1
WUQ
XSW
ZY4
~G-
AATTM
AAXKI
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADMHG
ADNMO
ADVLN
AEIPS
AEUPX
AFJKZ
AFPUW
AFXIZ
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AGRNS
AIGII
AIIUN
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
ANKPU
APXCP
BNPGV
CITATION
SSH
NPM
PKN
7X8
EFKBS
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c467t-c4bec382a3d8eedf48cbf7d493fb4e1f7809beca07b01f7778003cf34a645c3f3
IEDL.DBID .~1
ISSN 0360-1315
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 18:12:17 EDT 2025
Thu Sep 04 22:04:54 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:08:34 EST 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:55:33 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:31:56 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:36:02 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Keywords Cognitive interactivity
Medical education
Cognitive load
Behavioral interactivity
Multimedia learning
Language English
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c467t-c4bec382a3d8eedf48cbf7d493fb4e1f7809beca07b01f7778003cf34a645c3f3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0001-5161-6989
OpenAccessLink http://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017
PMID 27274609
PQID 1835683395
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 8
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4892375
proquest_miscellaneous_1835683395
pubmed_primary_27274609
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2014-02-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2014-02-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 02
  year: 2014
  text: 2014-02-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
PublicationTitle Computers and education
PublicationTitleAlternate Comput Educ
PublicationYear 2014
Publisher Elsevier Ltd
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Ltd
References Cook, Mitchell, Goldin-Meadow (bib4) 2008; 106
Domagk, Schwartz, Plass (bib5) 2010; 26
Seufert, Brünken (bib34) 2006; 20
Huff, Schwan (bib13) 2011; 25
Schnotz, Böckheler, Grzondziel (bib31) 1999; 14
Schwan, Riempp (bib32) 2004; 14
Kennedy (bib18) 2004; 15
Bodemer, Ploetzner, Bruchmüller, Häcker (bib2) 2005; 33
Sweller, van Merriënboer, Paas (bib38) 1998; 10
Rasch, Schnotz (bib29) 2009; 19
Govaere Jan, de Kruif, Valcke (bib10) 2012; 58
Paas, Sweller (bib26) 2012; 24
Kalyuga (bib17) 2012; 24
Kalet, Song, Sarpel, Schwartz, Brenner, Ark (bib14) 2012; 34
Lowe (bib22) 2004; 14
Merriënboer, Sweller (bib25) 2005; 17
Gegenfurtner, Seppänen (bib7) 2013; 63
Seufert (bib33) 2003; 13
Um, Plass, Hayward, Homer (bib40) 2012; 104
Glenberg, Goldberg, Zhu (bib9) 2011; 39
Barsalou (bib1) 2008; 59
Paas, Tuovinen, Tabbers, Van Gerven (bib27) 2003; 38
Plass, J. L., Heidig, S., Hayward, E. O., Homer, B. D., & Um, E. J. (in press). Emotional design in multimedia learning: effects of shape and color on affect and learning.
Lowe (bib21) 2003; 13
Schnotz (bib30) 2005
Holzinger, Kickmeier-Rust, Wassertheurer, Hessinger (bib11) 2009; 52
Sweller (bib36) 2005
Mayer, Dow, Mayer (bib24) 2003; 95
Homer, Plass (bib12) 2009; 38
Kalyuga (bib16) 2010
Mayer, Chandler (bib23) 2001; 93
Gavora, Hannafin (bib6) 1995; 22
Triona, L. M., & Klahr, D. (2010). Point and click or Grab and Heft: comparing the influence of physical and virtual instructional materials on elementary school students' ability to design experiments, (February 2013), 37–41.
.
Seufert, Jänen, Brünken (bib35) 2007; 23
Zacharia, Olympiou (bib42) 2011; 21
Gerjets, Scheiter, Opfermann, Hesse, Eysink (bib8) 2009; 25
Koning, Tabbers (bib19) 2011; 23
Chandler (bib3) 2009; 25
Kalyuga (bib15) 2007; 19
Wouters, Tabbers, Paas (bib41) 2007; 19
Kraiger, Jerden (bib20) 2007
Sweller, Chandler (bib37) 1994; 12
Kalyuga (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib17) 2012; 24
Mayer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib24) 2003; 95
Wouters (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib41) 2007; 19
Kraiger (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib20) 2007
Glenberg (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib9) 2011; 39
Kalyuga (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib15) 2007; 19
Mayer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib23) 2001; 93
Govaere Jan (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib10) 2012; 58
Merriënboer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib25) 2005; 17
Paas (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib26) 2012; 24
Sweller (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib36) 2005
Kennedy (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib18) 2004; 15
Barsalou (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib1) 2008; 59
Chandler (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib3) 2009; 25
Kalet (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib14) 2012; 34
Seufert (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib35) 2007; 23
Sweller (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib37) 1994; 12
10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib28
Schnotz (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib31) 1999; 14
Schwan (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib32) 2004; 14
Zacharia (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib42) 2011; 21
Huff (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib13) 2011; 25
Homer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib12) 2009; 38
Domagk (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib5) 2010; 26
Lowe (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib21) 2003; 13
Seufert (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib34) 2006; 20
Gavora (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib6) 1995; 22
Kalyuga (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib16) 2010
Bodemer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib2) 2005; 33
Gegenfurtner (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib7) 2013; 63
Cook (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib4) 2008; 106
Paas (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib27) 2003; 38
Seufert (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib33) 2003; 13
Lowe (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib22) 2004; 14
Rasch (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib29) 2009; 19
Um (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib40) 2012; 104
Koning (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib19) 2011; 23
Sweller (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib38) 1998; 10
10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib39
Gerjets (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib8) 2009; 25
Holzinger (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib11) 2009; 52
Schnotz (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib30) 2005
22917265 - Med Teach. 2012;34(10):833-9
17705682 - Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:617-45
17560971 - Cognition. 2008 Feb;106(2):1047-58
References_xml – volume: 106
  start-page: 1047
  year: 2008
  end-page: 1058
  ident: bib4
  article-title: Gesturing makes learning last
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 13
  start-page: 157
  year: 2003
  end-page: 176
  ident: bib21
  article-title: Animation and learning: selective processing of information in dynamic graphics
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
– volume: 59
  start-page: 617
  year: 2008
  end-page: 645
  ident: bib1
  article-title: Grounded cognition
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
– start-page: 19
  year: 2005
  end-page: 30
  ident: bib36
  article-title: Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning
  publication-title: Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning
– volume: 19
  start-page: 411
  year: 2009
  end-page: 422
  ident: bib29
  article-title: Interactive and non-interactive pictures in multimedia learning environments: effects on learning outcomes and learning efficiency
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
– volume: 19
  start-page: 327
  year: 2007
  end-page: 342
  ident: bib41
  article-title: Interactivity in video-based models
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
– volume: 15
  start-page: 43
  year: 2004
  end-page: 61
  ident: bib18
  article-title: Promoting cognition in multimedia interactivity research
  publication-title: Journal of Interactive Learning Research
– volume: 20
  start-page: 321
  year: 2006
  end-page: 331
  ident: bib34
  article-title: Cognitive load and the format of instructional aids for coherence formation
  publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 58
  start-page: 1076
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1084
  ident: bib10
  article-title: Differential impact of unguided versus guided use of a multimedia introduction to equine obstetrics in veterinary education
  publication-title: Computers & Education
– volume: 25
  start-page: 389
  year: 2009
  end-page: 392
  ident: bib3
  article-title: Dynamic visualisations and hypermedia: beyond the “Wow” factor
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
– volume: 14
  start-page: 245
  year: 1999
  end-page: 265
  ident: bib31
  article-title: Individual and co-operative learning with interactive animated pictures
  publication-title: European Journal of Psychology of Education
– start-page: 49
  year: 2005
  end-page: 69
  ident: bib30
  article-title: An integrated model of text and picture comprehension
  publication-title: The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning
– volume: 13
  start-page: 227
  year: 2003
  end-page: 237
  ident: bib33
  article-title: Supporting coherence formation in learning from multiple representations
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
– volume: 63
  start-page: 393
  year: 2013
  end-page: 403
  ident: bib7
  article-title: Transfer of expertise: an eye tracking and think aloud study using dynamic medical visualizations
  publication-title: Computers & Education
– volume: 33
  start-page: 73
  year: 2005
  end-page: 95
  ident: bib2
  article-title: Supporting learning with interactive multimedia through active integration of representations
  publication-title: Instructional Science
– volume: 95
  start-page: 806
  year: 2003
  end-page: 812
  ident: bib24
  article-title: Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: what works in the design of agent-based microworlds?
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
– start-page: 48
  year: 2010
  end-page: 64
  ident: bib16
  article-title: Schema acquisition and sources of cognitive load
  publication-title: Cognitive load theory
– volume: 24
  start-page: 182
  year: 2012
  end-page: 208
  ident: bib17
  article-title: Interactive distance education: a cognitive load perspective
  publication-title: Journal of Computing in Higher Education
– volume: 19
  start-page: 387
  year: 2007
  end-page: 399
  ident: bib15
  article-title: Enhancing instructional efficiency of interactive e-learning environments: a cognitive load perspective
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
– volume: 14
  start-page: 257
  year: 2004
  end-page: 274
  ident: bib22
  article-title: Interrogation of a dynamic visualization during learning
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
– volume: 10
  start-page: 251
  year: 1998
  end-page: 296
  ident: bib38
  article-title: Cognitive architecture and instructional design
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
– volume: 38
  start-page: 259
  year: 2009
  end-page: 276
  ident: bib12
  article-title: Expertise reversal for iconic representations in science visualizations
  publication-title: Instructional Science
– volume: 14
  start-page: 293
  year: 2004
  end-page: 305
  ident: bib32
  article-title: The cognitive benefits of interactive videos: learning to tie nautical knots
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
– reference: Triona, L. M., & Klahr, D. (2010). Point and click or Grab and Heft: comparing the influence of physical and virtual instructional materials on elementary school students' ability to design experiments, (February 2013), 37–41.
– volume: 34
  start-page: 833
  year: 2012
  end-page: 839
  ident: bib14
  article-title: Just enough, but not too much interactivity leads to better clinical skills performance after a computer assisted learning module
  publication-title: Medical Teacher
– volume: 12
  start-page: 185
  year: 1994
  end-page: 233
  ident: bib37
  article-title: Why some material is difficult to learn
  publication-title: Cognition and Instruction
– volume: 21
  start-page: 317
  year: 2011
  end-page: 331
  ident: bib42
  article-title: Physical versus virtual manipulative experimentation in physics learning
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
– volume: 25
  start-page: 360
  year: 2009
  end-page: 370
  ident: bib8
  article-title: Learning with hypermedia: the influence of representational formats and different levels of learner control on performance and learning behavior
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
– volume: 25
  start-page: 878
  year: 2011
  end-page: 886
  ident: bib13
  article-title: Integrating information from two pictorial animations: complexity and cognitive prerequisites influence performance
  publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 104
  start-page: 485
  year: 2012
  end-page: 498
  ident: bib40
  article-title: Emotional design in multimedia learning
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
– volume: 17
  start-page: 147
  year: 2005
  end-page: 177
  ident: bib25
  article-title: Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
– volume: 23
  start-page: 1055
  year: 2007
  end-page: 1071
  ident: bib35
  article-title: The impact of intrinsic cognitive load on the effectiveness of graphical help for coherence formation
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
– volume: 24
  start-page: 27
  year: 2012
  end-page: 45
  ident: bib26
  article-title: An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
– volume: 38
  start-page: 37
  year: 2003
  end-page: 41
  ident: bib27
  article-title: Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory
  publication-title: Educational Psychologist
– reference: Plass, J. L., Heidig, S., Hayward, E. O., Homer, B. D., & Um, E. J. (in press). Emotional design in multimedia learning: effects of shape and color on affect and learning.
– reference: .
– volume: 52
  start-page: 292
  year: 2009
  end-page: 301
  ident: bib11
  article-title: Learning performance with interactive simulations in medical education: lessons learned from results of learning complex physiological models with the HAEMOdynamics SIMulator
  publication-title: Computers & Education
– volume: 23
  start-page: 501
  year: 2011
  end-page: 521
  ident: bib19
  article-title: Facilitating understanding of movements in dynamic visualizations: an embodied perspective
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
– volume: 22
  start-page: 445
  year: 1995
  end-page: 477
  ident: bib6
  article-title: Perspectives on the design of human-computer interactions: issues and implications
  publication-title: Instructional Science
– volume: 39
  start-page: 27
  year: 2011
  end-page: 39
  ident: bib9
  article-title: Improving early reading comprehension using embodied CAI
  publication-title: Instructional Science
– start-page: 65
  year: 2007
  end-page: 90
  ident: bib20
  article-title: A meta-analytic investigation of learner control: old findings and new directions
  publication-title: Toward a science of distributed learning
– volume: 93
  start-page: 390
  year: 2001
  end-page: 397
  ident: bib23
  article-title: When learning is just a click away: does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages?
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
– reference: .
– volume: 26
  start-page: 1024
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1033
  ident: bib5
  article-title: Interactivity in multimedia learning: an integrated model
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
– volume: 95
  start-page: 806
  issue: 4
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib24
  article-title: Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: what works in the design of agent-based microworlds?
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.806
– volume: 33
  start-page: 73
  issue: 1
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib2
  article-title: Supporting learning with interactive multimedia through active integration of representations
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-004-7685-z
– volume: 10
  start-page: 251
  issue: 3
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib38
  article-title: Cognitive architecture and instructional design
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1023/A:1022193728205
– start-page: 49
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib30
  article-title: An integrated model of text and picture comprehension
– start-page: 65
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib20
  article-title: A meta-analytic investigation of learner control: old findings and new directions
– volume: 25
  start-page: 389
  issue: 2
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib3
  article-title: Dynamic visualisations and hypermedia: beyond the “Wow” factor
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.018
– volume: 52
  start-page: 292
  issue: 2
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib11
  article-title: Learning performance with interactive simulations in medical education: lessons learned from results of learning complex physiological models with the HAEMOdynamics SIMulator
  publication-title: Computers & Education
  doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2008.08.008
– volume: 26
  start-page: 1024
  issue: 5
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib5
  article-title: Interactivity in multimedia learning: an integrated model
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.003
– start-page: 48
  year: 2010
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib16
  article-title: Schema acquisition and sources of cognitive load
– volume: 24
  start-page: 182
  issue: 3
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib17
  article-title: Interactive distance education: a cognitive load perspective
  publication-title: Journal of Computing in Higher Education
  doi: 10.1007/s12528-012-9060-4
– start-page: 19
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib36
  article-title: Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning
– volume: 58
  start-page: 1076
  issue: 4
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib10
  article-title: Differential impact of unguided versus guided use of a multimedia introduction to equine obstetrics in veterinary education
  publication-title: Computers & Education
  doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2011.11.006
– volume: 39
  start-page: 27
  issue: 1
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib9
  article-title: Improving early reading comprehension using embodied CAI
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9096-7
– volume: 15
  start-page: 43
  issue: 1
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib18
  article-title: Promoting cognition in multimedia interactivity research
  publication-title: Journal of Interactive Learning Research
– volume: 13
  start-page: 227
  issue: 2
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib33
  article-title: Supporting coherence formation in learning from multiple representations
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00022-1
– volume: 19
  start-page: 327
  issue: 3
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib41
  article-title: Interactivity in video-based models
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9045-4
– ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib28
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.02.006
– volume: 59
  start-page: 617
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib1
  article-title: Grounded cognition
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
– volume: 23
  start-page: 501
  issue: 4
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib19
  article-title: Facilitating understanding of movements in dynamic visualizations: an embodied perspective
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9173-8
– volume: 19
  start-page: 411
  issue: 5
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib29
  article-title: Interactive and non-interactive pictures in multimedia learning environments: effects on learning outcomes and learning efficiency
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.008
– volume: 20
  start-page: 321
  issue: 3
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib34
  article-title: Cognitive load and the format of instructional aids for coherence formation
  publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1002/acp.1248
– volume: 21
  start-page: 317
  issue: 3
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib42
  article-title: Physical versus virtual manipulative experimentation in physics learning
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.03.001
– volume: 25
  start-page: 360
  issue: 2
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib8
  article-title: Learning with hypermedia: the influence of representational formats and different levels of learner control on performance and learning behavior
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.015
– volume: 22
  start-page: 445
  issue: 6
  year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib6
  article-title: Perspectives on the design of human-computer interactions: issues and implications
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/BF00897978
– volume: 63
  start-page: 393
  year: 2013
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib7
  article-title: Transfer of expertise: an eye tracking and think aloud study using dynamic medical visualizations
  publication-title: Computers & Education
  doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.021
– volume: 93
  start-page: 390
  issue: 2
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib23
  article-title: When learning is just a click away: does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages?
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.2.390
– volume: 24
  start-page: 27
  issue: 1
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib26
  article-title: An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2
– volume: 19
  start-page: 387
  issue: 3
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib15
  article-title: Enhancing instructional efficiency of interactive e-learning environments: a cognitive load perspective
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9051-6
– volume: 14
  start-page: 293
  issue: 14
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib32
  article-title: The cognitive benefits of interactive videos: learning to tie nautical knots
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.005
– volume: 25
  start-page: 878
  issue: 6
  year: 2011
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib13
  article-title: Integrating information from two pictorial animations: complexity and cognitive prerequisites influence performance
  publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology
  doi: 10.1002/acp.1762
– volume: 12
  start-page: 185
  issue: 3
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib37
  article-title: Why some material is difficult to learn
  publication-title: Cognition and Instruction
  doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci1203_1
– volume: 23
  start-page: 1055
  issue: 3
  year: 2007
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib35
  article-title: The impact of intrinsic cognitive load on the effectiveness of graphical help for coherence formation
  publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior
  doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2006.10.002
– volume: 104
  start-page: 485
  issue: 2
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib40
  article-title: Emotional design in multimedia learning
  publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology
  doi: 10.1037/a0026609
– volume: 38
  start-page: 37
  issue: 1
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib27
  article-title: Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory
  publication-title: Educational Psychologist
  doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8
– volume: 17
  start-page: 147
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib25
  article-title: Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions
  publication-title: Educational Psychology Review
  doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0
– ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib39
– volume: 14
  start-page: 245
  issue: 2
  year: 1999
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib31
  article-title: Individual and co-operative learning with interactive animated pictures
  publication-title: European Journal of Psychology of Education
  doi: 10.1007/BF03172968
– volume: 34
  start-page: 833
  issue: 10
  year: 2012
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib14
  article-title: Just enough, but not too much interactivity leads to better clinical skills performance after a computer assisted learning module
  publication-title: Medical Teacher
  doi: 10.3109/0142159X.2012.706727
– volume: 106
  start-page: 1047
  issue: 2
  year: 2008
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib4
  article-title: Gesturing makes learning last
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.010
– volume: 38
  start-page: 259
  issue: 3
  year: 2009
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib12
  article-title: Expertise reversal for iconic representations in science visualizations
  publication-title: Instructional Science
  doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9108-7
– volume: 14
  start-page: 257
  issue: 3
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib22
  article-title: Interrogation of a dynamic visualization during learning
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.003
– volume: 13
  start-page: 157
  issue: 2
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib21
  article-title: Animation and learning: selective processing of information in dynamic graphics
  publication-title: Learning and Instruction
  doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00018-X
– reference: 22917265 - Med Teach. 2012;34(10):833-9
– reference: 17560971 - Cognition. 2008 Feb;106(2):1047-58
– reference: 17705682 - Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:617-45
SSID ssj0004645
Score 2.321826
Snippet To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of...
To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 198
SubjectTerms Behavioral interactivity
Cognitive interactivity
Cognitive load
Medical education
Multimedia learning
Title The cognitive impact of interactive design features for learning complex materials in medical education
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27274609
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1835683395
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4892375
Volume 71
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3dS8MwEA8yX3wRv50fI4Kvc92SNOnjGMpU9EUF30KbJrqh3XAb-OTf7l2azk0FwZdC2ksbcpe7a3L3O0JOhTFSYnJMnMMvCm-nDNacSJomjxFbJIujFPc7bm7j_gO_ehSPK6RX5cJgWGXQ_aVO99o63GmF2WyNB4PWHeheBIsReCCDmC6Ywc4l4ueffbQXciN9oWIkbiL1VxZPa4jvHuOeCFhB5uFOfd2yX-3TT__zexjlgl262CDrwaGk3XLMm2TFFltYiznEbWyTJ5AEOo8SomVaJB05ikgRPkcK7uY-kIM663E-JxRcWRoKSjxRH3Zu3yk4t6W8Qlf6Wp7wUFt9aYc8XJzf9_rNUF6haUA7TuEK_GOqk7JcgaV0XJnMyZwnzGXctp1UUQIUaSSzCFoS2hEzjvEUptQwx3ZJrRgVdp9Q1ol42k5kZrnjqROJciK3THKRGxarvE54NanaBOxxLIHxoqsgs6EOvNDICx0lGnhRJ2fzbuMSfOOvDqrimF6SIg0G4q-uJxWHNawwPDZJCzuaTTQoPRErxhJRJ3slx-ej6YD7x-MoqRO5JAtzAkTvXn5SDJ49ijdX4FtLcfD_IR-SNWjxMob8iNSmbzN7DC7SNGv4NdAgq93L6_7tJ0FZFJg
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LT-MwEB7xOMBltbzLLmAkrqVpbcfOcYVA5XkBJG5W4thQBGm1FInT_vadcZxCAQmJSyS_EssznpnYM98A7ElrlaLgmLTEXxTRzTnuOZm1bZkStkiRJjmdd5xfpP1rcXIjb2bgoImFIbfKKPtrmR6kdazpxNXsjAaDziXKXgKLkXQhQ5guszAvJFfE2vv_um-CI0OmYurdpu6vYTyde3r5iA5FUA3ygHcaEpd9qqA-GqDv_SjfKKajn_AjWpTsTz3pJZhx1TIlY46OGytwi6zAJm5CrI6LZEPPCCoiBElhbRk8OZh3AejziaEty2JGiVsW_M7dC0PrtmZYHMoe6yse5povrcL10eHVQb8d8yu0LYrHMT6RgFz3cl5qVJVeaFt4VYqM-0K4rlc6ybBHnqgiwZLCcsKt5yLHJbXc8zWYq4aV2wDGe4nIu5kqnPAi9zLTXpaOKyFLy1NdtkA0i2psBB-nHBgPpvEyuzeRFoZoYZLMIC1asD8ZNqrRN74aoBuKmSk2Mqghvhq621DY4Baje5O8csPnJ4NST6aa80y2YL2m-GQ2PbT_RJpkLVBTvDDpQPDd0y3V4C7AeAuNxrWSm9-f8g4s9K_Oz8zZ8cXpL1jEFlE7lP-GufHfZ7eF9tK42A774T9_4hYu
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+cognitive+impact+of+interactive+design+features+for+learning+complex+materials+in+medical+education&rft.jtitle=Computers+and+education&rft.au=Song%2C+Hyuksoon+S.&rft.au=Pusic%2C+Martin&rft.au=Nick%2C+Michael+W.&rft.au=Sarpel%2C+Umut&rft.date=2014-02-01&rft.pub=Elsevier+Ltd&rft.issn=0360-1315&rft.eissn=1873-782X&rft.volume=71&rft.spage=198&rft.epage=205&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.compedu.2013.09.017&rft.externalDocID=S0360131513002807
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0360-1315&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0360-1315&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0360-1315&client=summon