The cognitive impact of interactive design features for learning complex materials in medical education
To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of func...
Saved in:
Published in | Computers and education Vol. 71; pp. 198 - 205 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Elsevier Ltd
01.02.2014
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0360-1315 1873-782X |
DOI | 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications.
•Low cognitive interactivity lead to better performance in a transfer task.•Students in the high cognitive interactivity spend more time on the materials.•Cognitive effort that manipulates contents may cause extraneous cognitive load.•Instructional designers must consider cognitive load in interaction design. |
---|---|
AbstractList | To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications.To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications. To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications. •Low cognitive interactivity lead to better performance in a transfer task.•Students in the high cognitive interactivity spend more time on the materials.•Cognitive effort that manipulates contents may cause extraneous cognitive load.•Instructional designers must consider cognitive load in interaction design. To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model's features may have overtaxed subjects' cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications. To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of stroke syndromes, this study manipulated the way in which subjects interacted with a graphical model of the brain and examined the impact of functional changes on learning outcomes. It was hypothesized that behavioral interactions that were behaviorally more engaging and which required deeper consideration of the model would result in heightened cognitive interaction and better learning than those whose manipulation required less deliberate behavioral and cognitive processing. One hundred forty four students were randomly assigned to four conditions whose model controls incorporated features that required different levels of behavioral and cognitive interaction: Movie (low behavioral/low cognitive, n = 40), Slider (high behavioral/low cognitive, n = 36), Click (low behavioral/high cognitive, n = 30), and Drag (high behavioral/high cognitive, n = 38). Analysis of Covariates (ANCOVA) showed that students who received the treatments associated with lower cognitive interactivity (Movie and Slider) performed better on a transfer task than those receiving the module associated with high cognitive interactivity (Click and Drag, partial eta squared = .03). In addition, the students in the high cognitive interactivity conditions spent significantly more time on the stroke locator activity than other conditions (partial eta squared = .36). The results suggest that interaction with controls that were tightly coupled with the model and whose manipulation required deliberate consideration of the model’s features may have overtaxed subjects’ cognitive resources. Cognitive effort that facilitated manipulation of content, though directed at the model, may have resulted in extraneous cognitive load, impeding subjects in recognizing the deeper, global relationships in the materials. Instructional designers must, therefore, keep in mind that the way in which functional affordances are integrated with the content can shape both behavioral and cognitive processing, and has significant cognitive load implications. |
Author | Song, Hyuksoon S. Sarpel, Umut Nick, Michael W. Kalet, Adina L. Pusic, Martin Plass, Jan L. |
AuthorAffiliation | b School of Medicine, New York University, United States c Mount Sinai School of Medicine, United States d Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, United States a School of Education, Georgian Court University, 900 Lakewood Ave., Lakewood, NJ 08701, United States |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: d Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, United States – name: b School of Medicine, New York University, United States – name: a School of Education, Georgian Court University, 900 Lakewood Ave., Lakewood, NJ 08701, United States – name: c Mount Sinai School of Medicine, United States |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Hyuksoon S. surname: Song fullname: Song, Hyuksoon S. email: hyuksong@gmail.com organization: School of Education, Georgian Court University, 900 Lakewood Ave., Lakewood, NJ 08701, United States – sequence: 2 givenname: Martin surname: Pusic fullname: Pusic, Martin organization: School of Medicine, New York University, United States – sequence: 3 givenname: Michael W. surname: Nick fullname: Nick, Michael W. organization: School of Medicine, New York University, United States – sequence: 4 givenname: Umut surname: Sarpel fullname: Sarpel, Umut organization: Mount Sinai School of Medicine, United States – sequence: 5 givenname: Jan L. orcidid: 0000-0001-5161-6989 surname: Plass fullname: Plass, Jan L. organization: Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, United States – sequence: 6 givenname: Adina L. surname: Kalet fullname: Kalet, Adina L. organization: School of Medicine, New York University, United States |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27274609$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqFUdGO1CAUJWaNO7v6CRoefZkKhRYaE43ZuKvJJr6siW-E0kuXSQsj0In-vdQZN-rLvgAXzjn3cM8FOvPBA0IvKakooe2bXWXCvIdhqWpCWUW6ilDxBG2oFGwrZP3tDG0Ia8mWMtqco4uUdoQQ3vLmGTqvRS14S7oNGu_uAZswepfdAbCb99pkHCx2PkMs5_V2gORGjy3ovERI2IaIJ9DROz_i1cYEP_CsC8HpKRUqnmFwRk-4-DM6u-Cfo6e2vMGL036Jvl5_vLv6tL39cvP56sPt1vBW5LL2YJisNRskwGC5NL0VA--Y7TlQKyTpCkIT0ZNSiVITZizjunzMMMsu0buj7n7piwkDPkc9qX10s44_VdBO_fvi3b0aw0Fx2dVMNEXg9Ukghu8LpKxmlwxMk_YQlqSoZE0rGetW6Ku_ez00-TPdAmiOABNDShHsA4QStaaoduqUolpTVKRTJcXCe_sfz7j8e4zFspseZb8_sqHM-eAgqmQceFMiiWCyGoJ7ROEX9WHAGQ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_021_09624_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jclinepi_2024_111399 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2024_102564 crossref_primary_10_7759_cureus_12059 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_intmar_2020_01_002 crossref_primary_10_1080_10494820_2021_1999274 crossref_primary_10_3389_feduc_2023_1134823 crossref_primary_10_1075_rcl_00126_sun crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_018_0092_9 crossref_primary_10_3390_informatics3040020 crossref_primary_10_1080_10410236_2019_1654172 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10639_022_10912_0 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10648_021_09607_8 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10209_021_00808_0 crossref_primary_10_1002_hbe2_184 crossref_primary_10_1080_07380569_2022_2037296 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11357_022_00711_3 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10956_018_9760_2 crossref_primary_10_1097_SIH_0000000000000254 crossref_primary_10_1111_jcal_12454 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10758_016_9297_5 crossref_primary_10_17718_tojde_803360 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2015_10_011 crossref_primary_10_1111_test_12324 crossref_primary_10_1177_0735633117706047 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2022_102974 crossref_primary_10_4085_120151 crossref_primary_10_1111_bjet_13397 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_athoracsur_2022_04_049 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2018_05_019 crossref_primary_10_1111_jonm_12919 crossref_primary_10_1002_ase_1987 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40670_019_00702_8 crossref_primary_10_1021_acs_jchemed_3c00498 crossref_primary_10_1088_1742_6596_2611_1_012013 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01191 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cpem_2016_05_002 crossref_primary_10_1007_s10639_022_11501_x crossref_primary_10_1007_s11042_022_12296_2 crossref_primary_10_1097_j_pain_0000000000001146 crossref_primary_10_1002_ase_2172 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2014_03_041 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tria_2023_100247 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compind_2018_08_005 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2017_10_005 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40692_023_00268_w crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2017_12_002 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.806 10.1007/s11251-004-7685-z 10.1023/A:1022193728205 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.018 10.1016/j.compedu.2008.08.008 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.003 10.1007/s12528-012-9060-4 10.1016/j.compedu.2011.11.006 10.1007/s11251-009-9096-7 10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00022-1 10.1007/s10648-007-9045-4 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.02.006 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 10.1007/s10648-011-9173-8 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.008 10.1002/acp.1248 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.03.001 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.015 10.1007/BF00897978 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.021 10.1037/0022-0663.93.2.390 10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2 10.1007/s10648-007-9051-6 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.005 10.1002/acp.1762 10.1207/s1532690xci1203_1 10.1016/j.chb.2006.10.002 10.1037/a0026609 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8 10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0 10.1007/BF03172968 10.3109/0142159X.2012.706727 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.010 10.1007/s11251-009-9108-7 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.003 10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00018-X |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2013 Elsevier Ltd |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2013 Elsevier Ltd |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic PubMed |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Education |
EISSN | 1873-782X |
EndPage | 205 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC4892375 27274609 10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017 S0360131513002807 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NLM NIH HHS grantid: R01 LM009538 |
GroupedDBID | --K --M -~X .DC .~1 0R~ 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29F 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 5GY 5VS 6J9 6TJ 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JO AABNK AACTN AAEDT AAEDW AAFJI AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAXUO ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABMAC ABMMH ABTAH ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFS ACHQT ACRLP ACTDY ADBBV ADEZE ADHUB ADIYS ADMUD AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AENEX AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AGHFR AGUBO AGYEJ AHHHB AI. AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV AKYCK ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ AOMHK ASPBG AVARZ AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CS3 DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q GBLVA HF~ HLZ HMY HVGLF HZ~ IHE J1W KOM LG9 M3Y M41 MO0 N9A NHB O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. PRBVW Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SBC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SPCBC SSB SSO SSS SSZ T5K UNMZH VH1 WUQ XSW ZY4 ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADMHG ADNMO ADVLN AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AFXIZ AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH NPM PKN 7X8 EFKBS 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c467t-c4bec382a3d8eedf48cbf7d493fb4e1f7809beca07b01f7778003cf34a645c3f3 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0360-1315 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:12:17 EDT 2025 Thu Sep 04 22:04:54 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:08:34 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:55:33 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:31:56 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:36:02 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | Cognitive interactivity Medical education Cognitive load Behavioral interactivity Multimedia learning |
Language | English |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c467t-c4bec382a3d8eedf48cbf7d493fb4e1f7809beca07b01f7778003cf34a645c3f3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0001-5161-6989 |
OpenAccessLink | http://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017 |
PMID | 27274609 |
PQID | 1835683395 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 8 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4892375 proquest_miscellaneous_1835683395 pubmed_primary_27274609 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_compedu_2013_09_017 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2014-02-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2014-02-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2014 text: 2014-02-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Computers and education |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Comput Educ |
PublicationYear | 2014 |
Publisher | Elsevier Ltd |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Ltd |
References | Cook, Mitchell, Goldin-Meadow (bib4) 2008; 106 Domagk, Schwartz, Plass (bib5) 2010; 26 Seufert, Brünken (bib34) 2006; 20 Huff, Schwan (bib13) 2011; 25 Schnotz, Böckheler, Grzondziel (bib31) 1999; 14 Schwan, Riempp (bib32) 2004; 14 Kennedy (bib18) 2004; 15 Bodemer, Ploetzner, Bruchmüller, Häcker (bib2) 2005; 33 Sweller, van Merriënboer, Paas (bib38) 1998; 10 Rasch, Schnotz (bib29) 2009; 19 Govaere Jan, de Kruif, Valcke (bib10) 2012; 58 Paas, Sweller (bib26) 2012; 24 Kalyuga (bib17) 2012; 24 Kalet, Song, Sarpel, Schwartz, Brenner, Ark (bib14) 2012; 34 Lowe (bib22) 2004; 14 Merriënboer, Sweller (bib25) 2005; 17 Gegenfurtner, Seppänen (bib7) 2013; 63 Seufert (bib33) 2003; 13 Um, Plass, Hayward, Homer (bib40) 2012; 104 Glenberg, Goldberg, Zhu (bib9) 2011; 39 Barsalou (bib1) 2008; 59 Paas, Tuovinen, Tabbers, Van Gerven (bib27) 2003; 38 Plass, J. L., Heidig, S., Hayward, E. O., Homer, B. D., & Um, E. J. (in press). Emotional design in multimedia learning: effects of shape and color on affect and learning. Lowe (bib21) 2003; 13 Schnotz (bib30) 2005 Holzinger, Kickmeier-Rust, Wassertheurer, Hessinger (bib11) 2009; 52 Sweller (bib36) 2005 Mayer, Dow, Mayer (bib24) 2003; 95 Homer, Plass (bib12) 2009; 38 Kalyuga (bib16) 2010 Mayer, Chandler (bib23) 2001; 93 Gavora, Hannafin (bib6) 1995; 22 Triona, L. M., & Klahr, D. (2010). Point and click or Grab and Heft: comparing the influence of physical and virtual instructional materials on elementary school students' ability to design experiments, (February 2013), 37–41. . Seufert, Jänen, Brünken (bib35) 2007; 23 Zacharia, Olympiou (bib42) 2011; 21 Gerjets, Scheiter, Opfermann, Hesse, Eysink (bib8) 2009; 25 Koning, Tabbers (bib19) 2011; 23 Chandler (bib3) 2009; 25 Kalyuga (bib15) 2007; 19 Wouters, Tabbers, Paas (bib41) 2007; 19 Kraiger, Jerden (bib20) 2007 Sweller, Chandler (bib37) 1994; 12 Kalyuga (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib17) 2012; 24 Mayer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib24) 2003; 95 Wouters (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib41) 2007; 19 Kraiger (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib20) 2007 Glenberg (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib9) 2011; 39 Kalyuga (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib15) 2007; 19 Mayer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib23) 2001; 93 Govaere Jan (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib10) 2012; 58 Merriënboer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib25) 2005; 17 Paas (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib26) 2012; 24 Sweller (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib36) 2005 Kennedy (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib18) 2004; 15 Barsalou (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib1) 2008; 59 Chandler (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib3) 2009; 25 Kalet (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib14) 2012; 34 Seufert (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib35) 2007; 23 Sweller (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib37) 1994; 12 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib28 Schnotz (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib31) 1999; 14 Schwan (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib32) 2004; 14 Zacharia (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib42) 2011; 21 Huff (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib13) 2011; 25 Homer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib12) 2009; 38 Domagk (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib5) 2010; 26 Lowe (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib21) 2003; 13 Seufert (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib34) 2006; 20 Gavora (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib6) 1995; 22 Kalyuga (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib16) 2010 Bodemer (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib2) 2005; 33 Gegenfurtner (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib7) 2013; 63 Cook (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib4) 2008; 106 Paas (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib27) 2003; 38 Seufert (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib33) 2003; 13 Lowe (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib22) 2004; 14 Rasch (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib29) 2009; 19 Um (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib40) 2012; 104 Koning (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib19) 2011; 23 Sweller (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib38) 1998; 10 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib39 Gerjets (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib8) 2009; 25 Holzinger (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib11) 2009; 52 Schnotz (10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib30) 2005 22917265 - Med Teach. 2012;34(10):833-9 17705682 - Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:617-45 17560971 - Cognition. 2008 Feb;106(2):1047-58 |
References_xml | – volume: 106 start-page: 1047 year: 2008 end-page: 1058 ident: bib4 article-title: Gesturing makes learning last publication-title: Cognition – volume: 13 start-page: 157 year: 2003 end-page: 176 ident: bib21 article-title: Animation and learning: selective processing of information in dynamic graphics publication-title: Learning and Instruction – volume: 59 start-page: 617 year: 2008 end-page: 645 ident: bib1 article-title: Grounded cognition publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology – start-page: 19 year: 2005 end-page: 30 ident: bib36 article-title: Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning publication-title: Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning – volume: 19 start-page: 411 year: 2009 end-page: 422 ident: bib29 article-title: Interactive and non-interactive pictures in multimedia learning environments: effects on learning outcomes and learning efficiency publication-title: Learning and Instruction – volume: 19 start-page: 327 year: 2007 end-page: 342 ident: bib41 article-title: Interactivity in video-based models publication-title: Educational Psychology Review – volume: 15 start-page: 43 year: 2004 end-page: 61 ident: bib18 article-title: Promoting cognition in multimedia interactivity research publication-title: Journal of Interactive Learning Research – volume: 20 start-page: 321 year: 2006 end-page: 331 ident: bib34 article-title: Cognitive load and the format of instructional aids for coherence formation publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology – volume: 58 start-page: 1076 year: 2012 end-page: 1084 ident: bib10 article-title: Differential impact of unguided versus guided use of a multimedia introduction to equine obstetrics in veterinary education publication-title: Computers & Education – volume: 25 start-page: 389 year: 2009 end-page: 392 ident: bib3 article-title: Dynamic visualisations and hypermedia: beyond the “Wow” factor publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior – volume: 14 start-page: 245 year: 1999 end-page: 265 ident: bib31 article-title: Individual and co-operative learning with interactive animated pictures publication-title: European Journal of Psychology of Education – start-page: 49 year: 2005 end-page: 69 ident: bib30 article-title: An integrated model of text and picture comprehension publication-title: The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning – volume: 13 start-page: 227 year: 2003 end-page: 237 ident: bib33 article-title: Supporting coherence formation in learning from multiple representations publication-title: Learning and Instruction – volume: 63 start-page: 393 year: 2013 end-page: 403 ident: bib7 article-title: Transfer of expertise: an eye tracking and think aloud study using dynamic medical visualizations publication-title: Computers & Education – volume: 33 start-page: 73 year: 2005 end-page: 95 ident: bib2 article-title: Supporting learning with interactive multimedia through active integration of representations publication-title: Instructional Science – volume: 95 start-page: 806 year: 2003 end-page: 812 ident: bib24 article-title: Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: what works in the design of agent-based microworlds? publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology – start-page: 48 year: 2010 end-page: 64 ident: bib16 article-title: Schema acquisition and sources of cognitive load publication-title: Cognitive load theory – volume: 24 start-page: 182 year: 2012 end-page: 208 ident: bib17 article-title: Interactive distance education: a cognitive load perspective publication-title: Journal of Computing in Higher Education – volume: 19 start-page: 387 year: 2007 end-page: 399 ident: bib15 article-title: Enhancing instructional efficiency of interactive e-learning environments: a cognitive load perspective publication-title: Educational Psychology Review – volume: 14 start-page: 257 year: 2004 end-page: 274 ident: bib22 article-title: Interrogation of a dynamic visualization during learning publication-title: Learning and Instruction – volume: 10 start-page: 251 year: 1998 end-page: 296 ident: bib38 article-title: Cognitive architecture and instructional design publication-title: Educational Psychology Review – volume: 38 start-page: 259 year: 2009 end-page: 276 ident: bib12 article-title: Expertise reversal for iconic representations in science visualizations publication-title: Instructional Science – volume: 14 start-page: 293 year: 2004 end-page: 305 ident: bib32 article-title: The cognitive benefits of interactive videos: learning to tie nautical knots publication-title: Learning and Instruction – reference: Triona, L. M., & Klahr, D. (2010). Point and click or Grab and Heft: comparing the influence of physical and virtual instructional materials on elementary school students' ability to design experiments, (February 2013), 37–41. – volume: 34 start-page: 833 year: 2012 end-page: 839 ident: bib14 article-title: Just enough, but not too much interactivity leads to better clinical skills performance after a computer assisted learning module publication-title: Medical Teacher – volume: 12 start-page: 185 year: 1994 end-page: 233 ident: bib37 article-title: Why some material is difficult to learn publication-title: Cognition and Instruction – volume: 21 start-page: 317 year: 2011 end-page: 331 ident: bib42 article-title: Physical versus virtual manipulative experimentation in physics learning publication-title: Learning and Instruction – volume: 25 start-page: 360 year: 2009 end-page: 370 ident: bib8 article-title: Learning with hypermedia: the influence of representational formats and different levels of learner control on performance and learning behavior publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior – volume: 25 start-page: 878 year: 2011 end-page: 886 ident: bib13 article-title: Integrating information from two pictorial animations: complexity and cognitive prerequisites influence performance publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology – volume: 104 start-page: 485 year: 2012 end-page: 498 ident: bib40 article-title: Emotional design in multimedia learning publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology – volume: 17 start-page: 147 year: 2005 end-page: 177 ident: bib25 article-title: Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions publication-title: Educational Psychology Review – volume: 23 start-page: 1055 year: 2007 end-page: 1071 ident: bib35 article-title: The impact of intrinsic cognitive load on the effectiveness of graphical help for coherence formation publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior – volume: 24 start-page: 27 year: 2012 end-page: 45 ident: bib26 article-title: An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks publication-title: Educational Psychology Review – volume: 38 start-page: 37 year: 2003 end-page: 41 ident: bib27 article-title: Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory publication-title: Educational Psychologist – reference: Plass, J. L., Heidig, S., Hayward, E. O., Homer, B. D., & Um, E. J. (in press). Emotional design in multimedia learning: effects of shape and color on affect and learning. – reference: . – volume: 52 start-page: 292 year: 2009 end-page: 301 ident: bib11 article-title: Learning performance with interactive simulations in medical education: lessons learned from results of learning complex physiological models with the HAEMOdynamics SIMulator publication-title: Computers & Education – volume: 23 start-page: 501 year: 2011 end-page: 521 ident: bib19 article-title: Facilitating understanding of movements in dynamic visualizations: an embodied perspective publication-title: Educational Psychology Review – volume: 22 start-page: 445 year: 1995 end-page: 477 ident: bib6 article-title: Perspectives on the design of human-computer interactions: issues and implications publication-title: Instructional Science – volume: 39 start-page: 27 year: 2011 end-page: 39 ident: bib9 article-title: Improving early reading comprehension using embodied CAI publication-title: Instructional Science – start-page: 65 year: 2007 end-page: 90 ident: bib20 article-title: A meta-analytic investigation of learner control: old findings and new directions publication-title: Toward a science of distributed learning – volume: 93 start-page: 390 year: 2001 end-page: 397 ident: bib23 article-title: When learning is just a click away: does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages? publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology – reference: . – volume: 26 start-page: 1024 year: 2010 end-page: 1033 ident: bib5 article-title: Interactivity in multimedia learning: an integrated model publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior – volume: 95 start-page: 806 issue: 4 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib24 article-title: Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: what works in the design of agent-based microworlds? publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.806 – volume: 33 start-page: 73 issue: 1 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib2 article-title: Supporting learning with interactive multimedia through active integration of representations publication-title: Instructional Science doi: 10.1007/s11251-004-7685-z – volume: 10 start-page: 251 issue: 3 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib38 article-title: Cognitive architecture and instructional design publication-title: Educational Psychology Review doi: 10.1023/A:1022193728205 – start-page: 49 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib30 article-title: An integrated model of text and picture comprehension – start-page: 65 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib20 article-title: A meta-analytic investigation of learner control: old findings and new directions – volume: 25 start-page: 389 issue: 2 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib3 article-title: Dynamic visualisations and hypermedia: beyond the “Wow” factor publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.018 – volume: 52 start-page: 292 issue: 2 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib11 article-title: Learning performance with interactive simulations in medical education: lessons learned from results of learning complex physiological models with the HAEMOdynamics SIMulator publication-title: Computers & Education doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2008.08.008 – volume: 26 start-page: 1024 issue: 5 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib5 article-title: Interactivity in multimedia learning: an integrated model publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.003 – start-page: 48 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib16 article-title: Schema acquisition and sources of cognitive load – volume: 24 start-page: 182 issue: 3 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib17 article-title: Interactive distance education: a cognitive load perspective publication-title: Journal of Computing in Higher Education doi: 10.1007/s12528-012-9060-4 – start-page: 19 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib36 article-title: Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning – volume: 58 start-page: 1076 issue: 4 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib10 article-title: Differential impact of unguided versus guided use of a multimedia introduction to equine obstetrics in veterinary education publication-title: Computers & Education doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2011.11.006 – volume: 39 start-page: 27 issue: 1 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib9 article-title: Improving early reading comprehension using embodied CAI publication-title: Instructional Science doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9096-7 – volume: 15 start-page: 43 issue: 1 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib18 article-title: Promoting cognition in multimedia interactivity research publication-title: Journal of Interactive Learning Research – volume: 13 start-page: 227 issue: 2 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib33 article-title: Supporting coherence formation in learning from multiple representations publication-title: Learning and Instruction doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00022-1 – volume: 19 start-page: 327 issue: 3 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib41 article-title: Interactivity in video-based models publication-title: Educational Psychology Review doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9045-4 – ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib28 doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.02.006 – volume: 59 start-page: 617 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib1 article-title: Grounded cognition publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 – volume: 23 start-page: 501 issue: 4 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib19 article-title: Facilitating understanding of movements in dynamic visualizations: an embodied perspective publication-title: Educational Psychology Review doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9173-8 – volume: 19 start-page: 411 issue: 5 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib29 article-title: Interactive and non-interactive pictures in multimedia learning environments: effects on learning outcomes and learning efficiency publication-title: Learning and Instruction doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.008 – volume: 20 start-page: 321 issue: 3 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib34 article-title: Cognitive load and the format of instructional aids for coherence formation publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1002/acp.1248 – volume: 21 start-page: 317 issue: 3 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib42 article-title: Physical versus virtual manipulative experimentation in physics learning publication-title: Learning and Instruction doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.03.001 – volume: 25 start-page: 360 issue: 2 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib8 article-title: Learning with hypermedia: the influence of representational formats and different levels of learner control on performance and learning behavior publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.015 – volume: 22 start-page: 445 issue: 6 year: 1995 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib6 article-title: Perspectives on the design of human-computer interactions: issues and implications publication-title: Instructional Science doi: 10.1007/BF00897978 – volume: 63 start-page: 393 year: 2013 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib7 article-title: Transfer of expertise: an eye tracking and think aloud study using dynamic medical visualizations publication-title: Computers & Education doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.021 – volume: 93 start-page: 390 issue: 2 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib23 article-title: When learning is just a click away: does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages? publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.2.390 – volume: 24 start-page: 27 issue: 1 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib26 article-title: An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks publication-title: Educational Psychology Review doi: 10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2 – volume: 19 start-page: 387 issue: 3 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib15 article-title: Enhancing instructional efficiency of interactive e-learning environments: a cognitive load perspective publication-title: Educational Psychology Review doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9051-6 – volume: 14 start-page: 293 issue: 14 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib32 article-title: The cognitive benefits of interactive videos: learning to tie nautical knots publication-title: Learning and Instruction doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.005 – volume: 25 start-page: 878 issue: 6 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib13 article-title: Integrating information from two pictorial animations: complexity and cognitive prerequisites influence performance publication-title: Applied Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1002/acp.1762 – volume: 12 start-page: 185 issue: 3 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib37 article-title: Why some material is difficult to learn publication-title: Cognition and Instruction doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci1203_1 – volume: 23 start-page: 1055 issue: 3 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib35 article-title: The impact of intrinsic cognitive load on the effectiveness of graphical help for coherence formation publication-title: Computers in Human Behavior doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2006.10.002 – volume: 104 start-page: 485 issue: 2 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib40 article-title: Emotional design in multimedia learning publication-title: Journal of Educational Psychology doi: 10.1037/a0026609 – volume: 38 start-page: 37 issue: 1 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib27 article-title: Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory publication-title: Educational Psychologist doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8 – volume: 17 start-page: 147 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib25 article-title: Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions publication-title: Educational Psychology Review doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0 – ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib39 – volume: 14 start-page: 245 issue: 2 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib31 article-title: Individual and co-operative learning with interactive animated pictures publication-title: European Journal of Psychology of Education doi: 10.1007/BF03172968 – volume: 34 start-page: 833 issue: 10 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib14 article-title: Just enough, but not too much interactivity leads to better clinical skills performance after a computer assisted learning module publication-title: Medical Teacher doi: 10.3109/0142159X.2012.706727 – volume: 106 start-page: 1047 issue: 2 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib4 article-title: Gesturing makes learning last publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.010 – volume: 38 start-page: 259 issue: 3 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib12 article-title: Expertise reversal for iconic representations in science visualizations publication-title: Instructional Science doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9108-7 – volume: 14 start-page: 257 issue: 3 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib22 article-title: Interrogation of a dynamic visualization during learning publication-title: Learning and Instruction doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.003 – volume: 13 start-page: 157 issue: 2 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017_bib21 article-title: Animation and learning: selective processing of information in dynamic graphics publication-title: Learning and Instruction doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00018-X – reference: 22917265 - Med Teach. 2012;34(10):833-9 – reference: 17560971 - Cognition. 2008 Feb;106(2):1047-58 – reference: 17705682 - Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:617-45 |
SSID | ssj0004645 |
Score | 2.321826 |
Snippet | To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of... To identify the most effective way for medical students to interact with a browser-based learning module on the symptoms and neurological underpinnings of... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 198 |
SubjectTerms | Behavioral interactivity Cognitive interactivity Cognitive load Medical education Multimedia learning |
Title | The cognitive impact of interactive design features for learning complex materials in medical education |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.09.017 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27274609 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1835683395 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4892375 |
Volume | 71 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3dS8MwEA8yX3wRv50fI4Kvc92SNOnjGMpU9EUF30KbJrqh3XAb-OTf7l2azk0FwZdC2ksbcpe7a3L3O0JOhTFSYnJMnMMvCm-nDNacSJomjxFbJIujFPc7bm7j_gO_ehSPK6RX5cJgWGXQ_aVO99o63GmF2WyNB4PWHeheBIsReCCDmC6Ywc4l4ueffbQXciN9oWIkbiL1VxZPa4jvHuOeCFhB5uFOfd2yX-3TT__zexjlgl262CDrwaGk3XLMm2TFFltYiznEbWyTJ5AEOo8SomVaJB05ikgRPkcK7uY-kIM663E-JxRcWRoKSjxRH3Zu3yk4t6W8Qlf6Wp7wUFt9aYc8XJzf9_rNUF6haUA7TuEK_GOqk7JcgaV0XJnMyZwnzGXctp1UUQIUaSSzCFoS2hEzjvEUptQwx3ZJrRgVdp9Q1ol42k5kZrnjqROJciK3THKRGxarvE54NanaBOxxLIHxoqsgs6EOvNDICx0lGnhRJ2fzbuMSfOOvDqrimF6SIg0G4q-uJxWHNawwPDZJCzuaTTQoPRErxhJRJ3slx-ej6YD7x-MoqRO5JAtzAkTvXn5SDJ49ijdX4FtLcfD_IR-SNWjxMob8iNSmbzN7DC7SNGv4NdAgq93L6_7tJ0FZFJg |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LT-MwEB7xOMBltbzLLmAkrqVpbcfOcYVA5XkBJG5W4thQBGm1FInT_vadcZxCAQmJSyS_EssznpnYM98A7ElrlaLgmLTEXxTRzTnuOZm1bZkStkiRJjmdd5xfpP1rcXIjb2bgoImFIbfKKPtrmR6kdazpxNXsjAaDziXKXgKLkXQhQ5guszAvJFfE2vv_um-CI0OmYurdpu6vYTyde3r5iA5FUA3ygHcaEpd9qqA-GqDv_SjfKKajn_AjWpTsTz3pJZhx1TIlY46OGytwi6zAJm5CrI6LZEPPCCoiBElhbRk8OZh3AejziaEty2JGiVsW_M7dC0PrtmZYHMoe6yse5povrcL10eHVQb8d8yu0LYrHMT6RgFz3cl5qVJVeaFt4VYqM-0K4rlc6ybBHnqgiwZLCcsKt5yLHJbXc8zWYq4aV2wDGe4nIu5kqnPAi9zLTXpaOKyFLy1NdtkA0i2psBB-nHBgPpvEyuzeRFoZoYZLMIC1asD8ZNqrRN74aoBuKmSk2Mqghvhq621DY4Baje5O8csPnJ4NST6aa80y2YL2m-GQ2PbT_RJpkLVBTvDDpQPDd0y3V4C7AeAuNxrWSm9-f8g4s9K_Oz8zZ8cXpL1jEFlE7lP-GufHfZ7eF9tK42A774T9_4hYu |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+cognitive+impact+of+interactive+design+features+for+learning+complex+materials+in+medical+education&rft.jtitle=Computers+and+education&rft.au=Song%2C+Hyuksoon+S.&rft.au=Pusic%2C+Martin&rft.au=Nick%2C+Michael+W.&rft.au=Sarpel%2C+Umut&rft.date=2014-02-01&rft.pub=Elsevier+Ltd&rft.issn=0360-1315&rft.eissn=1873-782X&rft.volume=71&rft.spage=198&rft.epage=205&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.compedu.2013.09.017&rft.externalDocID=S0360131513002807 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0360-1315&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0360-1315&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0360-1315&client=summon |