A common misapplication of statistical inference: Nuisance control with null-hypothesis significance tests
•Researchers use statistical tests of stimulus or subjects characteristics to “control for confounds”.•This practice is conceptually misguided and pragmatically useless.•We discuss the problem and alternatives. Experimental research on behavior and cognition frequently rests on stimulus or subject s...
Saved in:
Published in | Brain and language Vol. 162; pp. 42 - 45 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Netherlands
Elsevier Inc
01.11.2016
Academic Press |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | •Researchers use statistical tests of stimulus or subjects characteristics to “control for confounds”.•This practice is conceptually misguided and pragmatically useless.•We discuss the problem and alternatives.
Experimental research on behavior and cognition frequently rests on stimulus or subject selection where not all characteristics can be fully controlled, even when attempting strict matching. For example, when contrasting patients to controls, variables such as intelligence or socioeconomic status are often correlated with patient status. Similarly, when presenting word stimuli, variables such as word frequency are often correlated with primary variables of interest. One procedure very commonly employed to control for such nuisance effects is conducting inferential tests on confounding stimulus or subject characteristics. For example, if word length is not significantly different for two stimulus sets, they are considered as matched for word length. Such a test has high error rates and is conceptually misguided. It reflects a common misunderstanding of statistical tests: interpreting significance not to refer to inference about a particular population parameter, but about 1. the sample in question, 2. the practical relevance of a sample difference (so that a nonsignificant test is taken to indicate evidence for the absence of relevant differences). We show inferential testing for assessing nuisance effects to be inappropriate both pragmatically and philosophically, present a survey showing its high prevalence, and briefly discuss an alternative in the form of regression including nuisance variables. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Experimental research on behavior and cognition frequently rests on stimulus or subject selection where not all characteristics can be fully controlled, even when attempting strict matching. For example, when contrasting patients to controls, variables such as intelligence or socioeconomic status are often correlated with patient status. Similarly, when presenting word stimuli, variables such as word frequency are often correlated with primary variables of interest. One procedure very commonly employed to control for such nuisance effects is conducting inferential tests on confounding stimulus or subject characteristics. For example, if word length is not significantly different for two stimulus sets, they are considered as matched for word length. Such a test has high error rates and is conceptually misguided. It reflects a common misunderstanding of statistical tests: interpreting significance not to refer to inference about a particular population parameter, but about 1. the sample in question, 2. the practical relevance of a sample difference (so that a nonsignificant test is taken to indicate evidence for the absence of relevant differences). We show inferential testing for assessing nuisance effects to be inappropriate both pragmatically and philosophically, present a survey showing its high prevalence, and briefly discuss an alternative in the form of regression including nuisance variables.Experimental research on behavior and cognition frequently rests on stimulus or subject selection where not all characteristics can be fully controlled, even when attempting strict matching. For example, when contrasting patients to controls, variables such as intelligence or socioeconomic status are often correlated with patient status. Similarly, when presenting word stimuli, variables such as word frequency are often correlated with primary variables of interest. One procedure very commonly employed to control for such nuisance effects is conducting inferential tests on confounding stimulus or subject characteristics. For example, if word length is not significantly different for two stimulus sets, they are considered as matched for word length. Such a test has high error rates and is conceptually misguided. It reflects a common misunderstanding of statistical tests: interpreting significance not to refer to inference about a particular population parameter, but about 1. the sample in question, 2. the practical relevance of a sample difference (so that a nonsignificant test is taken to indicate evidence for the absence of relevant differences). We show inferential testing for assessing nuisance effects to be inappropriate both pragmatically and philosophically, present a survey showing its high prevalence, and briefly discuss an alternative in the form of regression including nuisance variables. Experimental research on behavior and cognition frequently rests on stimulus or subject selection where not all characteristics can be fully controlled, even when attempting strict matching. For example, when contrasting patients to controls, variables such as intelligence or socioeconomic status are often correlated with patient status. Similarly, when presenting word stimuli, variables such as word frequency are often correlated with primary variables of interest. One procedure very commonly employed to control for such nuisance effects is conducting inferential tests on confounding stimulus or subject characteristics. For example, if word length is not significantly different for two stimulus sets, they are considered as matched for word length. Such a test has high error rates and is conceptually misguided. It reflects a common misunderstanding of statistical tests: interpreting significance not to refer to inference about a particular population parameter, but about 1. the sample in question, 2. the practical relevance of a sample difference (so that a nonsignificant test is taken to indicate evidence for the absence of relevant differences). We show inferential testing for assessing nuisance effects to be inappropriate both pragmatically and philosophically, present a survey showing its high prevalence, and briefly discuss an alternative in the form of regression including nuisance variables. •Researchers use statistical tests of stimulus or subjects characteristics to “control for confounds”.•This practice is conceptually misguided and pragmatically useless.•We discuss the problem and alternatives. Experimental research on behavior and cognition frequently rests on stimulus or subject selection where not all characteristics can be fully controlled, even when attempting strict matching. For example, when contrasting patients to controls, variables such as intelligence or socioeconomic status are often correlated with patient status. Similarly, when presenting word stimuli, variables such as word frequency are often correlated with primary variables of interest. One procedure very commonly employed to control for such nuisance effects is conducting inferential tests on confounding stimulus or subject characteristics. For example, if word length is not significantly different for two stimulus sets, they are considered as matched for word length. Such a test has high error rates and is conceptually misguided. It reflects a common misunderstanding of statistical tests: interpreting significance not to refer to inference about a particular population parameter, but about 1. the sample in question, 2. the practical relevance of a sample difference (so that a nonsignificant test is taken to indicate evidence for the absence of relevant differences). We show inferential testing for assessing nuisance effects to be inappropriate both pragmatically and philosophically, present a survey showing its high prevalence, and briefly discuss an alternative in the form of regression including nuisance variables. |
Author | Alday, Phillip M. Sassenhagen, Jona |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Jona surname: Sassenhagen fullname: Sassenhagen, Jona email: jona.sassenhagen@gmail.com organization: University of Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany – sequence: 2 givenname: Phillip M. surname: Alday fullname: Alday, Phillip M. organization: University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27543688$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkUtP3TAQhS0EKhfaX4BUReqmmwQ_EwepC4R4SQg2rdSdles4vY4cO9hOK_5953JhwwKx8oz1nRnNOUdo3wdvEDohuCKY1Kdjte587yoKTYVlhTHZQyuCW1xSIsQ-WmHcsrJl_PchOkppBIBwST6hQ9oIzmopV2g8L3SYpuCLyaZunp3VXbbQhqFIGcqU4ccV1g8mGq_NWXG_AAkVCH2OwRX_bN4UfnGu3DzNIW9MsqlI9o-3A2i3ZDYpp8_oYOhcMl9e3mP06-ry58VNefdwfXtxfldqXotcDj3HTFDWMN4KOIA2A5OC4l6ItW60Zpwx3IleaFlL01PNaiHpmtWGc0GMZsfo-27uHMPjApsVnKaNc503YUmKwLRaCEL5B1DW0JZyWQP67Q06hiV6OASopm05B1OB-vpCLevJ9GqOdurik3o1HIB2B-gYUopmUNrmZ8dz7KxTBKttuGpUz-GqbbgKSwXZgZa90b6Of1_1Y6cy4Plfa6JK2m6T7G00Oqs-2Hf1_wEUBL4B |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2020_09_007 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae032 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2018_0522 crossref_primary_10_1075_ml_20013_cla crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_018_1100_1 crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2019_1584679 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2021_107754 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02058 crossref_primary_10_16995_glossa_5728 crossref_primary_10_1002_aur_2992 crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2018_1502458 crossref_primary_10_1162_opmi_a_00083 crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0311_16_2017 crossref_primary_10_5334_jopd_110 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13415_023_01120_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2022_119036 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2022_930849 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2016_02005 crossref_primary_10_1075_itl_20010_van crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14656 crossref_primary_10_1111_jsr_14256 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218221114644 crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2024_2348649 crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0321_18_2019 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2023_105604 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jneumeth_2018_12_010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2024_120539 crossref_primary_10_1515_lingvan_2019_0069 crossref_primary_10_16995_glossa_5697 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0305000920000215 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13340 crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14269 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02078 crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2018_1546882 crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_13451 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0305000919000795 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_623648 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0226482 crossref_primary_10_1017_S0272263119000767 crossref_primary_10_1111_lang_12370 crossref_primary_10_15460_jlar_2024_2_1_1053 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2019_04_004 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2019_107199 crossref_primary_10_1007_s41809_024_00155_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2020_104516 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_023_01460_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2020_107660 crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_1_3 crossref_primary_10_3389_frvir_2022_697934 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12768 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01648 crossref_primary_10_1080_23273798_2020_1722847 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2022_2410 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_01070 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 10.1037/a0028347 10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3 10.1002/sim.4780131703 10.1038/nrn3475 10.1111/j.1467-985X.2007.00527.x 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2016 Elsevier Inc. Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Copyright Academic Press Nov 2016 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2016 Elsevier Inc. – notice: Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. – notice: Copyright Academic Press Nov 2016 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7T9 8BM NAPCQ 7TK 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) ComDisDome Nursing & Allied Health Premium Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Nursing & Allied Health Premium Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) ComDisDome Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Premium MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine Languages & Literatures Psychology |
EISSN | 1090-2155 |
EndPage | 45 |
ExternalDocumentID | 0001224267 27543688 10_1016_j_bandl_2016_08_001 S0093934X16300323 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M --Z -DZ -~X .GJ .~1 0R~ 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 23N 4.4 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5RE 5VS 6PF 7-5 71M 85S 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFJI AAGJA AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAQXK AAWTL AAXLA AAXUO AAYJJ ABCQJ ABFNM ABIVO ABJNI ABMAC ABMMH ABOYX ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFS ACHQT ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADFGL ADIYS ADMUD ADRHT AEBSH AEKER AENEX AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV AKYCK ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ AOMHK ASPBG AVARZ AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC C45 CAG COF CS3 DM4 EBS EFBJH EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q G8K GBLVA HMW HMY HVGLF HZ~ H~9 IHE J1W KOM LG5 LPU M3U M3X M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OKEIE OVD OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. PRBVW Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCU SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSO SSS SSY SSZ T5K TEORI TN5 TWZ UQL WH7 WUQ XJT XPP YK3 ZA5 ZCG ZGI ZMT ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABDPE ABWVN ACRPL ACVFH ADCNI ADMHG ADNMO ADVLN ADXHL AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGQPQ AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7T9 8BM EFKBS NAPCQ 7TK 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c465t-fd4035237349509327f38520d55bc7cc34330a5d5c868ed2c36582b36e4451ec3 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0093-934X 1090-2155 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 00:35:34 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 23:11:25 EDT 2025 Wed Aug 13 09:17:07 EDT 2025 Tue Apr 08 05:56:58 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:59:41 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:30:02 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:23:27 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Language | English |
License | Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c465t-fd4035237349509327f38520d55bc7cc34330a5d5c868ed2c36582b36e4451ec3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0093934X16300323 |
PMID | 27543688 |
PQID | 1879944275 |
PQPubID | 2038115 |
PageCount | 4 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_1852655124 proquest_miscellaneous_1837292486 proquest_journals_1879944275 pubmed_primary_27543688 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_bandl_2016_08_001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandl_2016_08_001 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_bandl_2016_08_001 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | November 2016 2016-11-00 2016-Nov 20161101 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-11-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 11 year: 2016 text: November 2016 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Netherlands |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Netherlands – name: San Diego |
PublicationTitle | Brain and language |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Brain Lang |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | Elsevier Inc Academic Press |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier Inc – name: Academic Press |
References | Imai, King, Stuart (b0040) 2008; 171 Judd, Westfall, Kenny (b0045) 2012; 103 April. Pinheiro, Bates (b0050) 2000 Senn (b0055) 1994; 13 Button, Katherine S., Ioannidis, John P. A., Mokrysz, Claire, Nosek, Brian A., Flint, Jonathan, Robinson, Emma S. J., Munafó, Marcus R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Clark (b0020) 1973; 12 Baayen, Davidson, Bates (b0005) 2008; 59 Cohen (b0025) 1992; 112 Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker (b0010) 2015; 67 Gelman, Hill (b0035) 2006 Westfall, Yarkoni (b0060) 2016; 3 Fox (b0030) 2016 Bates (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0010) 2015; 67 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0015 Imai (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0040) 2008; 171 Westfall (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0060) 2016; 3 Clark (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0020) 1973; 12 Pinheiro (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0050) 2000 Judd (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0045) 2012; 103 Cohen (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0025) 1992; 112 Senn (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0055) 1994; 13 Baayen (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0005) 2008; 59 Gelman (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0035) 2006 Fox (10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0030) 2016 |
References_xml | – volume: 59 start-page: 390 year: 2008 end-page: 412 ident: b0005 article-title: Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – reference: , April. – year: 2006 ident: b0035 article-title: Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models – volume: 3 start-page: 1 year: 2016 end-page: 100 ident: b0060 article-title: Statistically controlling for confounding constructs is harder than you think publication-title: PLoS ONE – year: 2000 ident: b0050 article-title: Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS – volume: 171 start-page: 481 year: 2008 end-page: 502 ident: b0040 article-title: Misunderstandings between experimentalists and observationalists about causal inference publication-title: Journal of the Royal Statistical Society A – reference: Button, Katherine S., Ioannidis, John P. A., Mokrysz, Claire, Nosek, Brian A., Flint, Jonathan, Robinson, Emma S. J., Munafó, Marcus R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. – year: 2016 ident: b0030 article-title: Applied regression analysis and generalized linear models – volume: 67 start-page: 1 year: 2015 end-page: 48 ident: b0010 article-title: Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 publication-title: Journal of Statistical Software – volume: 12 start-page: 335 year: 1973 end-page: 359 ident: b0020 article-title: The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior – volume: 13 start-page: 1715 year: 1994 end-page: 1726 ident: b0055 article-title: Testing for baseline balance in clinical trials publication-title: Statistics in Medicine – volume: 112 start-page: 55 year: 1992 end-page: 159 ident: b0025 article-title: A power primer publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 103 start-page: 54 year: 2012 end-page: 69 ident: b0045 article-title: Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology – volume: 112 start-page: 55 issue: 1 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0025 article-title: A power primer publication-title: Psychological Bulletin doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 – volume: 103 start-page: 54 issue: 1 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0045 article-title: Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem publication-title: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology doi: 10.1037/a0028347 – year: 2000 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0050 – volume: 12 start-page: 335 year: 1973 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0020 article-title: The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research publication-title: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3 – year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0035 – volume: 13 start-page: 1715 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0055 article-title: Testing for baseline balance in clinical trials publication-title: Statistics in Medicine doi: 10.1002/sim.4780131703 – volume: 3 start-page: 1 issue: 11 year: 2016 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0060 article-title: Statistically controlling for confounding constructs is harder than you think publication-title: PLoS ONE – ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0015 doi: 10.1038/nrn3475 – year: 2016 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0030 – volume: 171 start-page: 481 issue: Part 2 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0040 article-title: Misunderstandings between experimentalists and observationalists about causal inference publication-title: Journal of the Royal Statistical Society A doi: 10.1111/j.1467-985X.2007.00527.x – volume: 59 start-page: 390 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0005 article-title: Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 – volume: 67 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2015 ident: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001_b0010 article-title: Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 publication-title: Journal of Statistical Software doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 |
SSID | ssj0011481 |
Score | 2.4172664 |
Snippet | •Researchers use statistical tests of stimulus or subjects characteristics to “control for confounds”.•This practice is conceptually misguided and... Experimental research on behavior and cognition frequently rests on stimulus or subject selection where not all characteristics can be fully controlled, even... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 42 |
SubjectTerms | Behavioral Research - methods Cognition Confounding Factors (Epidemiology) Humans Inference Language Miscommunication Models, Statistical Statistical inference Word frequency Word length |
Title | A common misapplication of statistical inference: Nuisance control with null-hypothesis significance tests |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.001 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27543688 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1879944275 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1837292486 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1852655124 |
Volume | 162 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3dT9swED8hkCZeJug2VsYqIyGeFmhsx3H2ViGq8tWnIfXNip1EFKG0Iu0DL_ztu0ucaEijD7zl45zYPvvubN_9DuAkVDq1FN2TSFUE0nKUgyoMg8JamdsUFZKm2OG7qZrcy-tZNNuCizYWhtwqvexvZHotrf2Tc9-b58v5nGJ8E5EIOQsJNUpwQvyUMqZRfvbauXmQud9kzUtEQNQt8lDt42UJyYD8u1SN4-kzw_xHO71nfdZaaLwHn735yEZNDfdhKy97cHDrNx0rdspuO5zkqgef7vzReQ92O1H38gUeRwwbjCOQIZv_OcNmi4JRiFGN3oz_mbfhgL_ZdI2UeMW8czujHVxW4go2eHhZUhxXNa8YuYOQ81FNiVbsqvoK9-PLPxeTwGddCJxU0SooMkkYqSIWuHbCXuNxIXTEh1kUWRc7J6QQwzTKIqeVzjPuBBox3AqVE9ZZ7sQ32C4XZf4dWBZrl8WEB5NoyYdpIjRZjAXPChvjh_rA2942zkOSU2aMJ9P6nj2amkWGWGQoX-Yw7MOvrtCyQeTYTK5aNpo3A8ugzthc8KhluvHzujKUmz2RksdRH46718gqOmZJy3yxJho6CuVSq000lJYAjS3sg4NmQHWNwY9TWgB9-NGa_4BdumsiJo9ge_W8zn-i6bSyg3puDGBndHUzmf4FQ9YXWw |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3db9MwED-NToK9ICgwChszEuKJaI2_4vBWTZu6re3TJvXNip1E64TSirQP---5S5wIJOgDb1FydmyffT777n4H8CXWJnMU3ZNKXUbScZSDOo6j0jlZuAw3JEOxw_OFnt7Lm6VaHsBFFwtDbpVB9rcyvZHW4c15GM3zzWpFMb6pSIVcxoQaJbh4BoeETqUGcDi5vp0uemMCavxt4rxURFSgAx9q3LwcgRmQi5duoDxDcpi_bFD_UkCbjejqFbwMGiSbtI18DQdFNYTjWbh3rNlXNuuhkushPJ8H6_kQjnpp9_QGHicM-4yTkCGnfzNjs3XJKMqoAXDG_6y6iMDvbLFDSnxiwb-d0SUuq_AQGz08bSiUq17VjDxCyP-ooURFdlu_hfury7uLaRQSL0RearWNylwSTKpIBB6fcNR4Ugqj-DhXyvnEeyGFGGcqV95oU-TcC9RjuBO6ILizwot3MKjWVfEeWJ4YnycECZMaycdZKgwpjSXPS5dgRSPg3WhbH1DJKTnGD9u5nz3ahkWWWGQpZeY4HsG3vtCmBeXYT647Nto_5pbFbWN_wZOO6TYs7dpSevZUSp6oEXzuPyOryNKSVcV6RzRkDeXS6H00lJkA9S0cg-N2QvWdwcopM4D58L8tP4MX07v5zM6uF7cf4Yi-tAGUJzDY_twVp6hJbd2nsFJ-Ack_Ggw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=A+common+misapplication+of+statistical+inference%3A+Nuisance+control+with+null-hypothesis+significance+tests&rft.jtitle=Brain+and+language&rft.au=Sassenhagen%2C+Jona&rft.au=Alday%2C+Phillip+M.&rft.date=2016-11-01&rft.issn=0093-934X&rft.volume=162&rft.spage=42&rft.epage=45&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.bandl.2016.08.001&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_1016_j_bandl_2016_08_001 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0093-934X&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0093-934X&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0093-934X&client=summon |