Children Treat Grammatical Errors Differently for Native and Non-Native Speakers

Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such err...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inFrontiers in psychology Vol. 13; p. 855130
Main Authors Rett, Alexandra, White, Katherine S.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Switzerland Frontiers Media S.A 22.04.2022
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such errors. In the present study, we asked whether 5-6-year-old children place less weight on errors from speakers with a foreign accent. In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-year-old children ( N = 80) listened to pairs of either native or foreign-accented speakers (between-subjects) label objects. For native speaker pairings, children preferred information provided by grammatical speakers over information from speakers who made subject-verb agreement errors. In contrast, children chose between foreign-accented speakers at chance. In Experiment 2 ( N = 40), children preferred information from grammatical foreign-accented speakers over information from foreign-accented speakers who produced word-order violations. These findings constitute the first demonstration that children treat speech errors differently based on a speaker’s language background.
AbstractList Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such errors. In the present study, we asked whether 5-6-year-old children place less weight on errors from speakers with a foreign accent. In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-year-old children (N = 80) listened to pairs of either native or foreign-accented speakers (between-subjects) label objects. For native speaker pairings, children preferred information provided by grammatical speakers over information from speakers who made subject-verb agreement errors. In contrast, children chose between foreign-accented speakers at chance. In Experiment 2 (N = 40), children preferred information from grammatical foreign-accented speakers over information from foreign-accented speakers who produced word-order violations. These findings constitute the first demonstration that children treat speech errors differently based on a speaker's language background.Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such errors. In the present study, we asked whether 5-6-year-old children place less weight on errors from speakers with a foreign accent. In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-year-old children (N = 80) listened to pairs of either native or foreign-accented speakers (between-subjects) label objects. For native speaker pairings, children preferred information provided by grammatical speakers over information from speakers who made subject-verb agreement errors. In contrast, children chose between foreign-accented speakers at chance. In Experiment 2 (N = 40), children preferred information from grammatical foreign-accented speakers over information from foreign-accented speakers who produced word-order violations. These findings constitute the first demonstration that children treat speech errors differently based on a speaker's language background.
Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such errors. In the present study, we asked whether 5-6-year-old children place less weight on errors from speakers with a foreign accent. In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-year-old children (N = 80) listened to pairs of either native or foreign-accented speakers (between-subjects) label objects. For native speaker pairings, children preferred information provided by grammatical speakers over information from speakers who made subject-verb agreement errors. In contrast, children chose between foreign-accented speakers at chance. In Experiment 2 (N = 40), children preferred information from grammatical foreign-accented speakers over information from foreign-accented speakers who produced word-order violations. These findings constitute the first demonstration that children treat speech errors differently based on a speaker’s language background.
Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such errors. In the present study, we asked whether 5-6-year-old children place less weight on errors from speakers with a foreign accent. In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-year-old children ( N = 80) listened to pairs of either native or foreign-accented speakers (between-subjects) label objects. For native speaker pairings, children preferred information provided by grammatical speakers over information from speakers who made subject-verb agreement errors. In contrast, children chose between foreign-accented speakers at chance. In Experiment 2 ( N = 40), children preferred information from grammatical foreign-accented speakers over information from foreign-accented speakers who produced word-order violations. These findings constitute the first demonstration that children treat speech errors differently based on a speaker’s language background.
Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such errors. In the present study, we asked whether 5-6-year-old children place less weight on errors from speakers with a foreign accent. In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-year-old children ( = 80) listened to pairs of either native or foreign-accented speakers (between-subjects) label objects. For native speaker pairings, children preferred information provided by grammatical speakers over information from speakers who made subject-verb agreement errors. In contrast, children chose between foreign-accented speakers at chance. In Experiment 2 ( = 40), children preferred information from grammatical foreign-accented speakers over information from foreign-accented speakers who produced word-order violations. These findings constitute the first demonstration that children treat speech errors differently based on a speaker's language background.
Author White, Katherine S.
Rett, Alexandra
AuthorAffiliation 1 Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego , La Jolla, CA , United States
2 Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo , Waterloo, ON , Canada
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: 2 Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo , Waterloo, ON , Canada
– name: 1 Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego , La Jolla, CA , United States
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Alexandra
  surname: Rett
  fullname: Rett, Alexandra
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Katherine S.
  surname: White
  fullname: White, Katherine S.
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35529559$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9UUtvEzEQtlARLaU_gAvaI5cNfu_uBQmFUipVBYlytvwYpy7edbA3lfLvcZpQtRzwxR7P95jR9xodTWkChN4SvGCsHz74ddmuFhRTuuiFIAy_QCdESt4S3PVHT97H6KyUO1wPxxRj-godMyHoIMRwgr4vb0N0GabmJoOem4usx1HPwerYnOeccmk-B--hIua4bXzKzXVt30OjJ9dcp6k9lD_WoH9BLm_QS69jgbPDfYp-fjm_WX5tr75dXC4_XbWWSzG31hmjmR2cGagXfMDcU2MH3BlvdN9JcByEIVbgAZzvtaOd6HvOsBeEdODZKbrc67qk79Q6h1HnrUo6qIePlFdK57pHBMUtc1gyUx0txxYMeNl1TnLqNceMV62Pe631xozgbN016_hM9HlnCrdqle5VnVcQLKrA-4NATr83UGY1hmIhRj1B2hRFpSS8Z4LKCn331OvR5G8kFUD2AJtTKRn8I4RgtUtePSSvdsmrffKV0_3DsWGuuaTduCH-h_kHyvi13g
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2022_101271
crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2357
crossref_primary_10_3390_educsci14101116
Cites_doi 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00792.x
10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011805
10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008
10.1016/j.jecp.2018.02.007
10.1121/1.1907309
10.1121/1.4929622
10.1371/journal.pone.0108308
10.1016/j.cortex.2018.03.007
10.1111/cdev.13415
10.1111/desc.12663
10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.08.001
10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.005
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01049.x
10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.025
10.1017/S0305000917000587
10.3758/s13428-015-0647-3
10.1111/hcre.12079
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01767.x
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x
10.1177/0261927X09359590
10.1044/jshr.3606.1208
10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.11.001
10.1111/cdev.13548
10.1111/infa.12056
10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0
10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00060-2
10.1371/journal.pone.0019812
10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104171
10.4159/harvard.9780674732469
10.1111/cdev.12797
10.1073/pnas.0705345104
10.1016/j.jecp.2018.09.012
10.1162/jocn_a_00103
10.1159/000107913
10.1080/10410236.2019.1584779
10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01546
10.3758/PBR.15.5.980
10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105022
10.1177/0142723719873499
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.x
10.1177/0956797616668557
10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.010
10.1111/bjdp.12137
10.1017/S0305000913000330
10.1177/0956797617690277
10.1037/a0030604
10.1037/dev0000659
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
10.1037/a0029658
10.1177/0261927X12463005
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x
10.1111/cdev.12421
10.1037/a0015808
10.1073/pnas.1216438110
10.1111/cdev.12299
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2022 Rett and White.
Copyright © 2022 Rett and White. 2022 Rett and White
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2022 Rett and White.
– notice: Copyright © 2022 Rett and White. 2022 Rett and White
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
NPM
7X8
5PM
DOA
DOI 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.855130
DatabaseName CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE - Academic

CrossRef

PubMed
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: DOA
  name: DOAJ (Directory of Open Access Journals)
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1664-1078
ExternalDocumentID oai_doaj_org_article_4c3d063bbbac40cebef677d642fa4034
PMC9075105
35529559
10_3389_fpsyg_2022_855130
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: ;
  grantid: RGPIN-2016-04511; PGSD3-532536-2019
GroupedDBID 53G
5VS
9T4
AAFWJ
AAKDD
AAYXX
ABIVO
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACXDI
ADBBV
ADRAZ
AEGXH
AFPKN
AIAGR
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
BAWUL
BCNDV
CITATION
DIK
EBS
EJD
EMOBN
F5P
GROUPED_DOAJ
GX1
HYE
KQ8
M48
M~E
O5R
O5S
OK1
P2P
PGMZT
RNS
RPM
IPNFZ
NPM
RIG
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c465t-cdbba3c9db92f54904f2bc907bfba876ed4e5b1c509edf8ad27588430f5117ef3
IEDL.DBID M48
ISSN 1664-1078
IngestDate Wed Aug 27 01:09:04 EDT 2025
Thu Aug 21 14:12:18 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 23:22:50 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 07:05:35 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:10:50 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:35:00 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Keywords accent-based expectations
grammatical errors
foreign accents
speaker reliability
speech errors
Language English
License Copyright © 2022 Rett and White.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c465t-cdbba3c9db92f54904f2bc907bfba876ed4e5b1c509edf8ad27588430f5117ef3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
Reviewed by: Jesús Bas, Pompeu Fabra University, Spain; Hanna Schleihauf, University of California, Berkeley, United States
Edited by: Thomas Castelain, University of Girona, Spain
This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology
OpenAccessLink http://journals.scholarsportal.info/openUrl.xqy?doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2022.855130
PMID 35529559
PQID 2661483526
PQPubID 23479
ParticipantIDs doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_4c3d063bbbac40cebef677d642fa4034
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9075105
proquest_miscellaneous_2661483526
pubmed_primary_35529559
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_855130
crossref_citationtrail_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_855130
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2022-04-22
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2022-04-22
PublicationDate_xml – month: 04
  year: 2022
  text: 2022-04-22
  day: 22
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace Switzerland
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Switzerland
PublicationTitle Frontiers in psychology
PublicationTitleAlternate Front Psychol
PublicationYear 2022
Publisher Frontiers Media S.A
Publisher_xml – name: Frontiers Media S.A
References Kondrad (B31) 2012; 27
Bejjanki (B3) 2011; 6
Fairchild (B15) 2018; 181
Sobel (B47) 2013; 49
Paquette-Smith (B41) 2019; 55
Ferguson (B16) 2009; 40
Kinzler (B28) 2011; 14
Johnson (B26) 1989; 21
Lev-Ari (B33) 2010; 46
DeJesus (B12) 2017; 164
Gibson (B19) 2017; 28
Horst (B22) 2016; 48
Spence (B49) 2021; 92
Petõ (B43) 2018; 171
Pantos (B40) 2013; 32
Yurovsky (B57) 2017; 28
Brown (B7) 1973
Hwang (B24) 2018; 45
Santelmann (B45) 1998; 69
Birch (B4) 2008; 107
Wade (B51) 2007; 64
Corriveau (B10) 2009; 12
Cohen (B9) 1988
Corriveau (B11) 2013; 49
Sumby (B50) 1954; 26
Southgate (B48) 2010; 13
Baese-Berk (B1) 2015; 138
Howard (B23) 2015; 86
Kidd (B27) 2011; 14
Jaswal (B25) 2006; 17
Fairchild (B14) 2020; 200
Baquiran (B2) 2020; 35
Kinzler (B29) 2007; 104
Orena (B38) 2015; 86
Sobel (B46) 2020; 91
Dragojevic (B13) 2016; 42
Wagner (B52) 2014; 41
Lev-Ari (B32) 2015; 5
Owens (B39) 2016; 34
Brosseau-Liard (B5) 2014; 9
Luchkina (B34) 2018; 21
Nurmsoo (B37) 2009; 80
White (B55) 2020; 40
Gluszek (B20) 2010; 29
Wulfeck (B56) 1993; 36
White (B54) 2021; 222
Paulus (B42) 2019; 180
Weatherhead (B53) 2018; 89
MacDonald (B35) 2008; 15
Matthews (B36) 2005; 20
Koenig (B30) 2005; 76
Brosseau-Liard (B6) 2014; 19
Hanulíková (B21) 2012; 24
Saffran (B44) 2018; 69
Gertner (B17) 2006; 17
Gibson (B18) 2013; 110
Caffarra (B8) 2019; 116
References_xml – volume: 12
  start-page: 426
  year: 2009
  ident: B10
  article-title: Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and recent accuracy.
  publication-title: Dev. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00792.x
– volume: 69
  start-page: 181
  year: 2018
  ident: B44
  article-title: Infant statistical learning.
  publication-title: Ann. Rev. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011805
– volume: 107
  start-page: 1018
  year: 2008
  ident: B4
  article-title: Three- and four-year-olds spontaneously use others’ past performance to guide their learning.
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.008
– volume: 171
  start-page: 99
  year: 2018
  ident: B43
  article-title: Learning how to use a tool: Mutually exclusive tool–function mappings are selectively acquired from linguistic in-group models.
  publication-title: J. Exp. Child Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.02.007
– volume: 26
  start-page: 212
  year: 1954
  ident: B50
  article-title: Visual contribution to speech intelligibility in noise.
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc.Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.1907309
– volume: 138
  start-page: EL223
  year: 2015
  ident: B1
  article-title: Speaking rate consistency in native and non-native speakers of English.
  publication-title: J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
  doi: 10.1121/1.4929622
– volume: 9
  year: 2014
  ident: B5
  article-title: You seem certain but you were wrong before: Developmental change in preschoolers’ relative trust in accurate versus confident speakers.
  publication-title: PloSOne
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108308
– volume: 116
  start-page: 308
  year: 2019
  ident: B8
  article-title: Not all errors are the same: ERP sensitivity to error typicality in foreign accented speech perception.
  publication-title: Cortex
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.03.007
– volume: 91
  start-page: e1134
  year: 2020
  ident: B46
  article-title: How children learn from others: an analysis of selective word learning.
  publication-title: Child Dev.
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.13415
– volume: 21
  year: 2018
  ident: B34
  article-title: Eighteen-month-olds selectively generalize words from accurate speakers to novel contexts.
  publication-title: Dev. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12663
– volume: 20
  start-page: 121
  year: 2005
  ident: B36
  article-title: The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order.
  publication-title: Cogn. Dev.
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.08.001
– volume: 164
  start-page: 178
  year: 2017
  ident: B12
  article-title: Bilingual children’s social preferences hinge on accent.
  publication-title: J. Exp. Child Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.005
– volume: 17
  start-page: 757
  year: 2006
  ident: B25
  article-title: Adults don’t always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words.
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x
– volume: 14
  start-page: 925
  year: 2011
  ident: B27
  article-title: Toddlers use speech disfluencies to predict speakers’ referential intentions.
  publication-title: Dev. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01049.x
– volume: 46
  start-page: 1093
  year: 2010
  ident: B33
  article-title: Why don’t we believe non-native speakers?
  publication-title: J.Exp. Soci. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.025
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1018
  year: 2018
  ident: B24
  article-title: Locals don’t have accents: Children weigh phonological proficiency over syntactic or semantic proficiency when categorizing individuals.
  publication-title: J. Child Lang.
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000917000587
– volume: 48
  start-page: 1393
  year: 2016
  ident: B22
  article-title: The Novel Object and Unusual Name (NOUN) database: A collection of novel images for use in experimental research.
  publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0647-3
– volume: 42
  start-page: 396
  year: 2016
  ident: B13
  article-title: I don’t like you because you’re hard to understand: the role of processing fluency in the language attitudes process.
  publication-title: Hum. Commun. Res.
  doi: 10.1111/hcre.12079
– volume: 17
  start-page: 684
  year: 2006
  ident: B17
  article-title: Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension.
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01767.x
– volume: 13
  start-page: 907
  year: 2010
  ident: B48
  article-title: Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication.
  publication-title: Dev. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x
– volume: 29
  start-page: 224
  year: 2010
  ident: B20
  article-title: Speaking with a non-native accent: Perceptions of bias, communication difficulties, and belonging in the United States.
  publication-title: J. Lang. Soc. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1177/0261927X09359590
– volume: 36
  start-page: 1208
  year: 1993
  ident: B56
  article-title: A reaction time study of grammaticality judgments in children.
  publication-title: J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res.
  doi: 10.1044/jshr.3606.1208
– volume: 27
  start-page: 126
  year: 2012
  ident: B31
  article-title: Explaining the errors away: young children forgive understandable semantic mistakes
  publication-title: Cogn. Dev.
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.11.001
– volume: 92
  start-page: 517
  year: 2021
  ident: B49
  article-title: Something about the way you speak: a meta-analysis on children’s linguistic-based social preferences.
  publication-title: Child Dev.
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.13548
– volume: 19
  start-page: 461
  year: 2014
  ident: B6
  article-title: Sensitivity to confidence cues increases during the second year of life.
  publication-title: Infancy
  doi: 10.1111/infa.12056
– volume: 21
  start-page: 60
  year: 1989
  ident: B26
  article-title: Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language.
  publication-title: Cogn. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0
– volume: 69
  start-page: 105
  year: 1998
  ident: B45
  article-title: Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: Evidence for limitations in processing space.
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00060-2
– volume: 6
  year: 2011
  ident: B3
  article-title: Cue integration in categorical tasks: insights from audio-visual speech perception.
  publication-title: PLoSOne
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019812
– volume: 200
  year: 2020
  ident: B14
  article-title: Pragmatics and social meaning: understanding Under-informativeness in native and non-native speakers.
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104171
– year: 1973
  ident: B7
  publication-title: A First Language.
  doi: 10.4159/harvard.9780674732469
– volume: 89
  start-page: 1613
  year: 2018
  ident: B53
  article-title: Accent, language, and race: 4-6-year-old children’s inferences differ by speaker cue
  publication-title: Child Dev.
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.12797
– year: 1988
  ident: B9
  publication-title: Statistical Power Analysis for the Social Sciences.
– volume: 104
  start-page: 12577
  year: 2007
  ident: B29
  article-title: The native language of social cognition.
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705345104
– volume: 180
  start-page: 113
  year: 2019
  ident: B42
  article-title: Young children protest against the incorrect use of novel words: Toward a normative pragmatic account on language acquisition.
  publication-title: J. Exp. Child Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.09.012
– volume: 24
  start-page: 878
  year: 2012
  ident: B21
  article-title: When one person’s mistake is another’s standard usage: The effect of foreign accent on syntactic processing.
  publication-title: J. Cogn. Neurosci.
  doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00103
– volume: 64
  start-page: 122
  year: 2007
  ident: B51
  article-title: Effects of acoustic variability in the perceptual learning of non-native-accented speech sounds.
  publication-title: Phonetica
  doi: 10.1159/000107913
– volume: 35
  start-page: 726
  year: 2020
  ident: B2
  article-title: A doctor’s foreign accent affects perceptions of competence.
  publication-title: Health Commun.
  doi: 10.1080/10410236.2019.1584779
– volume: 5
  year: 2015
  ident: B32
  article-title: Comprehending non-native speakers: Theory and evidence for adjustment in manner of processing.
  publication-title: Front.Psychol.
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01546
– volume: 15
  start-page: 980
  year: 2008
  ident: B35
  article-title: Differences in the cognitive demands of word order, plural, and subject-verb agreement constructions.
  publication-title: Psychono. Bull. Rev.
  doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.5.980
– volume: 222
  year: 2021
  ident: B54
  article-title: When it’s not appropriate to adapt: Toddlers’ learning of novel speech patterns is affected by visual information.
  publication-title: Brain Lang.
  doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105022
– volume: 40
  start-page: 3
  year: 2020
  ident: B55
  article-title: That’s thee, uhh blicket! How does disfluency affect children’s word learning?
  publication-title: First Lang.
  doi: 10.1177/0142723719873499
– volume: 14
  start-page: 106
  year: 2011
  ident: B28
  article-title: Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers.
  publication-title: Dev. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.x
– volume: 28
  start-page: 132
  year: 2017
  ident: B57
  article-title: Preschoolers flexibly adapt to linguistic input in a noisy channel.
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1177/0956797616668557
– volume: 181
  start-page: 80
  year: 2018
  ident: B15
  article-title: Sins of omission are more likely to be forgiven in non-native speakers.
  publication-title: Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.010
– volume: 34
  start-page: 388
  year: 2016
  ident: B39
  article-title: Thee, uhh disfluency effect in preschoolers: a cue to discourse status.
  publication-title: Br. J. Dev. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12137
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1062
  year: 2014
  ident: B52
  article-title: Children’s perception of dialect variation.
  publication-title: J.Child Lang.
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000913000330
– volume: 28
  start-page: 703
  year: 2017
  ident: B19
  article-title: Don’t underestimate the benefits of being misunderstood.
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1177/0956797617690277
– volume: 49
  year: 2013
  ident: B11
  article-title: Accuracy trumps accent in children’s endorsement of object labels.
  publication-title: Dev. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1037/a0030604
– volume: 55
  start-page: 809
  year: 2019
  ident: B41
  article-title: The effect of accent exposure on children’s sociolinguistic evaluation of peers.
  publication-title: Dev. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1037/dev0000659
– volume: 76
  start-page: 1261
  year: 2005
  ident: B30
  article-title: Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers.
  publication-title: Child Dev.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x
– volume: 49
  start-page: 523
  year: 2013
  ident: B47
  article-title: Children’s understanding of speaker reliability between lexical and syntactic knowledge.
  publication-title: Dev. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1037/a0029658
– volume: 32
  start-page: 3
  year: 2013
  ident: B40
  article-title: Measuring implicit and explicit attitudes toward foreign accented speech.
  publication-title: J. Lang. Soc. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1177/0261927X12463005
– volume: 80
  start-page: 23
  year: 2009
  ident: B37
  article-title: Children’s trust in previously inaccurate informants who were well or poorly informed: When past errors can be excused.
  publication-title: Child Dev.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01243.x
– volume: 86
  start-page: 1701
  year: 2015
  ident: B38
  article-title: I forget what that’s called! Children’s online processing of disfluencies depends on speaker knowledge.
  publication-title: Child Dev.
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.12421
– volume: 40
  start-page: 532
  year: 2009
  ident: B16
  article-title: An effect size primer: a guide for clinicians and researchers. professional psychology
  publication-title: Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract.
  doi: 10.1037/a0015808
– volume: 110
  start-page: 8051
  year: 2013
  ident: B18
  article-title: Rational integration of noisy evidence and prior semantic expectations in sentence interpretation.
  publication-title: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216438110
– volume: 86
  start-page: 259
  year: 2015
  ident: B23
  article-title: Infants’ and young children’s imitation of linguistic in-group and out-group informants.
  publication-title: Child Dev.
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.12299
SSID ssj0000402002
Score 2.3223665
Snippet Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers...
SourceID doaj
pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
SourceType Open Website
Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
StartPage 855130
SubjectTerms accent-based expectations
foreign accents
grammatical errors
Psychology
speaker reliability
speech errors
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  dbid: DOA
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1LT-MwELYQJy6I525YQEbihJTFdZykOfJGSFRICxI3y4_xLlo2rdJy6L9nxglVixB74ZjElq1vJp5vZPsbxg4dlKrvDaTG5AUmKL5ILfgy7fVCRermIcR7a7eD4vpB3Tzmj3OlvuhMWCsP3AJ3rFzmMYxaa41TwuGYoShLj7Q5GCWyqASKMW8umYprMKVFQrbbmJiFVcdhNJ7-xnxQyp99KmoiFgJR1Ov_iGS-Pys5F3wu19hqxxr5STvbdbYE9QZbmS1e0012d9bdyub3RAP5VWP-RTVW7HbRNMNmzM-7WiiT5ylHqsoHUfObm9rzwbBOu8dfIzB_kRNusYfLi_uz67SrlpA6VeST1HmEKHOVt5UMmPUJFaR1CLgN1uCaB15BbnsOGQL40DdelnRJNRMBOVcJIdtmy_Wwhu-MI0pe5hmIrB-UrZyBqlKlFS6XEMBBwsQbdNp1UuJU0eJZY0pBaOuItia0dYt2wo5mXUatjsZnjU_JHrOGJIEdX6Bj6M4x9P8cI2EHb9bU-MvQPoipYfgy1pGTEPMsEvatte5sKKRfkkT5ElYu2H1hLotf6qc_UZYboSa2uvMVk__BVggP2raScpctT5oX2EP2M7H70dFfAQNaBtE
  priority: 102
  providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals
Title Children Treat Grammatical Errors Differently for Native and Non-Native Speakers
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35529559
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2661483526
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9075105
https://doaj.org/article/4c3d063bbbac40cebef677d642fa4034
Volume 13
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwlV1ba9swFBale-nLWLdu8y5FhT4V3DmybNkPZbRdLxQaBmugb0LXbiyzUyeF5d_3HFkJywiFPsqWbOscXb7Pkr5DyL5xgldWuVSpogSCYstUOyvSwcDXqG7ufTi3dj0sL0f86ra43SCL8FbRgNO11A7jSY268eHf-_lX6PBHyDhhvv3iJ9P5HVA9xg4rjFcCDP4FTEwCAxpcR7QfBmbkSv0uxLLkMACJql_nXP-UlZkqCPqvQ6H_b6b8Z3Y6f0VeRlhJj_t2sE02XPOabC1Ht_kb8v00HtumN4gT6UWn_gS5Vih21nVtN6XfYrCU2XhOAcvSYRAFp6qxdNg2aUz-mDj1G0DjDhmdn92cXqYxnEJqeFnMUmO1Vrmpra6ZB1qYcc-0AY9orxUMis5yV-iBAQjhrK-UZQJPseaZB1AmnM_fks2mbdx7QsFKlhW5y_LKc10b5eqaC52ZgjnvjEtItjCdNFFrHENejCVwDrS2DNaWaG3ZWzshB8sik15o46nMJ-iPZUbUyA4X2u5Oxi4nucktADANtTY8M9BafSmEBcLlFc9ynpC9hTcl9ClcKFGNax-mMoAWhKZlQt713l2-CvAZQ9W-hIgVv698y-qd5tfPoNsNpkY4--E5Nf1ItjCF61eMfSKbs-7BfQYYNNO74ffBbmjij687CFE
linkProvider Scholars Portal
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Children+Treat+Grammatical+Errors+Differently+for+Native+and+Non-Native+Speakers&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+psychology&rft.au=Rett%2C+Alexandra&rft.au=White%2C+Katherine+S.&rft.date=2022-04-22&rft.issn=1664-1078&rft.eissn=1664-1078&rft.volume=13&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2022.855130&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_3389_fpsyg_2022_855130
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon