Spatial orientation and balance control changes induced by altered gravitoinertial force vectors

To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects ("abnormal"; four bilateral and four unilateral lesions) to an interaural centripetal acceleration of 1 g (resultant 45 degrees roll-tilt of...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inExperimental brain research Vol. 137; no. 4-Mar; pp. 397 - 410
Main Authors Kaufman, G. D., Wood, S. J., Gianna, C. C., Black, F. O., Paloski, W. H.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Johnson Space Center Springer 01.04.2001
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects
Ear
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0014-4819
1432-1106
DOI10.1007/s002210000636

Cover

Loading…
Abstract To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects ("abnormal"; four bilateral and four unilateral lesions) to an interaural centripetal acceleration of 1 g (resultant 45 degrees roll-tilt of 1.4 g) on a 0.8-m-radius centrifuge for periods of 90 min. The subjects sat upright (body z-axis parallel to centrifuge rotation axis) in the dark with head stationary, except during 4 min of every 10 min, when they performed head saccades toward visual targets switched on at 3- to 5-s intervals at random locations (within +/- 30 degrees) in the earth-horizontal plane. Eight of the normal subjects also performed the head saccade protocol in a stationary chair adjusted to a static roll-tilt angle of 45 degrees for 90 min (reproducing the change in orientation but not the magnitude of the gravitoinertial force on the centrifuge). Eye movements, including voluntary saccades directed along perceived earth- and head-referenced planes, were recorded before, during, and immediately after centrifugation. Postural center of pressure (COP) and multisegment body kinematics were also gathered before and within 10 min after centrifugation. Normal subjects overestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation and revealed errors in perception of head-vertical provided by directed saccades. Errors in this perceptual response tended to increase with time and became significant after approximately 30 min. Motion-sickness symptoms caused approximately 25% of normal subjects to limit their head movements during centrifugation and led three normal subjects to stop the test early. Immediately after centrifugation, subjects reported feeling tilted 10 degrees in the opposite direction, which was in agreement with the direction of their earth-referenced directed saccades. Postural COP, segmental body motion amplitude, and hip-sway frequency increased significantly after centrifugation. These postural effects were short-lived, however, with a recovery time of several postural test trials (minutes). There were also asymmetries in the direction of postcentrifugation COP and head tilt which depended on the subject's orientation during the centrifugation adaptation period (left ear or right ear out). The amount of total head movements during centrifugation correlated poorly or inversely with postcentrifugation postural stability, and the most unstable subject made no head movements. There was no decrease in postural stability after static tilt, although these subjects also reported a perceived tilt briefly after return to upright, and they also had COP asymmetries. Abnormal subjects underestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation, and their directed saccades revealed permanent spatial distortions. Bilateral abnormal subjects started out with poor postural control, but showed no postural decrements after centrifugation, while unilateral abnormal subjects had varying degrees of postural decrement, both in their everyday function and as a result of experiencing the centrifugation. In addition, three unilateral, abnormal subjects, who rode twice in opposite orientations, revealed a consistent orthogonal pattern of COP offsets after centrifugation. These results suggest that both orientation and magnitude of the gravitoinertial vector are used by the central nervous system for calibration of multiple orientation systems. A change in the background gravitoinertial force (otolith input) can rapidly initiate postural and perceptual adaptation in several sensorimotor systems, independent of a structured visual surround.
AbstractList To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects ("abnormal"; four bilateral and four unilateral lesions) to an interaural centripetal acceleration of 1g (resultant 45° roll-tilt of 1.4g) on a 0.8-m-radius centrifuge for periods of 90 min. The subjects sat upright (body z-axis parallel to centrifuge rotation axis) in the dark with head stationary, except during 4 min of every 10 min, when they performed head saccades toward visual targets switched on at 3- to 5-s intervals at random locations (within ±30°) in the earth-horizontal plane. Eight of the normal subjects also performed the head saccade protocol in a stationary chair adjusted to a static roll-tilt angle of 45° for 90 min (reproducing the change in orientation but not the magnitude of the gravitoinertial force on the centrifuge). Eye movements, including voluntary saccades directed along perceived earth- and head-referenced planes, were recorded before, during, and immediately after centrifugation. Postural center of pressure (COP) and multisegment body kinematics were also gathered before and within 10 min after centrifugation. Normal subjects overestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation and revealed errors in perception of head-vertical provided by directed saccades. Errors in this perceptual response tended to increase with time and became significant after approximately 30 min. Motion-sickness symptoms caused approximately 25% of normal subjects to limit their head movements during centrifugation and led three normal subjects to stop the test early. Immediately after centrifugation, subjects reported feeling tilted 10° in the opposite direction, which was in agreement with the direction of their earth-referenced directed saccades. Postural COP, segmental body motion amplitude, and hip-sway frequency increased significantly after centrifugation. These postural effects were short-lived, however, with a recovery time of several postural test trials (minutes). There were also asymmetries in the direction of postcentrifugation COP and head tilt which depended on the subject's orientation during the centrifugation adaptation period (left ear or right ear out). The amount of total head movements during centrifugation correlated poorly or inversely with postcentrifugation postural stability, and the most unstable subject made no head movements. There was no decrease in postural stability after static tilt, although these subjects also reported a perceived tilt briefly after return to upright, and they also had COP asymmetries. Abnormal subjects underestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation, and their directed saccades revealed permanent spatial distortions. Bilateral abnormal subjects started out with poor postural control, but showed no postural decrements after centrifugation, while unilateral abnormal subjects had varying degrees of postural decrement, both in their everyday function and as a result of experiencing the centrifugation. In addition, three unilateral, abnormal subjects, who rode twice in opposite orientations, revealed a consistent orthogonal pattern of COP offsets after centrifugation. These results suggest that both orientation and magnitude of the gravitoinertial vector are used by the central nervous system for calibration of multiple orientation systems. A change in the background gravitoinertial force (otolith input) can rapidly initiate postural and perceptual adaptation in several sensorimotor systems, independent of a structured visual surround.
To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects ("abnormal"; four bilateral and four unilateral lesions) to an interaural centripetal acceleration of 1 g (resultant 45 degrees roll-tilt of 1.4 g) on a 0.8-m-radius centrifuge for periods of 90 min. The subjects sat upright (body z-axis parallel to centrifuge rotation axis) in the dark with head stationary, except during 4 min of every 10 min, when they performed head saccades toward visual targets switched on at 3- to 5-s intervals at random locations (within +/- 30 degrees) in the earth-horizontal plane. Eight of the normal subjects also performed the head saccade protocol in a stationary chair adjusted to a static roll-tilt angle of 45 degrees for 90 min (reproducing the change in orientation but not the magnitude of the gravitoinertial force on the centrifuge). Eye movements, including voluntary saccades directed along perceived earth- and head-referenced planes, were recorded before, during, and immediately after centrifugation. Postural center of pressure (COP) and multisegment body kinematics were also gathered before and within 10 min after centrifugation. Normal subjects overestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation and revealed errors in perception of head-vertical provided by directed saccades. Errors in this perceptual response tended to increase with time and became significant after approximately 30 min. Motion-sickness symptoms caused approximately 25% of normal subjects to limit their head movements during centrifugation and led three normal subjects to stop the test early. Immediately after centrifugation, subjects reported feeling tilted 10 degrees in the opposite direction, which was in agreement with the direction of their earth-referenced directed saccades. Postural COP, segmental body motion amplitude, and hip-sway frequency increased significantly after centrifugation. These postural effects were short-lived, however, with a recovery time of several postural test trials (minutes). There were also asymmetries in the direction of postcentrifugation COP and head tilt which depended on the subject's orientation during the centrifugation adaptation period (left ear or right ear out). The amount of total head movements during centrifugation correlated poorly or inversely with postcentrifugation postural stability, and the most unstable subject made no head movements. There was no decrease in postural stability after static tilt, although these subjects also reported a perceived tilt briefly after return to upright, and they also had COP asymmetries. Abnormal subjects underestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation, and their directed saccades revealed permanent spatial distortions. Bilateral abnormal subjects started out with poor postural control, but showed no postural decrements after centrifugation, while unilateral abnormal subjects had varying degrees of postural decrement, both in their everyday function and as a result of experiencing the centrifugation. In addition, three unilateral, abnormal subjects, who rode twice in opposite orientations, revealed a consistent orthogonal pattern of COP offsets after centrifugation. These results suggest that both orientation and magnitude of the gravitoinertial vector are used by the central nervous system for calibration of multiple orientation systems. A change in the background gravitoinertial force (otolith input) can rapidly initiate postural and perceptual adaptation in several sensorimotor systems, independent of a structured visual surround.To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects ("abnormal"; four bilateral and four unilateral lesions) to an interaural centripetal acceleration of 1 g (resultant 45 degrees roll-tilt of 1.4 g) on a 0.8-m-radius centrifuge for periods of 90 min. The subjects sat upright (body z-axis parallel to centrifuge rotation axis) in the dark with head stationary, except during 4 min of every 10 min, when they performed head saccades toward visual targets switched on at 3- to 5-s intervals at random locations (within +/- 30 degrees) in the earth-horizontal plane. Eight of the normal subjects also performed the head saccade protocol in a stationary chair adjusted to a static roll-tilt angle of 45 degrees for 90 min (reproducing the change in orientation but not the magnitude of the gravitoinertial force on the centrifuge). Eye movements, including voluntary saccades directed along perceived earth- and head-referenced planes, were recorded before, during, and immediately after centrifugation. Postural center of pressure (COP) and multisegment body kinematics were also gathered before and within 10 min after centrifugation. Normal subjects overestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation and revealed errors in perception of head-vertical provided by directed saccades. Errors in this perceptual response tended to increase with time and became significant after approximately 30 min. Motion-sickness symptoms caused approximately 25% of normal subjects to limit their head movements during centrifugation and led three normal subjects to stop the test early. Immediately after centrifugation, subjects reported feeling tilted 10 degrees in the opposite direction, which was in agreement with the direction of their earth-referenced directed saccades. Postural COP, segmental body motion amplitude, and hip-sway frequency increased significantly after centrifugation. These postural effects were short-lived, however, with a recovery time of several postural test trials (minutes). There were also asymmetries in the direction of postcentrifugation COP and head tilt which depended on the subject's orientation during the centrifugation adaptation period (left ear or right ear out). The amount of total head movements during centrifugation correlated poorly or inversely with postcentrifugation postural stability, and the most unstable subject made no head movements. There was no decrease in postural stability after static tilt, although these subjects also reported a perceived tilt briefly after return to upright, and they also had COP asymmetries. Abnormal subjects underestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation, and their directed saccades revealed permanent spatial distortions. Bilateral abnormal subjects started out with poor postural control, but showed no postural decrements after centrifugation, while unilateral abnormal subjects had varying degrees of postural decrement, both in their everyday function and as a result of experiencing the centrifugation. In addition, three unilateral, abnormal subjects, who rode twice in opposite orientations, revealed a consistent orthogonal pattern of COP offsets after centrifugation. These results suggest that both orientation and magnitude of the gravitoinertial vector are used by the central nervous system for calibration of multiple orientation systems. A change in the background gravitoinertial force (otolith input) can rapidly initiate postural and perceptual adaptation in several sensorimotor systems, independent of a structured visual surround.
To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects ("abnormal"; four bilateral and four unilateral lesions) to an interaural centripetal acceleration of 1 g (resultant 45 degrees roll-tilt of 1.4 g) on a 0.8-m-radius centrifuge for periods of 90 min. The subjects sat upright (body z-axis parallel to centrifuge rotation axis) in the dark with head stationary, except during 4 min of every 10 min, when they performed head saccades toward visual targets switched on at 3- to 5-s intervals at random locations (within +/- 30 degrees) in the earth-horizontal plane. Eight of the normal subjects also performed the head saccade protocol in a stationary chair adjusted to a static roll-tilt angle of 45 degrees for 90 min (reproducing the change in orientation but not the magnitude of the gravitoinertial force on the centrifuge). Eye movements, including voluntary saccades directed along perceived earth- and head-referenced planes, were recorded before, during, and immediately after centrifugation. Postural center of pressure (COP) and multisegment body kinematics were also gathered before and within 10 min after centrifugation. Normal subjects overestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation and revealed errors in perception of head-vertical provided by directed saccades. Errors in this perceptual response tended to increase with time and became significant after approximately 30 min. Motion-sickness symptoms caused approximately 25% of normal subjects to limit their head movements during centrifugation and led three normal subjects to stop the test early. Immediately after centrifugation, subjects reported feeling tilted 10 degrees in the opposite direction, which was in agreement with the direction of their earth-referenced directed saccades. Postural COP, segmental body motion amplitude, and hip-sway frequency increased significantly after centrifugation. These postural effects were short-lived, however, with a recovery time of several postural test trials (minutes). There were also asymmetries in the direction of postcentrifugation COP and head tilt which depended on the subject's orientation during the centrifugation adaptation period (left ear or right ear out). The amount of total head movements during centrifugation correlated poorly or inversely with postcentrifugation postural stability, and the most unstable subject made no head movements. There was no decrease in postural stability after static tilt, although these subjects also reported a perceived tilt briefly after return to upright, and they also had COP asymmetries. Abnormal subjects underestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation, and their directed saccades revealed permanent spatial distortions. Bilateral abnormal subjects started out with poor postural control, but showed no postural decrements after centrifugation, while unilateral abnormal subjects had varying degrees of postural decrement, both in their everyday function and as a result of experiencing the centrifugation. In addition, three unilateral, abnormal subjects, who rode twice in opposite orientations, revealed a consistent orthogonal pattern of COP offsets after centrifugation. These results suggest that both orientation and magnitude of the gravitoinertial vector are used by the central nervous system for calibration of multiple orientation systems. A change in the background gravitoinertial force (otolith input) can rapidly initiate postural and perceptual adaptation in several sensorimotor systems, independent of a structured visual surround.
To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects ("abnormal; four bilateral and four unilateral lesions) to an interaural centripetal acceleration of 1g (resultant 45 degree roll-tilt of 1.4g) on a 0.8-m-radius centrifuge for periods of 90 min. The subjects sat upright (bodyz-axis parallel to centrifuge rotation axis) in the dark with head stationary, except during 4 min of every 10 min, when they performed head saccades toward visual targets switched on at 3- to 5-s intervals at random locations (within +30 degree ) in the earth-horizontal plane. Eight of the normal subjects also performed the head saccade protocol in a stationary chair adjusted to a static roll-tilt angle of 45 degree for 90 min (reproducing the change in orientation but not the magnitude of the gravitoinertial force on the centrifuge). Eye movements, including voluntary saccades directed along perceived earth- and head-referenced planes, were recorded before, during, and immediately after centrifugation. Postural center of pressure (COP) and multisegment body kinematics were also gathered before and within 10 min after centrifugation.Normal subjects overestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation and revealed errors in perception of head-vertical provided by directed saccades. Errors in this perceptual response tended to increase with time and became significant after approximately 30 min. Motion-sickness symptoms caused approximately 25% of normal subjects to limit their head movements during centrifugation and led three normal subjects to stop the test early. Immediately after centrifugation, subjects reported feeling tilted 10 degree in the opposite direction, which was in agreement with the direction of their earth-referenced directed saccades. Postural COP, segmental body motion amplitude, and hip-sway frequency increased significantly after centrifugation. These postural effects were short-lived, however, with a recovery time of several postural test trials (minutes). There were also asymmetries in the direction of postcentrifugation COP and head tilt which depended on the subject's orientation during the centrifugation adaptation period (left ear or right ear out). The amount of total head movements during centrifugation correlated poorly or inversely with postcentrifugation postural stability, and the most unstable subject made no head movements. There was no decrease in postural stability after static tilt, although these subjects also reported a perceived tilt briefly after return to upright, and they also had COP asymmetries.Abnormal subjects underestimated roll-tilt during centrifugation, and their directed saccades revealed permanent spatial distortions. Bilateral abnormal subjects started out with poor postural control, but showed no postural decrements after centrifugation, while unilateral abnormal subjects had varying degrees of postural decrement, both in their everyday function and as a result of experiencing the centrifugation. In addition, three unilateral, abnormal subjects, who rode twice in opposite orientations, revealed a consistent orthogonal pattern of COP offsets after centrifugation. These results suggest that both orientation and magnitude of the gravitoinertial vector are used by the central nervous system for calibration of multiple orientation systems. A change in the background gravitoinertial force (otolith input) can rapidly initiate postural and perceptual adaptation in several sensorimotor systems, independent of a structured visual surround.
Audience PUBLIC
Author Black, F. O.
Kaufman, G. D.
Gianna, C. C.
Wood, S. J.
Paloski, W. H.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: G. D.
  surname: Kaufman
  fullname: Kaufman, G. D.
  organization: NASA Johnson Space Center
– sequence: 2
  givenname: S. J.
  surname: Wood
  fullname: Wood, S. J.
– sequence: 3
  givenname: C. C.
  surname: Gianna
  fullname: Gianna, C. C.
– sequence: 4
  givenname: F. O.
  surname: Black
  fullname: Black, F. O.
– sequence: 5
  givenname: W. H.
  surname: Paloski
  fullname: Paloski, W. H.
BackLink http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=1100076$$DView record in Pascal Francis
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11355385$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqF0c9rFDEUB_AgFbutHr2JDCjeRl9-TH4cpfijUOiheh7fJpmaMpusSabQ_96su6ItiKe8kM8LvPc9IUcxRU_IcwpvKYB6VwAYaxWA5PIRWVHBWU8pyCOyAqCiF5qaY3JSys3uyhU8IceU8mHgeliRb1dbrAHnLuXgY211ih1G161xxmh9Z1OsOc2d_Y7x2pcuRLdY397vOpyrz628zngbagrR519fTSm3xltva8rlKXk84Vz8s8N5Sr5-_PDl7HN_cfnp_Oz9RW-FFLVHh0ZPEoz2jBtrJFNKMrZmXArnNaChwk0C13zQwkkDftDDhI5xraXTyE_Jm_2_25x-LL7UcROK9XObwqeljAq0ZIOG_0KqNBMMdvDVA3iTlhzbECNTVFFDwYimXh7Ust54N25z2GC-G3-vuIHXB4DF4jzlttZQ_nItOiUb6_fM5lRK9tMfAeMu6fFe0s3zB96GfXw1Y5j_2fVi3xWx4NhomwVAAKVMCMZ_ArVAsYI
CODEN EXBRAP
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00140_2018
crossref_primary_10_1589_jpts_14_69
crossref_primary_10_1177_1045389X19898752
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainresrev_2005_06_001
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_006_0697_y
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_024_06940_6
crossref_primary_10_3389_fneur_2018_00323
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2005_04_002
crossref_primary_10_3109_08990220_2015_1029608
ContentType Journal Article
Contributor Black, F O
Paloski, W H
Contributor_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: F O
  surname: Black
  fullname: Black, F O
  organization: Legacy Health Sys, Portland, OR
– sequence: 2
  givenname: W H
  surname: Paloski
  fullname: Paloski, W H
  organization: NASA JSC
Copyright 2001 INIST-CNRS
Springer-Verlag 2001.
Copyright_xml – notice: 2001 INIST-CNRS
– notice: Springer-Verlag 2001.
DBID CYE
CYI
AAYXX
CITATION
IQODW
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
0-V
3V.
7QP
7QR
7RV
7TK
7TM
7X7
7XB
88E
88G
88J
8AO
8FD
8FI
8FJ
8FK
ABUWG
AFKRA
ALSLI
AZQEC
BENPR
CCPQU
DWQXO
FR3
FYUFA
GHDGH
GNUQQ
K9.
KB0
M0S
M1P
M2M
M2R
NAPCQ
P64
PHGZM
PHGZT
PJZUB
PKEHL
POGQB
PPXIY
PQEST
PQQKQ
PQUKI
PRINS
PRQQA
PSYQQ
Q9U
RC3
7X8
DOI 10.1007/s002210000636
DatabaseName NASA Scientific and Technical Information
NASA Technical Reports Server
CrossRef
Pascal-Francis
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】
ProQuest Central (Corporate)
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Nursing & Allied Health Database
Neurosciences Abstracts
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Health & Medical Collection
ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)
Medical Database (Alumni Edition)
Psychology Database (Alumni)
Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Pharma Collection
Technology Research Database
ProQuest Hospital Collection
Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
ProQuest Central UK/Ireland
Social Science Premium Collection
ProQuest Central Essentials
ProQuest Central
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest Central Korea
Engineering Research Database
Health Research Premium Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Health & Medical Collection
Medical Database
Psychology Database
Social Science Database (subscription)
Nursing & Allied Health Premium
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
ProQuest Central Premium
ProQuest One Academic (New)
ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection
ProQuest One Health & Nursing
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)
ProQuest One Academic
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Central China
ProQuest One Social Sciences
ProQuest One Psychology
ProQuest Central Basic
Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
ProQuest One Psychology
ProQuest Central Student
ProQuest Central Essentials
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Sociology & Social Sciences Collection
ProQuest Central China
Health Research Premium Collection
Health & Medical Research Collection
Chemoreception Abstracts
ProQuest Central (New)
ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni)
Social Science Premium Collection
ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition
ProQuest Hospital Collection
Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Nursing & Allied Health Premium
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete
ProQuest Social Science Journals
ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection
ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition
ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source (Alumni)
Engineering Research Database
ProQuest One Academic
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
ProQuest One Academic (New)
ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection
Technology Research Database
ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New)
ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest One Community College
ProQuest One Health & Nursing
ProQuest Pharma Collection
ProQuest Central
ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection
Genetics Abstracts
Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition)
ProQuest Central Korea
ProQuest One Social Sciences
ProQuest Central Basic
ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source
ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni)
ProQuest Medical Library
ProQuest Psychology Journals
ProQuest Central (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList ProQuest One Psychology
MEDLINE - Academic
MEDLINE
Neurosciences Abstracts

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: BENPR
  name: ProQuest Central
  url: https://www.proquest.com/central
  sourceTypes: Aggregation Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
Psychology
EISSN 1432-1106
EndPage 410
ExternalDocumentID 11355385
1100076
10_1007_s002210000636
20040112442
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Research Support, U.S. Gov't, P.H.S
Clinical Trial
Journal Article
GrantInformation DC 00205
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: NIDCD NIH HHS
  grantid: DC 00205
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-XW
-Y2
-~C
-~X
.55
.86
.GJ
.VR
06C
06D
0R~
0VY
199
1N0
1SB
203
29G
29~
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2P1
2VQ
2~H
30V
36B
3O-
4.4
406
408
409
40D
40E
53G
5GY
5RE
5VS
67N
67Z
6NX
78A
7RV
7X7
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8TC
8UJ
95-
95.
95~
96X
AABHQ
AACDK
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AANXM
AANZL
AAPKM
AARHV
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
AAYZH
ABAKF
ABBBX
ABBRH
ABBXA
ABDBE
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFSG
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABHQN
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABKCH
ABKTR
ABLJU
ABMNI
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABQBU
ABQSL
ABRTQ
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABULA
ABUWG
ABWNU
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACBXY
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIWK
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACOMO
ACPIV
ACPRK
ACSTC
ACZOJ
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADHKG
ADIMF
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMSY
AENEX
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AETLH
AEVLU
AEXYK
AEZWR
AFBBN
AFDZB
AFFNX
AFGCZ
AFHIU
AFKRA
AFLOW
AFQWF
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGQPQ
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHBYD
AHKAY
AHMBA
AHPBZ
AHSBF
AHWEU
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AILAN
AITGF
AIXLP
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
AKMHD
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALSLI
ALWAN
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
ARMRJ
ASPBG
ATHPR
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYFIA
AZFZN
AZQEC
B-.
BA0
BDATZ
BENPR
BGNMA
BPHCQ
BSONS
CAG
CCPQU
COF
CS3
CSCUP
CYE
CYI
DDRTE
DL5
DNIVK
DPUIP
DU5
DWQXO
EBD
EBLON
EBS
EIOEI
EJD
EMB
EMOBN
EN4
EPAXT
ESBYG
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRRFC
FSGXE
FWDCC
FYUFA
G-Y
G-Z
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ7
GQ8
GXS
HF~
HG5
HG6
HMCUK
HMJXF
HQYDN
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
I09
IAO
IHE
IHR
IHW
IJ-
IKXTQ
INH
INR
IPY
ISR
ITM
IWAJR
IXC
IZIGR
IZQ
I~X
I~Z
J-C
J0Z
JBSCW
JCJTX
JZLTJ
KDC
KOV
KPH
L7B
LAS
LLZTM
M1P
M2M
M2R
M4Y
MA-
N2Q
NAPCQ
NB0
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O9-
O93
O9G
O9I
O9J
OAM
OVD
P19
P2P
PF-
PHGZM
PHGZT
PJZUB
PPXIY
PRQQA
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT4
PT5
QOK
QOR
QOS
R89
R9I
RHV
RIG
ROL
RPX
RRX
RSV
S16
S27
S3A
S3B
SAP
SBL
SBY
SDH
SDM
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
TEORI
TSG
TSK
TSV
TUC
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UOJIU
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
W23
W48
WH7
WJK
WK8
X7M
YLTOR
Z45
ZGI
ZMTXR
ZOVNA
ZXP
~EX
~KM
AAYXX
ALIPV
CITATION
0-V
2.D
28-
3SX
5QI
AAYJJ
ADBBV
ADYPR
AEFIE
AFEXP
AFOHR
AGGDS
AHAVH
AIIXL
ARALO
BBWZM
BKEYQ
BVXVI
EX3
FA8
H13
IQODW
ITC
KOW
NDZJH
OHT
PQQKQ
PROAC
Q2X
R4E
RNI
RZK
S1Z
S26
S28
SCLPG
T16
WK6
WOW
-4W
-56
-5G
-BR
-EM
3V.
AAAVM
ADINQ
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
GQ6
NPM
PKN
Z7R
Z7U
Z7W
Z7X
Z82
Z83
Z87
Z88
Z8M
Z8O
Z8Q
Z8R
Z8V
Z8W
Z91
Z92
7QP
7QR
7TK
7TM
7XB
8FD
8FK
FR3
K9.
P64
PKEHL
POGQB
PQEST
PQUKI
PRINS
PUEGO
Q9U
RC3
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c464t-ada98f6098e239c96277622b2364de80a914df4ab3584d690e585fad23886d8a3
IEDL.DBID BENPR
ISSN 0014-4819
IngestDate Thu Sep 04 17:33:01 EDT 2025
Thu Sep 04 21:50:59 EDT 2025
Sat Aug 23 14:30:04 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:32:39 EST 2025
Mon Jul 21 09:13:15 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:03:26 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 04:11:20 EDT 2025
Fri Aug 15 15:20:52 EDT 2025
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 4-Mar
Keywords Verbal Behavior/physiology
Middle Aged
Support, U.s. Gov't, P.h.s
Centrifugation
Non-Nasa Center
Male
Nasa Discipline Neuroscience
Otolithic Membrane/physiology
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology
Coriolis Force
Adult
Female
Musculoskeletal Equilibrium/physiology
Orientation/physiology
Space Perception/physiology
Support, U.s. Gov't, Non-P.h.s
Human
Gravitation
Clinical Trial
Head Movements/physiology
Nasa Center Jsc
Eye Movements/physiology
Algorithms
Adolescent
Models, Neurological
Acceleration
Nervous system diseases
Proprioception
Spatial orientation
Eye movement
Rotation
Posture
Sensorimotor coordination
Body movement
ENT disease
Equilibration
Tilt
Vestibular syndrome
Vestibular system
Adaptation
Gravity
NASA Discipline Neuroscience
NASA Center JSC
Non-NASA Center
Language English
License http://www.springer.com/tdm
CC BY 4.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c464t-ada98f6098e239c96277622b2364de80a914df4ab3584d690e585fad23886d8a3
Notes Johnson Space Center
JSC
ISSN: 0014-4819
ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
ObjectType-Article-2
ObjectType-Feature-1
content type line 23
PMID 11355385
PQID 2717191094
PQPubID 47176
PageCount 14
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_70862580
proquest_miscellaneous_17824200
proquest_journals_2717191094
pubmed_primary_11355385
pascalfrancis_primary_1100076
crossref_primary_10_1007_s002210000636
crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s002210000636
nasa_ntrs_20040112442
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2001-04-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2001-04-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 04
  year: 2001
  text: 2001-04-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2000
PublicationPlace Johnson Space Center
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Johnson Space Center
– name: Berlin
– name: Germany
– name: Heidelberg
PublicationTitle Experimental brain research
PublicationTitleAlternate Exp Brain Res
PublicationYear 2001
Publisher Springer
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer
– name: Springer Nature B.V
SSID ssj0014370
Score 1.7321346
Snippet To better understand the mechanisms of human adaptation to rotating environments, we exposed 19 healthy subjects and 8 vestibular-deficient subjects...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
pascalfrancis
crossref
nasa
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 397
SubjectTerms Acceleration
Adaptation, Physiological - physiology
Adolescent
Adult
Algorithms
Biological and medical sciences
Central nervous system
Centrifugation
Centrifuges
Coriolis Force
Ear
Eye Movements - physiology
Female
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Gravitation
Head Movements - physiology
Humans
Kinematics
Life Sciences (General)
Male
Middle Aged
Models, Neurological
motion sickness
Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration
Orientation - physiology
Orientation behavior
Otolithic Membrane - physiology
Postural Balance - physiology
Posture
Saccadic eye movements
Sensorimotor system
Space Perception - physiology
Verbal Behavior - physiology
Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs
Vestibular system
Title Spatial orientation and balance control changes induced by altered gravitoinertial force vectors
URI https://ntrs.nasa.gov/citations/20040112442
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11355385
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2717191094
https://www.proquest.com/docview/17824200
https://www.proquest.com/docview/70862580
Volume 137
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1NT9wwEB2V5cKlKl9tWqA-VJywukmcxDlVgECoUlFVgbS34MSOVKnNUrJU2n_fN443wGF7ixTHljzOzHsz4xmiT20LkFGbWMYgQFLVTSINrK7MgOSmpjYu881gvl3nV7fq6yybBYdbH9IqVzrRK2o7b9hH_jkB7wC3ABv5cv9Hctcojq6GFhobtAkVrLMJbZ5dXH__McYRVFoMl1BiJRWMX6iy6S_Psfny3u3c12d-skqTzvSGkyRNj31qhwYX6xGot0SXb-h1gJDidJD5Nr1y3Q7tnnagz7-X4lj4pE7vLd-hrVHBLXfpjvsP47yJ-cPPcOWoE6azoub8xsaJkLcuhtvAvQBfh-Txfil8VB2P3K4ISoCvDPqpgHnx4V_v--_36Pby4ub8SoYOC7JRuVpIY02p23xaapekZcONeKAck5qrylunp6aMlW2VqVPgFAsi7cAuWmNh53VutUn3sVnzzr0jUSrd6rTNbWOdaixoVAHolCrAHz1NbBnRyWqHqyaUH-cuGL-qsXDyc4FEdDwOvx_qbqwbuMfiqjBZzz01wRcZsiQRHb6Q39MssQ8_RnSwkmcVflt8Px6yiD6Or_HDcRTFdG7-2FcxMJXCQutHFEwTM40Rb4dz8mxtwLtUZ-__v_gH2hpS3ThB6IAmi4dHdwjss6iPaKOYFUfhmP8DgN__mA
linkProvider ProQuest
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1LT9wwEB5ROJQLKo-2oVB8aDk16iZxss6hqqAFLQVWVQUSt-DEjoRUskCWVvun-hv7jfOAHrY3bpHih-QZz3zjeRG9K0uAjFwHfgADyJd5EfoaWtePgeQGOtc2ds1gTsfJ6Fx-u4gvFuhPlwvDYZWdTHSC2kwKfiP_GMLugG0Ba-Tzza3PXaPYu9q10GjY4tjOfsNkqz8dfQV934fh4cHZl5HfdhXwC5nIqa-NTlWZDFJlwygtuPkMBEKYcyV1Y9VAp4E0pdR5BN1sYDxaIOpSG-g2lRilI6z7jJYAM1LcoqX9g_H3H73fQkbDJuklkL6Esm2rerpkPVaX7jU9cfWgH7TgYqVrzUGZugZdyqahxnzE6zTf4QtaaSGr2Gt4bJUWbLVG63sVzPXrmdgVLojUvc6v0XIvUGfrdMn9jsHfYnJ31aY4VUJXRuQcT1lY0cbJiyb7uBZXlQGn4f9MOC8-Prk9EoQOpyi6pYCxMfGX8zXUG3T-JGf_Eoc1qexrEqlUpYrKxBTGysLAbBsCqkUScEsNQpN69KE74axoy51z142fWV-o-TFBPNrth980dT7mDdxgcmVYrOYenrBPGSKFHm3_Q7-HVQLn7vRoq6Nn1ooJzO-Z2qOd_jcuOHttdGUn93UWAMNJbDR_xJDN0lhhxKuGTx7tDTgZqXjz_5vv0PPR2elJdnI0Pn5Dy02YHQcnbdHi9O7ebgN3TfO3LbMLunzq-_UXUpA6mw
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1bT9RAFD7BJTG8GLmoVZB5UJ5s2LbT7vTBEBA2ILohRhLeyrQzTUi0i3TR7F_z1_Gd6QV8WN94a9K5JHPOnPOdOTeid2UJkJHrwA9gAPkyL0JfQ-v6MZDcUOfaxq4ZzNdJcnwuP1_EF0v0t8uF4bDKTiY6QW2mBb-R74awO2BbwBrZLduwiLPD8d71L587SLGntWun0bDIqZ3_gflWfzw5BK3fh-H46PunY7_tMOAXMpEzXxudqjIZpsqGUVpwIxoIhzDnqurGqqFOA2lKqfMIetrAkLRA16U20HMqMUpHWPcJLY-gFdWAlg-OJmffeh-GjEZNAkwgfQnF21b4dIl7rDrdy3riakPfa8RBpWvNAZq6Bo3KprnGYvTrtOD4OT1r4avYb_htlZZstUbr-xVM959zsSNcQKl7qV-jlV64ztfpknsfg9fF9OaqTXeqhK6MyDm2srCijZkXTSZyLa4qA67D_7lwHn18cqskCCBOV3RLAW9j4m_nd6g36PxRzv4FDmta2VckUqlKFZWJKYyVhYEJNwJsiySglxqGJvXoQ3fCWdGWPucOHD-yvmjzQ4J4tNMPv25qfiwauMHkyrBYzf08YasyXAo92vqHfverBM716dFmR8-sFRmY3zO4R9v9b1x29uDoyk5v6ywAnpPYaPGIEZuoscKIlw2fPNgb0DJS8ev_b75NT3Gvsi8nk9M3tNJE3HGc0iYNZje3dgsQbJa_bXld0OVjX687Di4-xw
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Spatial+orientation+and+balance+control+changes+induced+by+altered+gravitoinertial+force+vectors&rft.jtitle=Experimental+brain+research&rft.au=Kaufman%2C+G+D&rft.au=Wood%2C+S+J&rft.au=Gianna%2C+C+C&rft.au=Black%2C+F+O&rft.date=2001-04-01&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.volume=137&rft.issue=3-4&rft.spage=397&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs002210000636&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon