Bayesian probability estimates are not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes’ rule in elementary situations
This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes' rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that using Bayes' rule is not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes' rule. Simpler methods, even fallacious heuristics, might p...
Saved in:
Published in | Frontiers in psychology Vol. 6; p. 1194 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Switzerland
Frontiers Media S.A
17.08.2015
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1664-1078 1664-1078 |
DOI | 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01194 |
Cover
Abstract | This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes' rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that using Bayes' rule is not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes' rule. Simpler methods, even fallacious heuristics, might prescribe correct choices reasonably often under specific circumstances. We considered elementary situations with binary sets of hypotheses and data. We adopted an ecological approach and prepared two-stage computer tasks resembling natural sampling. Probabilistic relations were inferred from a set of pictures, followed by a choice which was made to maximize the chance of a preferred outcome. Use of Bayes' rule was deduced indirectly from choices. Study 1 used a stratified sample of N = 60 participants equally distributed with regard to gender and type of education (humanities vs. pure sciences). Choices satisfying Bayes' rule were dominant. To investigate ways of making choices more directly, we replicated Study 1, adding a task with a verbal report. In Study 2 (N = 76) choices conforming to Bayes' rule dominated again. However, the verbal reports revealed use of a new, non-inverse rule, which always renders correct choices, but is easier than Bayes' rule to apply. It does not require inversion of conditions [transforming P(H) and P(D|H) into P(H|D)] when computing chances. Study 3 examined the efficiency of three fallacious heuristics (pre-Bayesian, representativeness, and evidence-only) in producing choices concordant with Bayes' rule. Computer-simulated scenarios revealed that the heuristics produced correct choices reasonably often under specific base rates and likelihood ratios. Summing up we conclude that natural sampling results in most choices conforming to Bayes' rule. However, people tend to replace Bayes' rule with simpler methods, and even use of fallacious heuristics may be satisfactorily efficient. |
---|---|
AbstractList | This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes' rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that using Bayes' rule is not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes' rule. Simpler methods, even fallacious heuristics, might prescribe correct choices reasonably often under specific circumstances. We considered elementary situations with binary sets of hypotheses and data. We adopted an ecological approach and prepared two-stage computer tasks resembling natural sampling. Probabilistic relations were inferred from a set of pictures, followed by a choice which was made to maximize the chance of a preferred outcome. Use of Bayes' rule was deduced indirectly from choices. Study 1 used a stratified sample of N = 60 participants equally distributed with regard to gender and type of education (humanities vs. pure sciences). Choices satisfying Bayes' rule were dominant. To investigate ways of making choices more directly, we replicated Study 1, adding a task with a verbal report. In Study 2 (N = 76) choices conforming to Bayes' rule dominated again. However, the verbal reports revealed use of a new, non-inverse rule, which always renders correct choices, but is easier than Bayes' rule to apply. It does not require inversion of conditions [transforming P(H) and P(D|H) into P(H|D)] when computing chances. Study 3 examined the efficiency of three fallacious heuristics (pre-Bayesian, representativeness, and evidence-only) in producing choices concordant with Bayes' rule. Computer-simulated scenarios revealed that the heuristics produced correct choices reasonably often under specific base rates and likelihood ratios. Summing up we conclude that natural sampling results in most choices conforming to Bayes' rule. However, people tend to replace Bayes' rule with simpler methods, and even use of fallacious heuristics may be satisfactorily efficient.This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes' rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that using Bayes' rule is not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes' rule. Simpler methods, even fallacious heuristics, might prescribe correct choices reasonably often under specific circumstances. We considered elementary situations with binary sets of hypotheses and data. We adopted an ecological approach and prepared two-stage computer tasks resembling natural sampling. Probabilistic relations were inferred from a set of pictures, followed by a choice which was made to maximize the chance of a preferred outcome. Use of Bayes' rule was deduced indirectly from choices. Study 1 used a stratified sample of N = 60 participants equally distributed with regard to gender and type of education (humanities vs. pure sciences). Choices satisfying Bayes' rule were dominant. To investigate ways of making choices more directly, we replicated Study 1, adding a task with a verbal report. In Study 2 (N = 76) choices conforming to Bayes' rule dominated again. However, the verbal reports revealed use of a new, non-inverse rule, which always renders correct choices, but is easier than Bayes' rule to apply. It does not require inversion of conditions [transforming P(H) and P(D|H) into P(H|D)] when computing chances. Study 3 examined the efficiency of three fallacious heuristics (pre-Bayesian, representativeness, and evidence-only) in producing choices concordant with Bayes' rule. Computer-simulated scenarios revealed that the heuristics produced correct choices reasonably often under specific base rates and likelihood ratios. Summing up we conclude that natural sampling results in most choices conforming to Bayes' rule. However, people tend to replace Bayes' rule with simpler methods, and even use of fallacious heuristics may be satisfactorily efficient. This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes’ rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that using Bayes’ rule is not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes’ rule. Simpler methods, even fallacious heuristics, might prescribe correct choices reasonably often under specific circumstances. We considered elementary situations with binary sets of hypotheses and data. We adopted an ecological approach and prepared two-stage computer tasks resembling natural sampling. Probabilistic relations were inferred from a set of pictures, followed by a choice which was made to maximize the chance of a preferred outcome. Use of Bayes’ rule was deduced indirectly from choices. Study 1 used a stratified sample of N = 60 participants equally distributed with regard to gender and type of education (humanities vs. pure sciences). Choices satisfying Bayes’ rule were dominant. To investigate ways of making choices more directly, we replicated Study 1, adding a task with a verbal report. In Study 2 ( N = 76) choices conforming to Bayes’ rule dominated again. However, the verbal reports revealed use of a new, non-inverse rule, which always renders correct choices, but is easier than Bayes’ rule to apply. It does not require inversion of conditions [transforming P(H) and P(D|H) into P(H|D)] when computing chances. Study 3 examined the efficiency of three fallacious heuristics (pre-Bayesian, representativeness, and evidence-only) in producing choices concordant with Bayes’ rule. Computer-simulated scenarios revealed that the heuristics produced correct choices reasonably often under specific base rates and likelihood ratios. Summing up we conclude that natural sampling results in most choices conforming to Bayes’ rule. However, people tend to replace Bayes’ rule with simpler methods, and even use of fallacious heuristics may be satisfactorily efficient. This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes’ rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that using Bayes’ rule is not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes’ rule. Simpler methods, even fallacious heuristics, might prescribe correct choices reasonably often under specific circumstances. We considered elementary situations with binary sets of hypotheses and data. We adopted an ecological approach and prepared two-stage computer tasks resembling natural sampling. Probabilistic relations were to be inferred from a set of pictures, followed by a choice between the data which was made to maximize a chance for a preferred outcome. Using Bayes’ rule was deduced indirectly from choices.Study 1 (N=60) followed a 2 (gender: female vs. male) x 2 (education: humanities vs. pure sciences) between-subjects factorial design with balanced cells, and a number of correct choices as a dependent variable. Choices satisfying Bayes’ rule were dominant. To investigate ways of making choices more directly, we replicated Study 1, adding a task with a verbal report. In Study 2 (N=76) choices conforming to Bayes’ rule dominated again. However, the verbal reports revealed use of a new, non-inverse rule, which always renders correct choices, but is easier than Bayes’ rule to apply. It does not require inversing conditions (transforming P(H) and P(D|H) into P(H|D)) when computing chances). Study 3 examined efficiency of the three fallacious heuristics (pre-Bayesian, representativeness, and evidence-only) in producing choices concordant with Bayes’ rule. Computer-simulated scenarios revealed that the heuristics produce correct choices reasonably often under specific base rates and likelihood ratios. Summing up we conclude that natural sampling leads to most choices conforming to Bayes’ rule. However, people tend to replace Bayes’ rule with simpler methods, and even use of fallacious heuristics may be satisfactorily efficient. This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes' rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that using Bayes' rule is not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes' rule. Simpler methods, even fallacious heuristics, might prescribe correct choices reasonably often under specific circumstances. We considered elementary situations with binary sets of hypotheses and data. We adopted an ecological approach and prepared two-stage computer tasks resembling natural sampling. Probabilistic relations were inferred from a set of pictures, followed by a choice which was made to maximize the chance of a preferred outcome. Use of Bayes' rule was deduced indirectly from choices. Study 1 used a stratified sample of N = 60 participants equally distributed with regard to gender and type of education (humanities vs. pure sciences). Choices satisfying Bayes' rule were dominant. To investigate ways of making choices more directly, we replicated Study 1, adding a task with a verbal report. In Study 2 (N = 76) choices conforming to Bayes' rule dominated again. However, the verbal reports revealed use of a new, non-inverse rule, which always renders correct choices, but is easier than Bayes' rule to apply. It does not require inversion of conditions [transforming P(H) and P(D|H) into P(H|D)] when computing chances. Study 3 examined the efficiency of three fallacious heuristics (pre-Bayesian, representativeness, and evidence-only) in producing choices concordant with Bayes' rule. Computer-simulated scenarios revealed that the heuristics produced correct choices reasonably often under specific base rates and likelihood ratios. Summing up we conclude that natural sampling results in most choices conforming to Bayes' rule. However, people tend to replace Bayes' rule with simpler methods, and even use of fallacious heuristics may be satisfactorily efficient. |
Author | Borzymowska, Zuzanna Idzikowska, Katarzyna Kowalczuk, Olga Nowak-Przygodzka, Marta Domurat, Artur |
AuthorAffiliation | 4 Laboratory of Visual System, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology Warsaw, Poland 3 Centre for Economic Psychology and Decision Sciences, Kozminski University Warsaw, Poland 1 Department of Cognitive Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw Warsaw, Poland 2 Center for Complex Systems and New Technologies, The Robert B. Zajonc Institute for Social Studies, University of Warsaw Warsaw, Poland |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 4 Laboratory of Visual System, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology Warsaw, Poland – name: 2 Center for Complex Systems and New Technologies, The Robert B. Zajonc Institute for Social Studies, University of Warsaw Warsaw, Poland – name: 3 Centre for Economic Psychology and Decision Sciences, Kozminski University Warsaw, Poland – name: 1 Department of Cognitive Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw Warsaw, Poland |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Artur surname: Domurat fullname: Domurat, Artur – sequence: 2 givenname: Olga surname: Kowalczuk fullname: Kowalczuk, Olga – sequence: 3 givenname: Katarzyna surname: Idzikowska fullname: Idzikowska, Katarzyna – sequence: 4 givenname: Zuzanna surname: Borzymowska fullname: Borzymowska, Zuzanna – sequence: 5 givenname: Marta surname: Nowak-Przygodzka fullname: Nowak-Przygodzka, Marta |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26347676$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp1Ustu1DAUjVARLaV7VshLNjPYjmPHGySoKFSqxAbW1o3nZuqS2IPtVMqO3-D3-JJ6MqVqkfDG1vU5577Oy-rIB49V9ZrRdV23-l2_S_N2zSlr1pQxLZ5VJ0xKsWJUtUeP3sfVWUo3tBxBOaX8RXXMZS2UVPKkih9hxuTAk10MHXRucHkmmLIbIWMiEJH4kIlHiylBnEkOZIQfSOx1cCVGEmSX-tn5LVm0_vz6TeI0IHGe4IAj-rynJZenggw-vaqe9zAkPLu_T6vvF5--nX9ZXX39fHn-4WplheR51bWy7uuWKyGUQkY1MA2CbRAkdppR2_XYcAZUo4JGtFY1taAbygRXveh1fVpdHnQ3AW7MLpaO4mwCOLMEQtwaiNnZAQ2ztgNZhoU9FR1l0KCVilutNeuQYdF6f9DaTd2IG1uaijA8EX3649212YZbI5rSgqBF4O29QAw_pzJfM7pkcRjAY5iSYYpR2dRcqAJ98zjXQ5K_SysAegDYGFKK2D9AGDV7a5jFGmZvDbNYo1DkPxTr8rKOUq0b_k-8A_LIwm4 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_022_02075_3 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23565 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2017_04_003 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_00863 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01872 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00664-8 10.1086/428020 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00340 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00715.x 10.1002/bdm.681 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01144 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00847.x 10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.97 10.2307/1884852 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00058.x 10.1017/CBO9780511840265 10.1080/00223980.1988.9915525 10.1037/h0034747 10.1007/s00199-012-0692-4 10.1037/h0042769 10.3758/BF03195278 10.1037/xge0000013 10.3758/s13423-013-0464-6 10.1017/CBO9780511809477.011 10.1007/978-94-010-2288-0-3 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.380 10.1007/978-1-4612-4308-3-27 10.1080/14792779143000033 10.1037/e683322011-032 10.1002/9780470752937.ch1 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00387 10.1017/S1930297500005775 10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.215 10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.3 10.1002/jhbs.21579 10.1017/S0140525X00041157 10.1080/09540129850124451 10.1016/S0001-6918(96)00028-5 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01272 10.1017/S1930297500001315 10.1037/h0060889 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00642 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.959 10.1016/S0749-5978(03)00021-9 10.1017/CBO9780511809477.019 10.1017/S0140525X07001653 10.1002/9780470752937.ch4 10.1002/acp.1460 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.003 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00133-5 10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231 10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004 10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.684 10.1037/h0023653 10.1037/0033-295x.106.2.417 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2015 Domurat, Kowalczuk, Idzikowska, Borzymowska and Nowak-Przygodzka. 2015 Domurat, Kowalczuk, Idzikowska, Borzymowska and Nowak-Przygodzka |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2015 Domurat, Kowalczuk, Idzikowska, Borzymowska and Nowak-Przygodzka. 2015 Domurat, Kowalczuk, Idzikowska, Borzymowska and Nowak-Przygodzka |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01194 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic PubMed |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1664-1078 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_1ccba6107ef04b01a5ec672c9991be1e PMC4538240 26347676 10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01194 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | 53G 5VS 9T4 AAFWJ AAKDD AAYXX ABIVO ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACXDI ADBBV ADRAZ AEGXH AFPKN AIAGR ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS BAWUL BCNDV CITATION DIK EBS EJD EMOBN F5P GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HYE IPNFZ KQ8 M48 M~E O5R O5S OK1 P2P PGMZT RIG RNS RPM NPM 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c462t-b863f38274477e109a19a41dea6eb910cbfe521a09e7a548c75340d01427f4f93 |
IEDL.DBID | M48 |
ISSN | 1664-1078 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:04:08 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 13:48:52 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 05:06:44 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:50:47 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:45:05 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:02:55 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | Bayes’ rule natural sampling heuristics ecological rationality binary hypothesis non-inverse rule choices |
Language | English |
License | This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c462t-b863f38274477e109a19a41dea6eb910cbfe521a09e7a548c75340d01427f4f93 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 This article was submitted to Cognition, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology Edited by: Gorka Navarrete, Universidad Diego Portales, Chile Reviewed by: Gaëlle Vallée-Tourangeau, Kingston University London, UK; David E. Over, Durham University, UK |
OpenAccessLink | http://journals.scholarsportal.info/openUrl.xqy?doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01194 |
PMID | 26347676 |
PQID | 1710653247 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_1ccba6107ef04b01a5ec672c9991be1e pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4538240 proquest_miscellaneous_1710653247 pubmed_primary_26347676 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01194 crossref_citationtrail_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01194 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2015-08-17 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2015-08-17 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 08 year: 2015 text: 2015-08-17 day: 17 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Switzerland |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Switzerland |
PublicationTitle | Frontiers in psychology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Front Psychol |
PublicationYear | 2015 |
Publisher | Frontiers Media S.A |
Publisher_xml | – name: Frontiers Media S.A |
References | Yamagishi (B54) 2003; 50 Gigerenzer (B19) 1995; 102 Gigerenzer (B15) 1991; 2 Brase (B7) 2015; 6 Cosmides (B10) 1996; 58 Camilleri (B9) 2009; 4 Simon (B47) 1955; 69 Gigerenzer (B20) 1998; 10 Nance (B38) 2005; 34 Sirota (B49) 2014; 21 Villejoubert (B53) 2002; 30 Tyszka (B52) 2006; 26 Karni (B31) 2013; 53 Edwards (B13) 1968 Navarrete (B39) 2014; 5 Mandel (B35) 2014; 5 Tversky (B51) 1982 Brase (B5) 2008; 23 Barbey (B3) 2007; 30 Huber (B28) 1997; 95 Pleskac (B43) 2014; 143 Kleiter (B33) 1994 Brase (B6) 1998; 127 Hertwig (B24) 2004; 15 Kahneman (B30) 1973; 80 Mellers (B37) 1999; 106 Simon (B48) 1956; 63 Sloman (B50) 2003; 91 Dhami (B11) 2004; 130 Ayal (B2) 2014; 9 Ericsson (B14) 1980; 87 Hertwig (B25) 2009; 13 Eddy (B12) 1982 Koehler (B34) 1996; 19 Girotto (B23) 2001; 78 Brunswik (B8) 1943; 50 Over (B41) 2004 Phillips (B42) 1966; 72 Hoffrage (B27) 2015; 6 Gigerenzer (B17) 2004 Gigerenzer (B16) 1998 Hertwig (B26) 2001; 24 Baron (B4) 1994 Karp (B32) 1988; 122 Gigerenzer (B18) 2008; 3 Anderson (B1) 2000 Zhu (B55) 2006; 98 Mandel (B36) 2015; 6 Giocoli (B22) 2013; 49 Kahneman (B29) 1972; 3 Rakow (B44) 2010; 14 Savage (B45) 1954 Gigerenzer (B21) 1999 Nisbett (B40) 1977; 84 Sedlmeier (B46) 2001; 130 25414688 - Front Psychol. 2014 Nov 06;5:1272 16399266 - Cognition. 2006 Jan;98(3):287-308 23794254 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2014 Feb;21(1):198-204 5968681 - J Exp Psychol. 1966 Sep;72(3):346-54 15535744 - Psychol Bull. 2004 Nov;130(6):959-88 13310708 - Psychol Rev. 1956 Mar;63(2):129-38 19836292 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Dec;13(12):517-23 3172033 - J Psychol. 1988 Jul;122(4):383-8 11561916 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001 Sep;130(3):380-400 12693194 - Exp Psychol. 2003;50(2):97-106 12035879 - Mem Cognit. 2002 Mar;30(2):171-8 17963533 - Behav Brain Sci. 2007 Jun;30(3):241-54; discussion 255-97 25873904 - Front Psychol. 2015 Mar 31;6:340 11124351 - Cognition. 2001 Mar;78(3):247-76 9625903 - AIDS Care. 1998 Apr;10(2):197-211 11682798 - Behav Brain Sci. 2001 Jun;24(3):383-403; discussion 403-51 24979239 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014 Oct;143(5):2000-19 17184402 - Risk Anal. 2006 Dec;26(6):1623-36 25346713 - Front Psychol. 2014 Oct 09;5:1144 25904882 - Front Psychol. 2015 Apr 08;6:387 26157397 - Front Psychol. 2015 Jun 22;6:642 23165740 - J Hist Behav Sci. 2013 Winter;49(1):63-95 26158666 - Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008 Jan;3(1):20-9 15270998 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Aug;15(8):534-9 |
References_xml | – volume: 58 start-page: 1 year: 1996 ident: B10 article-title: Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00664-8 – volume: 34 start-page: 395 year: 2005 ident: B38 article-title: Juror understanding of DNA evidence: an empirical assessment of presentation formats for trace evidence with a relatively small random-match probability. publication-title: J. Legal Stud. doi: 10.1086/428020 – volume: 6 issue: 340 year: 2015 ident: B7 article-title: Good fences make for good neighbors but bad science: a review of what improves Bayesian reasoning and why. publication-title: Front. Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00340 – volume: 15 start-page: 534 year: 2004 ident: B24 article-title: Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. publication-title: Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00715.x – volume: 14 start-page: 1 year: 2010 ident: B44 article-title: Degrees of uncertainty: an overview and framework for future research on experience-based choice. publication-title: J. Behav. Decis. Mak. doi: 10.1002/bdm.681 – volume: 5 issue: 1144 year: 2014 ident: B35 article-title: The psychology of Bayesian reasoning. publication-title: Front. Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01144 – volume: 26 start-page: 1623 year: 2006 ident: B52 article-title: When does information about probability count in choices under risk? publication-title: Risk Anal. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00847.x – volume: 50 start-page: 97 year: 2003 ident: B54 article-title: Facilitating normative judgments of conditional probability: frequency or nested sets? publication-title: Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.97 – year: 1954 ident: B45 publication-title: The Foundations of Statistics. – volume: 69 start-page: 99 year: 1955 ident: B47 article-title: A behavioral model of rational choice. publication-title: Q. J. Econ. doi: 10.2307/1884852 – volume: 3 start-page: 20 year: 2008 ident: B18 article-title: Why heuristics work. publication-title: Perspect. Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00058.x – year: 1994 ident: B4 publication-title: Thinking and Deciding doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511840265 – year: 1999 ident: B21 publication-title: Simple Heuristics that Make Us Smart. – volume: 122 start-page: 383 year: 1988 ident: B32 article-title: Color associations of male and female fourth-grade school children. publication-title: J. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1988.9915525 – volume: 80 start-page: 237 year: 1973 ident: B30 article-title: On the psychology of prediction. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/h0034747 – volume: 53 start-page: 335 year: 2013 ident: B31 article-title: Bayesian decision theory with action-dependent probabilities and risk attitudes. publication-title: Econ. Theory doi: 10.1007/s00199-012-0692-4 – volume: 63 start-page: 129 year: 1956 ident: B48 article-title: Rational choice and the structure of environment. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/h0042769 – volume: 30 start-page: 171 year: 2002 ident: B53 article-title: The inverse fallacy: an account of deviations from Bayes theorem and the additivity principle. publication-title: Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.3758/BF03195278 – volume: 143 start-page: 2000 year: 2014 ident: B43 article-title: Ecologically rational choice and the structure of the environment. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/xge0000013 – volume: 21 start-page: 198 year: 2014 ident: B49 article-title: Ecological rationality or nested sets? Individual differences in cognitive processing predict Bayesian reasoning. publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0464-6 – start-page: 153 year: 1982 ident: B51 article-title: “Evidential impact of base rates,” in publication-title: Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511809477.011 – volume: 3 start-page: 430 year: 1972 ident: B29 article-title: Subjective probability: a judgment of representativeness. publication-title: Cogn. Psychol. doi: 10.1007/978-94-010-2288-0-3 – volume: 130 start-page: 380 year: 2001 ident: B46 article-title: Teaching Bayesian reasoning in less than two hours. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.380 – start-page: 375 year: 1994 ident: B33 article-title: “Natural sampling: rationality without base rates,” in publication-title: Contributions to Mathematical Psychology, Psychometrics, and Methodology doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-4308-3-27 – volume: 2 start-page: 83 year: 1991 ident: B15 article-title: How to make cognitive illusions disappear: beyond heuristics and biases. publication-title: Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/14792779143000033 – volume: 24 start-page: 383 year: 2001 ident: B26 article-title: Experimental practices in economics: a methodological challenge for psychologists? publication-title: Behav. Brain Sci. doi: 10.1037/e683322011-032 – start-page: 3 year: 2004 ident: B41 article-title: “Rationality and the normative/descriptive distinction,” in publication-title: Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making doi: 10.1002/9780470752937.ch1 – volume: 6 issue: 387 year: 2015 ident: B36 article-title: Instruction in information structuring improves Bayesian judgment in intelligence analysts. publication-title: Front. Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00387 – volume: 9 start-page: 226 year: 2014 ident: B2 article-title: The effects of mental steps and compatibility on Bayesian reasoning. publication-title: Judgm. Decis. Mak. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500005775 – volume: 87 start-page: 215 year: 1980 ident: B14 article-title: Verbal reports as data. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.215 – volume: 127 start-page: 3 year: 1998 ident: B6 article-title: Individuation, counting, and statistical inference: the role of frequency and whole-object representations in judgment under uncertainty. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.3 – volume: 49 start-page: 63 year: 2013 ident: B22 article-title: From wald to savage: homo economicus becomes a Bayesian statistician. publication-title: J. Hist. Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1002/jhbs.21579 – start-page: 9 year: 1998 ident: B16 article-title: “Ecological intelligence. An adaptation for frequencies,” in publication-title: The Evolution of Mind – volume: 19 start-page: 1 year: 1996 ident: B34 article-title: The base rate fallacy reconsidered: descriptive, normative, and methodological challenges. publication-title: Behav. Brain Sci. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00041157 – volume: 10 start-page: 197 year: 1998 ident: B20 article-title: AIDS counseling for low-risk clients. publication-title: AIDS Care doi: 10.1080/09540129850124451 – volume: 95 start-page: 15 year: 1997 ident: B28 article-title: Active information search and complete information presentation in naturalistic risky decision tasks. publication-title: Acta Psychol. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(96)00028-5 – volume: 5 issue: 1272 year: 2014 ident: B39 article-title: Communicating risk in prenatal screening: the consequences of Bayesian misapprehension. publication-title: Front. Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01272 – volume: 4 start-page: 518 year: 2009 ident: B9 article-title: The role of representation in experience-based choice. publication-title: Judgm. Decis. Mak. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500001315 – volume: 50 start-page: 255 year: 1943 ident: B8 article-title: Organismic achievement and environmental probability. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/h0060889 – volume: 6 issue: 642 year: 2015 ident: B27 article-title: Natural frequencies facilitate diagnostic inferences of managers. publication-title: Front. Psychol. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00642 – volume: 130 start-page: 959 year: 2004 ident: B11 article-title: The role of representative design in an ecological approach to cognition. publication-title: Psychol. Bull. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.959 – volume: 91 start-page: 296 year: 2003 ident: B50 article-title: Frequency illusions and other fallacies. publication-title: Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. doi: 10.1016/S0749-5978(03)00021-9 – start-page: 249 year: 1982 ident: B12 article-title: “Probabilistic reasoning in clinical medicine: problems and opportunities,” in publication-title: Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511809477.019 – year: 2000 ident: B1 publication-title: Learning and Memory: An Integrated Approach – volume: 30 start-page: 241 year: 2007 ident: B3 article-title: Base-rate respect: from ecological rationality to dual processes. publication-title: Behav. Brain Sci. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001653 – start-page: 62 year: 2004 ident: B17 article-title: “Fast and frugal heuristics: the tools of bounded rationality,” in publication-title: Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making doi: 10.1002/9780470752937.ch4 – volume: 23 start-page: 369 year: 2008 ident: B5 article-title: Pictorial representations in statistical reasoning. publication-title: Appl. Cogn. Psychol. doi: 10.1002/acp.1460 – volume: 98 start-page: 287 year: 2006 ident: B55 article-title: Children can solve Bayesian problems: the role of representation in mental computation. publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.003 – volume: 78 start-page: 247 year: 2001 ident: B23 article-title: Solving probabilistic and statistical problems: a matter of information structure and question form. publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00133-5 – start-page: 17 year: 1968 ident: B13 article-title: “Conservatism in human information processing,” in publication-title: Formal Representation of Human Judgment – volume: 84 start-page: 231 year: 1977 ident: B40 article-title: Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231 – volume: 13 start-page: 517 year: 2009 ident: B25 article-title: The description–experience gap in risky choice. publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004 – volume: 102 start-page: 684 year: 1995 ident: B19 article-title: How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: frequency formats. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.684 – volume: 72 start-page: 346 year: 1966 ident: B42 article-title: Conservatism in a simple probability inference task. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/h0023653 – volume: 106 start-page: 417 year: 1999 ident: B37 article-title: How to improve Bayesian reasoning: comment on Gigerenzer and Hoffrage (1995). publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.2.417 – reference: 24979239 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014 Oct;143(5):2000-19 – reference: 12693194 - Exp Psychol. 2003;50(2):97-106 – reference: 9625903 - AIDS Care. 1998 Apr;10(2):197-211 – reference: 13310708 - Psychol Rev. 1956 Mar;63(2):129-38 – reference: 17963533 - Behav Brain Sci. 2007 Jun;30(3):241-54; discussion 255-97 – reference: 12035879 - Mem Cognit. 2002 Mar;30(2):171-8 – reference: 25346713 - Front Psychol. 2014 Oct 09;5:1144 – reference: 3172033 - J Psychol. 1988 Jul;122(4):383-8 – reference: 19836292 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Dec;13(12):517-23 – reference: 25904882 - Front Psychol. 2015 Apr 08;6:387 – reference: 11561916 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001 Sep;130(3):380-400 – reference: 23794254 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2014 Feb;21(1):198-204 – reference: 11124351 - Cognition. 2001 Mar;78(3):247-76 – reference: 23165740 - J Hist Behav Sci. 2013 Winter;49(1):63-95 – reference: 15270998 - Psychol Sci. 2004 Aug;15(8):534-9 – reference: 16399266 - Cognition. 2006 Jan;98(3):287-308 – reference: 25414688 - Front Psychol. 2014 Nov 06;5:1272 – reference: 26157397 - Front Psychol. 2015 Jun 22;6:642 – reference: 15535744 - Psychol Bull. 2004 Nov;130(6):959-88 – reference: 11682798 - Behav Brain Sci. 2001 Jun;24(3):383-403; discussion 403-51 – reference: 25873904 - Front Psychol. 2015 Mar 31;6:340 – reference: 17184402 - Risk Anal. 2006 Dec;26(6):1623-36 – reference: 5968681 - J Exp Psychol. 1966 Sep;72(3):346-54 – reference: 26158666 - Perspect Psychol Sci. 2008 Jan;3(1):20-9 |
SSID | ssj0000402002 |
Score | 2.1178184 |
Snippet | This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes' rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that... This paper has two aims. First, we investigate how often people make choices conforming to Bayes’ rule when natural sampling is applied. Second, we show that... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 1194 |
SubjectTerms | Bayesian inference Bayes’ rule Binary hypothesis choices ecological rationality Heuristics Psychology |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV3NbtQwELaqnnpB5a-EAjJSLz1Eaydee3OkiKpCghOVerNsZ0xXtNkqyR5y62v09XgSZpx0tYsQXLgmtmN57Jlv4plvGDsRoRRF5VReq4XIVSx97jVIVIYQyqKWwRnKRv7yVV9cqs9X86utUl8UEzbSA48LN5MheIc23kAUygvp5hC0KQIBGw8SSPuKSmw5U0kHk1tEoTt0L4leWDWLd93wnUK5iKZTVmrHDiW6_j9hzN9DJbdsz_khezKBRv5hnOxTtgfNM3aw0V3Dc9aeuQEoH5JThZiRe3vgxKBxS2CSuxZ4s-p5A5QW4NqB9yt-634AxyFIV_AupTlQ0hNPY_28f-Dt-gb4suEwxZhjt27Zj-Tg3Qt2ef7p28eLfCqnkAeliz73C13GckGUgMaAFJWTKCRZg9PgETUEHwGNuRMVGIeOTEBPRokafajCRBWr8iXbb1YNvGJc1qVyCoTGTlRM3ckihIid4iJKEPOMzR4X14aJa5xKXtxY9DlIHDaJw5I4bBJHxk43Pe5Gno2_tD0jeW3aEUN2eoD7xk77xv5r32Ts_aO0LZ4ouiZxDazWnZUIuvQcgabJ2NEo_c2nCl0qo43OmNnZFztz2X3TLK8Ta7dC04Lw6fX_mPwxO6DloH_b0rxh-327hrcIjnr_Lp2DXxLyEn0 priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals |
Title | Bayesian probability estimates are not necessary to make choices satisfying Bayes’ rule in elementary situations |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26347676 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1710653247 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4538240 https://doaj.org/article/1ccba6107ef04b01a5ec672c9991be1e |
Volume | 6 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Nb9QwELWgXHpB5TstrYyEhDiE2onXzh5QRRFVhVROrNRb5DjjsmKbLElWav49M0526aKFaxI7H2PPvOd43jD2VrhUJFOr4lJlIlY-LeJCg0RnCC5NSumsoWzkq2_6cqa-Xk-u_6RHjx-w3UntqJ7UrFl8uPvVn-GE_0iME-PtqV-2_Q3t0iIFTiTlD9kjjEuaqNjVCPaDXyaqNGxC1Fqh_zHZ8N9yZyekEqxTZTSpkdwLWUHZfxcc_XtX5b0wdXHAHo_4kn8aBsQT9gCqp2x_4-b6Z2x5bnug1ElOxWQGme6ek9jGLeFObhvgVd3xCiiDwDY972p-a38Cxy7IrfA2ZERQfhQPfb3jzWoBfF5xGDejY6N23g0q4u1zNrv48v3zZTzWXYid0kkXF5lOfZqRdqAxIMXUSrSmLMFqKBBeuMIDRn0rpmAsMh6HlEeJEslWYrzy0_QF26vqCl4xLstUWQVCYyOqum5l4pzHRj7zEsQkYqfrT5u7UZScamMsciQnZJc82CUnu-TBLhF7v2mxHAQ5_nPtOVlrcx1JaYcDdXOTjzMzl84VFkGkAS9UIaSdgNMmcYScC5AQsTdrW-c49eh_iq2gXrW5RHSmJ4hITcReDrbf3Go9diJmtkbF1rNsn6nmP4K8t8IYhDjr8J99HrF9ekda2ZbmNdvrmhUcIzTqipOwpHASxv1vnP8N3g |
linkProvider | Scholars Portal |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Bayesian+probability+estimates+are+not+necessary+to+make+choices+satisfying+Bayes%27+rule+in+elementary+situations&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+psychology&rft.au=Domurat%2C+Artur&rft.au=Kowalczuk%2C+Olga&rft.au=Idzikowska%2C+Katarzyna&rft.au=Borzymowska%2C+Zuzanna&rft.date=2015-08-17&rft.issn=1664-1078&rft.eissn=1664-1078&rft.volume=6&rft.spage=1194&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2015.01194&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F26347676&rft.externalDocID=26347676 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |