Scratching under positive and negative arousal in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)

Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anx...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inAmerican journal of primatology Vol. 78; no. 2; pp. 216 - 226
Main Authors Neal, Sarah J., Caine, Nancy G.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Blackwell Publishing Ltd 01.02.2016
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor‐housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post‐induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post‐induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety–scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non‐human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species. Am. J. Primatol. 78:216–226, 2016. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
AbstractList Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor-housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post-induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post-induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety-scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non-human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species.
Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor‐housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post‐induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post‐induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety–scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non‐human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species. Am. J. Primatol. 78:216–226, 2016. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor-housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post-induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post-induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety-scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non-human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species.Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor-housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post-induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post-induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety-scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non-human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species.
Author Caine, Nancy G.
Neal, Sarah J.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Sarah J.
  surname: Neal
  fullname: Neal, Sarah J.
  email: Correspondence to: Sarah J. Neal, University of Wisconsin, Madison, 1202 W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706. , sneal2@wisc.edu
  organization: California State University San Marcos, California, San Marcos
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Nancy G.
  surname: Caine
  fullname: Caine, Nancy G.
  organization: California State University San Marcos, California, San Marcos
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26530306$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kU9P3DAQxa2KChbKoV-gstQLHAJjO7aTI1oVaEFtpaWiN8txHNbbxN7aCX--fU0XOCC1p9FIv_c0894u2vLBW4TeEzgiAPRYr9ZHlJZ19QbNCNRVQVnJt9AMqOQF5YLvoN2UVgCElIJvox0qOAMGYoauFybq0Sydv8GTb23E65Dc6G4t1r7F3t7ozRLDlHSPnccmDEPweNBxCMmOCR_Mdd-7cRndPV5pY5ZTOnyH3na6T3b_ae6hH6efrubnxeW3s8_zk8vClIJURW0JFbI1LTS1YaUACR2jjJmWV5oQ0xjStUA0aSRvZK0ry2tWd41uDadQcraHDja-6xh-TzaNanDJ2L7X3uaLFZECKgG8phn9-ApdhSn6fF2muJSUEvZo-OGJmprBtmodXf70QT1HloHDDWBiSCna7gUhoB7rULkO9beOzB6_Yo0bc6DBj1G7_n-KO9fbh39bq5Mv358VxUbh0mjvXxQ6_lJCMsnV9dczdbUQcvETLtQF-wPJgqmN
CODEN AJPTDU
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23138
crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23677
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2017_12_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2019_03_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2017_06_001
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0250913
crossref_primary_10_3390_ani12213002
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_39214_2
crossref_primary_10_3390_jzbg2020012
crossref_primary_10_7120_09627286_27_1_081
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_025_92251_x
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_10754_8
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2021_0484
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2024_105950
Cites_doi 10.1002/ajp.1350130405
10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00349-8
10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00065-X
10.1038/sj.mp.4002135
10.3109/10253890.2010.505272
10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1997)23:5<315::AID-AB2>3.0.CO;2-H
10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.018
10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.007
10.1159/000156561
10.1896/044.015.0209
10.1007/s10071-007-0125-0
10.1002/ajp.20290
10.1002/ajp.22167
10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00153-9
10.1007/BF02381606
10.1016/0163-1047(91)90469-7
10.1097/PSY.0b013e318244fbde
10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.047
10.1002/ajp.22363
10.1159/000078307
10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.016
10.1007/BF02629625
10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00527.x
10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00058.x
10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.008
10.1006/hbeh.1998.1469
10.1016/j.beproc.2012.04.009
10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.011
10.1097/00006842-198108000-00007
10.1163/156853997X00386
10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.20
10.1007/s10329-010-0228-0
10.1016/0031-9384(89)90089-9
10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007
10.3377/004.047.0111
10.1002/ajp.1034
10.1146/annurev.an.09.100180.002351
10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00376-0
10.1002/ajp.1350190105
10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_07
10.1163/1568539042360189
10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80592-5
10.1080/0300443941020107
10.1086/393866
10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199807)33:1<21::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-U
10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.016
10.1111/j.1439-0310.1976.tb00922.x
10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00174-5
10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.012
10.1163/156853990X00518
10.1002/ajpa.1330330212
10.1016/j.pbb.2007.02.016
10.1016/0091-3057(91)90258-4
10.1098/rsbl.2009.0513
10.1007/s00726-006-0335-9
10.1002/ajp.22057
10.1002/(SICI)1522-7154(1996)2:4<186::AID-ANXI5>3.0.CO;2-M
10.1016/S0196-9781(02)00020-7
10.1007/s10329-012-0294-6
10.1111/eth.12008
10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00037-2
10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00453.x
10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.06.003
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03987.x
10.1097/00006842-199905000-00011
10.1111/eth.12119
10.1002/ajp.1350110204
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Copyright_xml – notice: 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
– notice: 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
DBID BSCLL
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7SN
7TK
8BJ
8FD
C1K
FQK
FR3
JBE
K9.
P64
RC3
7X8
DOI 10.1002/ajp.22498
DatabaseName Istex
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
Technology Research Database
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
Engineering Research Database
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Genetics Abstracts
International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)
Technology Research Database
Animal Behavior Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Engineering Research Database
Ecology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE

Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anthropology
Zoology
Psychology
EISSN 1098-2345
EndPage 226
ExternalDocumentID 3924352671
26530306
10_1002_ajp_22498
AJP22498
ark_67375_WNG_TS67SX0K_K
Genre article
Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
.3N
.GA
.Y3
05W
0R~
10A
1L6
1OB
1OC
1ZS
23M
31~
33P
36B
3SF
3WU
4.4
50Y
50Z
51W
51X
52M
52N
52O
52P
52S
52T
52U
52W
52X
53G
5GY
5RE
5VS
66C
702
7PT
8-0
8-1
8-3
8-4
8-5
8UM
930
A03
AAESR
AAEVG
AAHHS
AANLZ
AAONW
AASGY
AAXRX
AAZKR
ABCQN
ABCUV
ABEML
ABIJN
ABIVO
ABPVW
ACAHQ
ACBWZ
ACCFJ
ACCZN
ACFBH
ACGFS
ACPOU
ACPRK
ACSCC
ACXBN
ACXQS
ADBBV
ADEOM
ADIYS
ADIZJ
ADKYN
ADMGS
ADOZA
ADXAS
ADZMN
ADZOD
AEEZP
AEIGN
AEIMD
AENEX
AEQDE
AEUQT
AEUYR
AFBPY
AFFPM
AFGKR
AFPWT
AFRAH
AFZJQ
AHBTC
AHMBA
AITYG
AIURR
AIWBW
AJBDE
AJXKR
ALAGY
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALUQN
AMBMR
AMYDB
ASPBG
ATUGU
AUFTA
AVWKF
AZBYB
AZFZN
AZVAB
BAFTC
BDRZF
BFHJK
BHBCM
BMNLL
BMXJE
BNHUX
BROTX
BRXPI
BSCLL
BY8
CS3
D-E
D-F
DCZOG
DPXWK
DR2
DRFUL
DRSTM
DVXWH
EBS
ECGQY
EJD
F00
F01
F04
F5P
FEDTE
G-S
G.N
GNP
GODZA
H.T
H.X
HBH
HF~
HGLYW
HHY
HHZ
HVGLF
HZ~
IX1
J0M
JPC
KQQ
LATKE
LAW
LC2
LC3
LEEKS
LH4
LITHE
LOXES
LP6
LP7
LUTES
LW6
LYRES
M66
MEWTI
MK4
MRFUL
MRSTM
MSFUL
MSSTM
MXFUL
MXSTM
N04
N05
N9A
NF~
NNB
O66
O9-
OIG
P2P
P2W
P2X
P4D
PALCI
PQQKQ
Q.N
Q11
QB0
QRW
R.K
RIWAO
RJQFR
ROL
RWI
RWV
RX1
RYL
SAMSI
SUPJJ
TN5
UB1
V2E
VQA
W8V
W99
WBKPD
WIB
WIH
WIK
WJL
WNSPC
WOHZO
WQJ
WRC
WTM
WXSBR
WYISQ
XG1
XV2
ZCG
ZZTAW
~02
~IA
~WT
AAHQN
AAMNL
AANHP
AAYCA
ACRPL
ACYXJ
ADNMO
AFWVQ
ALVPJ
AAYXX
AEYWJ
AGHNM
AGQPQ
AGYGG
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7SN
7TK
8BJ
8FD
AAMMB
AEFGJ
AGXDD
AIDQK
AIDYY
C1K
FQK
FR3
JBE
K9.
P64
RC3
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c4618-9e1267dcd0b9c346070f3233cd58a11cbc1fd01a1b75b79a8e5939fbadc520453
IEDL.DBID DR2
ISSN 0275-2565
1098-2345
IngestDate Fri Jul 11 09:38:32 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 25 10:30:33 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 07:05:56 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:08:36 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 02:53:16 EDT 2025
Wed Jan 22 16:37:00 EST 2025
Wed Oct 30 10:05:32 EDT 2024
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 2
Keywords anxiety
callitrichid
excitement
scratching
arousal
Language English
License http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor
2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4618-9e1267dcd0b9c346070f3233cd58a11cbc1fd01a1b75b79a8e5939fbadc520453
Notes ArticleID:AJP22498
ark:/67375/WNG-TS67SX0K-K
istex:08A439C206E7A6C9ADE800A114A34CA4DD6424E4
ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
PMID 26530306
PQID 1757722135
PQPubID 866340
PageCount 11
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_1760860592
proquest_journals_1757722135
pubmed_primary_26530306
crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_22498
crossref_citationtrail_10_1002_ajp_22498
wiley_primary_10_1002_ajp_22498_AJP22498
istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_TS67SX0K_K
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate February 2016
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2016-02-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 02
  year: 2016
  text: February 2016
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: New York
PublicationTitle American journal of primatology
PublicationTitleAlternate Am. J. Primatol.
PublicationYear 2016
Publisher Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
Publisher_xml – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd
– name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
References Braesicke K, Parkinson JA, Reekir Y, et al. 2005. Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis. European Journal of Neuroscience 21:1733-1740.
Schlafani A, Norscia I, Antonacci D, Palagi E. 2012. Scratching around mating: factors affecting anxiety in wild Lemur catta. Primates 53:247-254.
Mellou E. 2006. Play theories: a contemporary review. Early Child Development and Care 102:91-100.
Polizzi di Sorrentino E, Schino G, Tiddi B, Aureli F. 2012. Scratching as a window into the emotional responses of wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Ethology 118:1072-1084.
Buckley V, Semple S. 2012. Evidence that displacement activities facilitate behavioural transitions in ring-tailed lemurs. Behavioural Processes 90:433-435.
Omedes A. 1981. Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets Callithrix a. argentata and Callithrix a. melanura. Miscellania Zoologica 7:193-211.
Castles DL, Whiten A. 1998. Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. II. Stress and self-directed behaviour. Ethology 104:148-160.
Barros M, Giorgetti M, Souto AAV, et al. 2007. Persistent anxiety-like behavior in marmosets following a recent predatory stress condition: reversal by diazepam. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 84:705-711.
Maestripieri D. 1993. Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) I. Measurement of anxiety and identification of anxiety-eliciting situations. Ethology 95:19-31.
Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Schino G. 1991. Conflict, displacement activities, and menstrual cycle in long-tailed macaques. Primates 32:115-118.
Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2002. Anxiolytic-like effects of Substance P fragment (SP1-7) in non-human primates (Callithrix jacchus). Peptides 23:967-973.
Aureli F, van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM. 1989. Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology 19:39-51.
Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M. 1989. On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiology & Behavior 45:667-670.
Aureli F. 1997. Post-conflict anxiety in non-human primates: the mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution. Aggressive Behavior 23:315-328.
Judge PG, Griffaton NS, Fincke AM. 2006. Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) during crowding: a tension-reduction strategy. American Journal of Primatology 68:993-1006.
Judge PG, Evans DW, Schroepfer KK, Gross AC. 2011. Perseveration on a reversal-learning task correlates with rates of self-directed behavior in nonhuman primates. Behavioural Brain Research 222:57-65.
Nakayama K. 2004. Observing conspecifics scratching induces a contagion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Journal of Comparative Psychology 118:20-24.
Judge PG, Bachman KA. 2013. Witnessing reconciliation reduces arousal of bystanders in a baboon group (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). Animal Behaviour 85:881-889.
Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD, Hyatt CW. 2001. Effects of cognitive challenge on self-directed behaviors by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 55:1-14.
Waldstein SR, Kop WJ, Schmidt LA, et al. 2000. Frontal electrocortical and cardiovascular reactivity during happiness and anger. Biological Psychology 55:3-23.
Smith TE, McGreer-Whitworth B, French JA. 1998. Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel-cage housing in black tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Hormones and Behavior 34:211-222.
Norscia I, Palagi E. 2011. When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. Primates 52:101-104.
Ebner K, Singewald N. 2006. The role of substance P in stress and anxiety responses. Amino Acids 31:251-272.
Held SDE, Spinka M. 2011. Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81:891-899.
Kaburu SSK, MacLarnon A, Majolo B, Qarro M, Semple S. 2012. Dominance rank and self-scratching among wild female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). African Zoololgy 47:74-79.
Aureli F, Yates K. 2010. Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques. Biology Letters 6:27-29.
Cross N, Rogers LJ. 2006. Mobbing vocalizations as a coping response in the common marmoset. Hormones and Behavior 49:237-245.
Manson JH, Perry S. 2000. Correlates of self-directed behaviour in wild white-faced Capuchins. Ethology 106:301-317.
Silva IO, Alvarenga ABB, Boere V. 2008. Occasional field observations of the predation on mice, dove and ants by black-tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix penicillata). Neotropical Primates 15:59-62.
Cilia J, Piper DC. 1996. Marmoset conspecific confrontation: an ethologically-based model of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 58:85-91.
Bassett L, Buchanan-Smith HM, McKinley J, Smith T. 2003. Effects of training on stress-related behavior of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping with routine husbandry procedures. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6:221-233.
Mootnick AR, Cunningham C, Baker E. 2012. Phylogenetic separation in limb use in captive gibbons (Hylobatidae): a comparison across the primate order. American Journal of Primatology 74:1035-1043.
Waitt C, Buchanan-Smith HM. 2001. What time is feeding?: how delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump-tailed macaque behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 75:75-85.
Troisi A, Schino G, D'Antoni M, et al. 1991. Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers. Behavioral and Neural Biology 56:307-313.
Easley SP, Coelho AM Jr., Taylor LL. 1987. Scratching dominance, tension, and displacement in male baboons. American Journal of Primatology 13:397-411.
Engel SR, Creson TK, Hao Y, et al. 2009. The extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway contributed to the control of behavioral excitement. Molecular Psychiatry 14:448-461.
Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2004. Multibehavioral analysis of fear and anxiety before, during, and after experimentally induced predatory stress in Callithrix penicillata. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 78:357-367.
Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A. 1991. A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Animal Behaviour 44:967-979.
Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Freeman H, et al. 2006. Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal. Emotion 65:53-559.
Semple S, Harrison C, Lehmann J. 2013. Grooming and anxiety in Barbary macaques. Ethology 119:779-785.
Spinka M, Newberry RC, Bekoff M. 2001. Mammilian play: training for the unexpected. The Quarterly Review of Biology 76:141-168.
Barros M, Boere V, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2000. Measuring fear and anxiety in the marmoset (Callithrix penicillata) with a novel predator confrontation model: effects of diazepam. Behavioural Brain Research 108:205-211.
Dettling A, Pryce CR, Martin RD, Dobeli M. 1998. Physiological responses to parental separation and a strange situation are related to parental care received in juvenile Goeldi's monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Developmental Psychobiology 33:21-31.
Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ. 1998. Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia 36:1265-1273.
Schino G, Perretta G, Taglioni AM, Monaco V, Troisi A. 1996. Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety 2:186-191.
Baker KC, Aureli F. 1997. Behavioural indicators of anxiety: an empirical test in chimpanzees. Behaviour 134:1031-1050.
Saltzman W, Abbott DH. 2009. Effects of elevated circulating cortisol concentration on maternal behavior in common marmoset mothers (Callithrix jacchus). Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:1222-1234.
Judge PG, Mullen SH. 2005. Quadratic post-conflict affiliation among bystanders in a hamadryas baboon group. Animal Behaviour 69:1345-1355.
Schino G, Maestripieri D, Scucchi S, Turillazzi PG. 1990. Social tension in familiar and unfamiliar pairs of long-tailed macaques. Behaviour 113:264-272.
O'Neill-Wagner PL, Bolig R, Price CS. 1994. Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups?. Communication and Cognition 27:261-272.
de Boer RA, Overduin-de Vries AM, Louwerse AL, Sterck EHM. 2013. The behavioral context of visual displays in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology 75:1084-1095.
Schwartz GE, Weinberger DA, Singer JA. 1981. Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine 43:343-364.
Peignot P, Jankowski F, Anderson JR. 2004. On self-scratching in captive mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Folia Primatologica 75:160-164.
Schino G, Troisi A, Perretta G, Monaco V. 1991. Measuring anxiety in non-human primates: effect of lorazepam on macaque scratching. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 38:889-891.
Dettmer AM, Novak MA, Suomi SJ, Meyer JS. 2012. Physiological and behavioral adaptation to relocation stress in differentially reared rhesus monkeys: hair cortisol as a biomarker for anxiety-related responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:191-199.
Pearson BL, Reeder D, Judge PG. 2015. Crowding increases salivary cortisol but not self-directed behavior in captive baboons. American Journal of Primatology 77:462-467.
Diezinger F, Anderson JR. 1986. Starting from scratch: a first look at a "displacement activity" in group-living rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 11:117-124.
Parr LA, Hopkins WD. 2000. Brain temperature asymmetries and emotional perception in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Physiology & Behavior 71:363-371.
Kato Y, Gokan H, Oh-Nishi A, et al. 2014. Vocalizations associated with anxiety and fear in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Behavioural Brain Research 275:43-52.
Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD. 2004. Behavioral evidence for the cutaneous expression of emotion in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Behavior 141:979-997.
Palagi E, Norscia I. 2011. Scratching aro
1989; 45
2006; 31
1991; 56
1991; 57
2015; 77
1970; 33
2011; 52
2005; 21
1994; 27
2011; 14
1981; 43
2012; 53
2001; 42
2005; 69
2009; 14
2004; 75
1974; 40
2006; 68
2013; 119
2006; 65
1991; 44
2003; 6
2000; 55
2004; 78
1980
2001; 55
1996; 2
2010; 6
1987; 13
1991; 38
2010
1991; 32
1986; 11
2004; 141
2011; 81
1997; 23
1997; 134
2013; 85
1981; 7
2008; 15
2006
2000; 71
2008; 11
2012; 37
1999; 61
1996; 58
2014; 275
2012; 74
2014; 87
1999
2009; 34
2012; 90
1993; 95
2002; 23
2006; 49
2000; 106
2000; 108
2013; 75
1998; 104
2007; 84
2012; 47
1990; 113
2004; 118
1989; 19
2011; 222
2012; 118
1998; 34
1998; 33
1998; 36
2001; 75
2006; 102
2001; 76
e_1_2_6_51_1
e_1_2_6_74_1
e_1_2_6_53_1
e_1_2_6_32_1
e_1_2_6_70_1
e_1_2_6_30_1
e_1_2_6_72_1
O'Neill‐Wagner PL (e_1_2_6_55_1) 1994; 27
e_1_2_6_19_1
e_1_2_6_36_1
e_1_2_6_59_1
e_1_2_6_11_1
e_1_2_6_17_1
e_1_2_6_15_1
e_1_2_6_38_1
e_1_2_6_57_1
e_1_2_6_62_1
e_1_2_6_64_1
e_1_2_6_43_1
e_1_2_6_20_1
e_1_2_6_41_1
e_1_2_6_60_1
e_1_2_6_9_1
e_1_2_6_5_1
e_1_2_6_7_1
e_1_2_6_24_1
e_1_2_6_49_1
e_1_2_6_3_1
e_1_2_6_22_1
e_1_2_6_66_1
e_1_2_6_28_1
e_1_2_6_45_1
e_1_2_6_26_1
e_1_2_6_47_1
e_1_2_6_68_1
e_1_2_6_52_1
Omedes A (e_1_2_6_54_1) 1981; 7
e_1_2_6_73_1
e_1_2_6_75_1
e_1_2_6_10_1
e_1_2_6_31_1
e_1_2_6_50_1
e_1_2_6_71_1
Biben M (e_1_2_6_13_1) 1999
Hopkins WD (e_1_2_6_34_1) 2006; 65
e_1_2_6_14_1
e_1_2_6_35_1
e_1_2_6_12_1
e_1_2_6_33_1
e_1_2_6_18_1
e_1_2_6_39_1
e_1_2_6_56_1
e_1_2_6_16_1
e_1_2_6_37_1
e_1_2_6_58_1
e_1_2_6_63_1
e_1_2_6_42_1
e_1_2_6_65_1
e_1_2_6_21_1
e_1_2_6_40_1
e_1_2_6_61_1
e_1_2_6_8_1
e_1_2_6_4_1
e_1_2_6_6_1
e_1_2_6_25_1
e_1_2_6_48_1
e_1_2_6_23_1
e_1_2_6_2_1
e_1_2_6_29_1
e_1_2_6_44_1
e_1_2_6_67_1
e_1_2_6_27_1
e_1_2_6_46_1
e_1_2_6_69_1
References_xml – reference: Peignot P, Jankowski F, Anderson JR. 2004. On self-scratching in captive mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Folia Primatologica 75:160-164.
– reference: Judge PG, Evans DW, Schroepfer KK, Gross AC. 2011. Perseveration on a reversal-learning task correlates with rates of self-directed behavior in nonhuman primates. Behavioural Brain Research 222:57-65.
– reference: Judge PG, Griffaton NS, Fincke AM. 2006. Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) during crowding: a tension-reduction strategy. American Journal of Primatology 68:993-1006.
– reference: Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M. 1989. On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiology & Behavior 45:667-670.
– reference: Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ. 1998. Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia 36:1265-1273.
– reference: Waitt C, Buchanan-Smith HM. 2001. What time is feeding?: how delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump-tailed macaque behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 75:75-85.
– reference: Cross N, Rogers LJ. 2006. Mobbing vocalizations as a coping response in the common marmoset. Hormones and Behavior 49:237-245.
– reference: Dettling A, Pryce CR, Martin RD, Dobeli M. 1998. Physiological responses to parental separation and a strange situation are related to parental care received in juvenile Goeldi's monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Developmental Psychobiology 33:21-31.
– reference: Schino G, Perretta G, Taglioni AM, Monaco V, Troisi A. 1996. Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety 2:186-191.
– reference: Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Freeman H, et al. 2006. Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal. Emotion 65:53-559.
– reference: Barros M, Boere V, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2000. Measuring fear and anxiety in the marmoset (Callithrix penicillata) with a novel predator confrontation model: effects of diazepam. Behavioural Brain Research 108:205-211.
– reference: Polizzi di Sorrentino E, Schino G, Tiddi B, Aureli F. 2012. Scratching as a window into the emotional responses of wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Ethology 118:1072-1084.
– reference: Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Schino G. 1991. Conflict, displacement activities, and menstrual cycle in long-tailed macaques. Primates 32:115-118.
– reference: Bassett L, Buchanan-Smith HM, McKinley J, Smith T. 2003. Effects of training on stress-related behavior of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping with routine husbandry procedures. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6:221-233.
– reference: Duboscq J, Agil M, Engelhardt A, Thierry B. 2014. The function of postconflict interactions: new prospects from the study of a tolerant species of primate. Animal Behaviour 87:107-120.
– reference: D'Anna KL, Hoffman MC, Zerbe GO, et al. 2012. Acculturation, maternal cortisol release and birth outcomes in women of Mexican descent. Psychosomatic Medicine 74:296-304.
– reference: Baldwin JD, Baldwin JI. 1974. Effects of food ecology on social play: a laboratory simulation. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 40:1-14.
– reference: Barros M, Giorgetti M, Souto AAV, et al. 2007. Persistent anxiety-like behavior in marmosets following a recent predatory stress condition: reversal by diazepam. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 84:705-711.
– reference: Castles DL, Whiten A. 1998. Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. II. Stress and self-directed behaviour. Ethology 104:148-160.
– reference: Nakayama K. 2004. Observing conspecifics scratching induces a contagion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Journal of Comparative Psychology 118:20-24.
– reference: O'Neill-Wagner PL, Bolig R, Price CS. 1994. Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups?. Communication and Cognition 27:261-272.
– reference: Braesicke K, Parkinson JA, Reekir Y, et al. 2005. Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis. European Journal of Neuroscience 21:1733-1740.
– reference: Palagi E, Norscia I. 2011. Scratching around stress: hierarchy and reconciliation make the difference in wild brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus). Stress 14:93-97.
– reference: Mootnick AR, Cunningham C, Baker E. 2012. Phylogenetic separation in limb use in captive gibbons (Hylobatidae): a comparison across the primate order. American Journal of Primatology 74:1035-1043.
– reference: Kaburu SSK, MacLarnon A, Majolo B, Qarro M, Semple S. 2012. Dominance rank and self-scratching among wild female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). African Zoololgy 47:74-79.
– reference: Schino G, Troisi A, Perretta G, Monaco V. 1991. Measuring anxiety in non-human primates: effect of lorazepam on macaque scratching. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 38:889-891.
– reference: Smith TE, McGreer-Whitworth B, French JA. 1998. Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel-cage housing in black tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Hormones and Behavior 34:211-222.
– reference: Manson JH, Perry S. 2000. Correlates of self-directed behaviour in wild white-faced Capuchins. Ethology 106:301-317.
– reference: Clara E, Tommasi L, Rogers LJ. 2008. Social mobbing calls in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): effects of experience and associated cortisol levels. Animal Cognition 11:349-358.
– reference: Parr LA, Hopkins WD. 2000. Brain temperature asymmetries and emotional perception in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Physiology & Behavior 71:363-371.
– reference: Silva IO, Alvarenga ABB, Boere V. 2008. Occasional field observations of the predation on mice, dove and ants by black-tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix penicillata). Neotropical Primates 15:59-62.
– reference: Semple S, Harrison C, Lehmann J. 2013. Grooming and anxiety in Barbary macaques. Ethology 119:779-785.
– reference: Buckley V, Semple S. 2012. Evidence that displacement activities facilitate behavioural transitions in ring-tailed lemurs. Behavioural Processes 90:433-435.
– reference: Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2002. Anxiolytic-like effects of Substance P fragment (SP1-7) in non-human primates (Callithrix jacchus). Peptides 23:967-973.
– reference: Baker KC, Aureli F. 1997. Behavioural indicators of anxiety: an empirical test in chimpanzees. Behaviour 134:1031-1050.
– reference: Dettmer AM, Novak MA, Suomi SJ, Meyer JS. 2012. Physiological and behavioral adaptation to relocation stress in differentially reared rhesus monkeys: hair cortisol as a biomarker for anxiety-related responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:191-199.
– reference: Loy J. 1970. Behavioral responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 33:263-271.
– reference: Jacob RG, Thayer JF, Manuck SB, et al. 1999. Ambulatory blood pressure responses and the circumplex model of mood: a 4-day study. Psychosomatic Medicine 61:319-333.
– reference: Judge PG, Bachman KA. 2013. Witnessing reconciliation reduces arousal of bystanders in a baboon group (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). Animal Behaviour 85:881-889.
– reference: Easley SP, Coelho AM Jr., Taylor LL. 1987. Scratching dominance, tension, and displacement in male baboons. American Journal of Primatology 13:397-411.
– reference: Omedes A. 1981. Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets Callithrix a. argentata and Callithrix a. melanura. Miscellania Zoologica 7:193-211.
– reference: Pearson BL, Reeder D, Judge PG. 2015. Crowding increases salivary cortisol but not self-directed behavior in captive baboons. American Journal of Primatology 77:462-467.
– reference: Aureli F. 1997. Post-conflict anxiety in non-human primates: the mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution. Aggressive Behavior 23:315-328.
– reference: Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A. 1991. A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Animal Behaviour 44:967-979.
– reference: Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD. 2004. Behavioral evidence for the cutaneous expression of emotion in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Behavior 141:979-997.
– reference: de Boer RA, Overduin-de Vries AM, Louwerse AL, Sterck EHM. 2013. The behavioral context of visual displays in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology 75:1084-1095.
– reference: Diezinger F, Anderson JR. 1986. Starting from scratch: a first look at a "displacement activity" in group-living rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 11:117-124.
– reference: Aureli F, Yates K. 2010. Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques. Biology Letters 6:27-29.
– reference: Judge PG, Mullen SH. 2005. Quadratic post-conflict affiliation among bystanders in a hamadryas baboon group. Animal Behaviour 69:1345-1355.
– reference: Mellou E. 2006. Play theories: a contemporary review. Early Child Development and Care 102:91-100.
– reference: Schino G, Maestripieri D, Scucchi S, Turillazzi PG. 1990. Social tension in familiar and unfamiliar pairs of long-tailed macaques. Behaviour 113:264-272.
– reference: Waldstein SR, Kop WJ, Schmidt LA, et al. 2000. Frontal electrocortical and cardiovascular reactivity during happiness and anger. Biological Psychology 55:3-23.
– reference: Schlafani A, Norscia I, Antonacci D, Palagi E. 2012. Scratching around mating: factors affecting anxiety in wild Lemur catta. Primates 53:247-254.
– reference: Norscia I, Palagi E. 2011. When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. Primates 52:101-104.
– reference: Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2004. Multibehavioral analysis of fear and anxiety before, during, and after experimentally induced predatory stress in Callithrix penicillata. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 78:357-367.
– reference: Kato Y, Gokan H, Oh-Nishi A, et al. 2014. Vocalizations associated with anxiety and fear in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Behavioural Brain Research 275:43-52.
– reference: Engel SR, Creson TK, Hao Y, et al. 2009. The extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway contributed to the control of behavioral excitement. Molecular Psychiatry 14:448-461.
– reference: Saltzman W, Abbott DH. 2009. Effects of elevated circulating cortisol concentration on maternal behavior in common marmoset mothers (Callithrix jacchus). Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:1222-1234.
– reference: Aureli F, van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM. 1989. Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology 19:39-51.
– reference: Maestripieri D. 1993. Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) I. Measurement of anxiety and identification of anxiety-eliciting situations. Ethology 95:19-31.
– reference: Held SDE, Spinka M. 2011. Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81:891-899.
– reference: Cilia J, Piper DC. 1996. Marmoset conspecific confrontation: an ethologically-based model of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 58:85-91.
– reference: Elder CM, Menzel CR. 2001. Dissociation of cortisol and behavioral indicators of stress in an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) during a computerized task. Primates 42:345-357.
– reference: Spinka M, Newberry RC, Bekoff M. 2001. Mammilian play: training for the unexpected. The Quarterly Review of Biology 76:141-168.
– reference: Schwartz GE, Weinberger DA, Singer JA. 1981. Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine 43:343-364.
– reference: Pavani S, Maestripieri D, Schino G, Turillazzi PG, Scucchi S. 1991. Factors influencing scratching behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Folia Primatologica 57:34-38.
– reference: Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD, Hyatt CW. 2001. Effects of cognitive challenge on self-directed behaviors by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 55:1-14.
– reference: Ebner K, Singewald N. 2006. The role of substance P in stress and anxiety responses. Amino Acids 31:251-272.
– reference: Troisi A, Schino G, D'Antoni M, et al. 1991. Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers. Behavioral and Neural Biology 56:307-313.
– volume: 113
  start-page: 264
  year: 1990
  end-page: 272
  article-title: Social tension in familiar and unfamiliar pairs of long‐tailed macaques
  publication-title: Behaviour
– volume: 44
  start-page: 967
  year: 1991
  end-page: 979
  article-title: A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
– volume: 15
  start-page: 59
  year: 2008
  end-page: 62
  article-title: Occasional field observations of the predation on mice, dove and ants by black‐tufted‐ear marmosets ( )
  publication-title: Neotropical Primates
– volume: 14
  start-page: 448
  year: 2009
  end-page: 461
  article-title: The extracellular signal‐regulated kinase pathway contributed to the control of behavioral excitement
  publication-title: Molecular Psychiatry
– start-page: 191
  year: 1999
  end-page: 208
– volume: 75
  start-page: 1084
  year: 2013
  end-page: 1095
  article-title: The behavioral context of visual displays in common marmosets ( )
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 36
  start-page: 1265
  year: 1998
  end-page: 1273
  article-title: Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 104
  start-page: 148
  year: 1998
  end-page: 160
  article-title: Post‐conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. II. Stress and self‐directed behaviour
  publication-title: Ethology
– volume: 106
  start-page: 301
  year: 2000
  end-page: 317
  article-title: Correlates of self‐directed behaviour in wild white‐faced Capuchins
  publication-title: Ethology
– volume: 108
  start-page: 205
  year: 2000
  end-page: 211
  article-title: Measuring fear and anxiety in the marmoset ( ) with a novel predator confrontation model: effects of diazepam
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
– volume: 85
  start-page: 881
  year: 2013
  end-page: 889
  article-title: Witnessing reconciliation reduces arousal of bystanders in a baboon group ( )
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
– volume: 68
  start-page: 993
  year: 2006
  end-page: 1006
  article-title: Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons ( during crowding: a tension‐reduction strategy
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 21
  start-page: 1733
  year: 2005
  end-page: 1740
  article-title: Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 55
  start-page: 3
  year: 2000
  end-page: 23
  article-title: Frontal electrocortical and cardiovascular reactivity during happiness and anger
  publication-title: Biological Psychology
– volume: 275
  start-page: 43
  year: 2014
  end-page: 52
  article-title: Vocalizations associated with anxiety and fear in the common marmoset ( )
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
– volume: 23
  start-page: 315
  year: 1997
  end-page: 328
  article-title: Post‐conflict anxiety in non‐human primates: the mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution
  publication-title: Aggressive Behavior
– volume: 23
  start-page: 967
  year: 2002
  end-page: 973
  article-title: Anxiolytic‐like effects of Substance P fragment (SP ) in non‐human primates ( )
  publication-title: Peptides
– volume: 74
  start-page: 296
  year: 2012
  end-page: 304
  article-title: Acculturation, maternal cortisol release and birth outcomes in women of Mexican descent
  publication-title: Psychosomatic Medicine
– volume: 13
  start-page: 397
  year: 1987
  end-page: 411
  article-title: Scratching dominance, tension, and displacement in male baboons
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 31
  start-page: 251
  year: 2006
  end-page: 272
  article-title: The role of substance P in stress and anxiety responses
  publication-title: Amino Acids
– volume: 11
  start-page: 117
  year: 1986
  end-page: 124
  article-title: Starting from scratch: a first look at a “displacement activity” in group‐living rhesus monkeys
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 222
  start-page: 57
  year: 2011
  end-page: 65
  article-title: Perseveration on a reversal‐learning task correlates with rates of self‐directed behavior in nonhuman primates
  publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research
– volume: 47
  start-page: 74
  year: 2012
  end-page: 79
  article-title: Dominance rank and self‐scratching among wild female Barbary macaques ( )
  publication-title: African Zoololgy
– volume: 57
  start-page: 34
  year: 1991
  end-page: 38
  article-title: Factors influencing scratching behaviour in long‐tailed macaques ( )
  publication-title: Folia Primatologica
– volume: 75
  start-page: 160
  year: 2004
  end-page: 164
  article-title: On self‐scratching in captive mandrills ( )
  publication-title: Folia Primatologica
– volume: 32
  start-page: 115
  year: 1991
  end-page: 118
  article-title: Conflict, displacement activities, and menstrual cycle in long‐tailed macaques
  publication-title: Primates
– volume: 141
  start-page: 979
  year: 2004
  end-page: 997
  article-title: Behavioral evidence for the cutaneous expression of emotion in a chimpanzee ( )
  publication-title: Behavior
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1345
  year: 2005
  end-page: 1355
  article-title: Quadratic post‐conflict affiliation among bystanders in a hamadryas baboon group
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
– volume: 74
  start-page: 1035
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1043
  article-title: Phylogenetic separation in limb use in captive gibbons ( ): a comparison across the primate order
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 14
  start-page: 93
  year: 2011
  end-page: 97
  article-title: Scratching around stress: hierarchy and reconciliation make the difference in wild brown lemurs ( )
  publication-title: Stress
– volume: 119
  start-page: 779
  year: 2013
  end-page: 785
  article-title: Grooming and anxiety in Barbary macaques
  publication-title: Ethology
– volume: 65
  start-page: 53
  year: 2006
  end-page: 559
  article-title: Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees ( ): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal
  publication-title: Emotion
– volume: 102
  start-page: 91
  year: 2006
  end-page: 100
  article-title: Play theories: a contemporary review
  publication-title: Early Child Development and Care
– volume: 34
  start-page: 211
  year: 1998
  end-page: 222
  article-title: Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel‐cage housing in black tufted‐ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli)
  publication-title: Hormones and Behavior
– volume: 77
  start-page: 462
  year: 2015
  end-page: 467
  article-title: Crowding increases salivary cortisol but not self‐directed behavior in captive baboons
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 118
  start-page: 1072
  year: 2012
  end-page: 1084
  article-title: Scratching as a window into the emotional responses of wild tufted capuchin monkeys
  publication-title: Ethology
– volume: 43
  start-page: 343
  year: 1981
  end-page: 364
  article-title: Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise
  publication-title: Psychosomatic Medicine
– start-page: 471
  year: 1980
  end-page: 495
– volume: 58
  start-page: 85
  year: 1996
  end-page: 91
  article-title: Marmoset conspecific confrontation: an ethologically‐based model of anxiety
  publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior
– volume: 52
  start-page: 101
  year: 2011
  end-page: 104
  article-title: When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets
  publication-title: Primates
– volume: 49
  start-page: 237
  year: 2006
  end-page: 245
  article-title: Mobbing vocalizations as a coping response in the common marmoset
  publication-title: Hormones and Behavior
– volume: 42
  start-page: 345
  year: 2001
  end-page: 357
  article-title: Dissociation of cortisol and behavioral indicators of stress in an orangutan ( ) during a computerized task
  publication-title: Primates
– volume: 27
  start-page: 261
  year: 1994
  end-page: 272
  article-title: Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups
  publication-title: Communication and Cognition
– volume: 37
  start-page: 191
  year: 2012
  end-page: 199
  article-title: Physiological and behavioral adaptation to relocation stress in differentially reared rhesus monkeys: hair cortisol as a biomarker for anxiety‐related responses
  publication-title: Psychoneuroendocrinology
– volume: 87
  start-page: 107
  year: 2014
  end-page: 120
  article-title: The function of postconflict interactions: new prospects from the study of a tolerant species of primate
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
– volume: 81
  start-page: 891
  year: 2011
  end-page: 899
  article-title: Animal play and animal welfare
  publication-title: Animal Behaviour
– volume: 40
  start-page: 1
  year: 1974
  end-page: 14
  article-title: Effects of food ecology on social play: a laboratory simulation
  publication-title: Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie
– volume: 134
  start-page: 1031
  year: 1997
  end-page: 1050
  article-title: Behavioural indicators of anxiety: an empirical test in chimpanzees
  publication-title: Behaviour
– volume: 76
  start-page: 141
  year: 2001
  end-page: 168
  article-title: Mammilian play: training for the unexpected
  publication-title: The Quarterly Review of Biology
– volume: 118
  start-page: 20
  year: 2004
  end-page: 24
  article-title: Observing conspecifics scratching induces a contagion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata)
  publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology
– year: 2010
– volume: 90
  start-page: 433
  year: 2012
  end-page: 435
  article-title: Evidence that displacement activities facilitate behavioural transitions in ring‐tailed lemurs
  publication-title: Behavioural Processes
– volume: 61
  start-page: 319
  year: 1999
  end-page: 333
  article-title: Ambulatory blood pressure responses and the circumplex model of mood: a 4‐day study
  publication-title: Psychosomatic Medicine
– volume: 19
  start-page: 39
  year: 1989
  end-page: 51
  article-title: Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long‐tailed macaques ( )
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 53
  start-page: 247
  year: 2012
  end-page: 254
  article-title: Scratching around mating: factors affecting anxiety in wild
  publication-title: Primates
– volume: 11
  start-page: 349
  year: 2008
  end-page: 358
  article-title: Social mobbing calls in common marmosets ( ): effects of experience and associated cortisol levels
  publication-title: Animal Cognition
– volume: 56
  start-page: 307
  year: 1991
  end-page: 313
  article-title: Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers
  publication-title: Behavioral and Neural Biology
– volume: 33
  start-page: 21
  year: 1998
  end-page: 31
  article-title: Physiological responses to parental separation and a strange situation are related to parental care received in juvenile Goeldi's monkeys ( )
  publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology
– volume: 55
  start-page: 1
  year: 2001
  end-page: 14
  article-title: Effects of cognitive challenge on self‐directed behaviors by chimpanzees ( )
  publication-title: American Journal of Primatology
– volume: 71
  start-page: 363
  year: 2000
  end-page: 371
  article-title: Brain temperature asymmetries and emotional perception in chimpanzees,
  publication-title: Physiology & Behavior
– volume: 7
  start-page: 193
  year: 1981
  end-page: 211
  article-title: Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets and
  publication-title: Miscellania Zoologica
– year: 2006
– volume: 6
  start-page: 27
  year: 2010
  end-page: 29
  article-title: Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques
  publication-title: Biology Letters
– volume: 84
  start-page: 705
  year: 2007
  end-page: 711
  article-title: Persistent anxiety‐like behavior in marmosets following a recent predatory stress condition: reversal by diazepam
  publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior
– volume: 34
  start-page: 1222
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1234
  article-title: Effects of elevated circulating cortisol concentration on maternal behavior in common marmoset mothers ( )
  publication-title: Psychoneuroendocrinology
– volume: 6
  start-page: 221
  year: 2003
  end-page: 233
  article-title: Effects of training on stress‐related behavior of the common marmoset ( ) in relation to coping with routine husbandry procedures
  publication-title: Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
– volume: 38
  start-page: 889
  year: 1991
  end-page: 891
  article-title: Measuring anxiety in non‐human primates: effect of lorazepam on macaque scratching
  publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior
– volume: 75
  start-page: 75
  year: 2001
  end-page: 85
  article-title: What time is feeding?: how delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump‐tailed macaque behavior
  publication-title: Applied Animal Behaviour Science
– volume: 95
  start-page: 19
  year: 1993
  end-page: 31
  article-title: Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques ( ) I. Measurement of anxiety and identification of anxiety‐eliciting situations
  publication-title: Ethology
– volume: 33
  start-page: 263
  year: 1970
  end-page: 271
  article-title: Behavioral responses of free‐ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage
  publication-title: American Journal of Physical Anthropology
– volume: 2
  start-page: 186
  year: 1996
  end-page: 191
  article-title: Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety
  publication-title: Anxiety
– volume: 78
  start-page: 357
  year: 2004
  end-page: 367
  article-title: Multibehavioral analysis of fear and anxiety before, during, and after experimentally induced predatory stress in
  publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior
– volume: 45
  start-page: 667
  year: 1989
  end-page: 670
  article-title: On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque
  publication-title: Physiology & Behavior
– ident: e_1_2_6_28_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350130405
– ident: e_1_2_6_57_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00349-8
– ident: e_1_2_6_75_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00065-X
– ident: e_1_2_6_31_1
  doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002135
– ident: e_1_2_6_56_1
  doi: 10.3109/10253890.2010.505272
– ident: e_1_2_6_2_1
  doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1997)23:5<315::AID-AB2>3.0.CO;2-H
– ident: e_1_2_6_27_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.018
– ident: e_1_2_6_21_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.007
– ident: e_1_2_6_58_1
  doi: 10.1159/000156561
– ident: e_1_2_6_70_1
  doi: 10.1896/044.015.0209
– ident: e_1_2_6_20_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0125-0
– ident: e_1_2_6_36_1
– ident: e_1_2_6_39_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.20290
– ident: e_1_2_6_23_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.22167
– ident: e_1_2_6_8_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00153-9
– ident: e_1_2_6_68_1
  doi: 10.1007/BF02381606
– ident: e_1_2_6_73_1
  doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(91)90469-7
– ident: e_1_2_6_22_1
  doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318244fbde
– ident: e_1_2_6_42_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.047
– ident: e_1_2_6_59_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.22363
– ident: e_1_2_6_60_1
  doi: 10.1159/000078307
– ident: e_1_2_6_38_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.016
– ident: e_1_2_6_30_1
  doi: 10.1007/BF02629625
– ident: e_1_2_6_49_1
  doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00527.x
– ident: e_1_2_6_18_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00058.x
– volume: 65
  start-page: 53
  year: 2006
  ident: e_1_2_6_34_1
  article-title: Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal
  publication-title: Emotion
– ident: e_1_2_6_10_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.008
– ident: e_1_2_6_71_1
  doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1469
– ident: e_1_2_6_17_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.04.009
– ident: e_1_2_6_37_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.011
– ident: e_1_2_6_66_1
  doi: 10.1097/00006842-198108000-00007
– ident: e_1_2_6_7_1
  doi: 10.1163/156853997X00386
– ident: e_1_2_6_52_1
  doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.20
– start-page: 191
  volume-title: Saimiri
  year: 1999
  ident: e_1_2_6_13_1
– ident: e_1_2_6_53_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10329-010-0228-0
– ident: e_1_2_6_14_1
  doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90089-9
– ident: e_1_2_6_32_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007
– ident: e_1_2_6_15_1
– ident: e_1_2_6_41_1
  doi: 10.3377/004.047.0111
– ident: e_1_2_6_44_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.1034
– ident: e_1_2_6_43_1
  doi: 10.1146/annurev.an.09.100180.002351
– ident: e_1_2_6_19_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00376-0
– ident: e_1_2_6_4_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350190105
– ident: e_1_2_6_12_1
  doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_07
– ident: e_1_2_6_45_1
  doi: 10.1163/1568539042360189
– ident: e_1_2_6_48_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80592-5
– ident: e_1_2_6_50_1
  doi: 10.1080/0300443941020107
– ident: e_1_2_6_72_1
  doi: 10.1086/393866
– ident: e_1_2_6_24_1
  doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199807)33:1<21::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-U
– ident: e_1_2_6_40_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.016
– ident: e_1_2_6_6_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1976.tb00922.x
– ident: e_1_2_6_74_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00174-5
– volume: 7
  start-page: 193
  year: 1981
  ident: e_1_2_6_54_1
  article-title: Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets Callithrix a. argentata and Callithrix a. melanura
  publication-title: Miscellania Zoologica
– ident: e_1_2_6_62_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.012
– ident: e_1_2_6_63_1
  doi: 10.1163/156853990X00518
– ident: e_1_2_6_46_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330330212
– ident: e_1_2_6_11_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.02.016
– ident: e_1_2_6_64_1
  doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90258-4
– ident: e_1_2_6_3_1
  doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0513
– ident: e_1_2_6_29_1
  doi: 10.1007/s00726-006-0335-9
– ident: e_1_2_6_51_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.22057
– ident: e_1_2_6_65_1
  doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-7154(1996)2:4<186::AID-ANXI5>3.0.CO;2-M
– ident: e_1_2_6_9_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0196-9781(02)00020-7
– ident: e_1_2_6_67_1
  doi: 10.1007/s10329-012-0294-6
– ident: e_1_2_6_5_1
– ident: e_1_2_6_61_1
  doi: 10.1111/eth.12008
– ident: e_1_2_6_33_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00037-2
– ident: e_1_2_6_47_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00453.x
– ident: e_1_2_6_25_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.06.003
– ident: e_1_2_6_16_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03987.x
– ident: e_1_2_6_35_1
  doi: 10.1097/00006842-199905000-00011
– ident: e_1_2_6_69_1
  doi: 10.1111/eth.12119
– volume: 27
  start-page: 261
  year: 1994
  ident: e_1_2_6_55_1
  article-title: Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups
  publication-title: Communication and Cognition
– ident: e_1_2_6_26_1
  doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350110204
SSID ssj0011465
Score 2.2073786
Snippet Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
wiley
istex
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 216
SubjectTerms Animal behavior
Animals
Anxiety
Arousal
Blood pressure
Callithrix - physiology
callitrichid
Correlation analysis
excitement
Female
Heart rate
Male
Monkeys & apes
scratching
Studies
Title Scratching under positive and negative arousal in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)
URI https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-TS67SX0K-K/fulltext.pdf
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002%2Fajp.22498
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26530306
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1757722135
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1760860592
Volume 78
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1ba9VAEB5KRSiCl3qLVllFpD7kNNlkdxt8KsVaWiziaelBhGVvsdeckpwj1V_v7OailQriW0ImyV5mdr5JZr4FeCUSxX3-YGy5yOJcWxprRou4RGfstEaEn_t65w97fPsg35mwyQK87WthWn6I4YObt4ywXnsDV7pZ-0Uaqk4uRuh_Cl_o63O1PCD6NFBH-WLbkL5IBYvRrbOeVSiha8OdV3zRDT-sl9cBzau4NTierTvwpW9ym29yOprP9Mj8-IPN8T_7dBdud4CUbLQadA8WXLUMt37bP-H7MiwNyySe3Pw8DUf34XBsUH9CMibxtWg1aVPAvjmiKksq91W1J_V03uA7jiuCDUK9J-eqPp82btaQ1U11hpHAUX18SU6UMUfz5s0DONh6t7-5HXcbNcQm5ykumC6lXFhjE12YLOe4jJQZzTJj2bpKU6NNWtokVakWTItCrTtWZEWplTXM0-FnD2GxmlbuMZDccWdzxaznyadGKJobV-JThKYUQ7MIVvspk6ZjMfebaZzJln-ZShxDGcYwgpeD6EVL3XGd0Osw74OEqk99rptg8nDvvdwfczGeJLtyN4KVXjFkZ-aNROyF0QlNMxbBi-EyGqj_66Iqh6OLMhzDRkSxNIJHrUINL6OcZT5ow14Ftfh7O-XGzsdw8OTfRZ_CEsK7Lsd8BRZn9dw9Qwg108-DrfwE4UsVkg
linkProvider Wiley-Blackwell
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3rb9MwED-NTYgJicdgIzDAIITGh3SJEyeNxJdpYpR1qxDttAoJWX6VPdMpadHgr-fsPGBoSIhvtnKJ7fOd7845_wzwKg1EYvMHfZ2kkR9LTX3JaOZP0BgbKdHDj-155_1B0juId8dsvABvm7MwFT5Eu-FmNcOt11bB7Yb05i_UUHFy0UEDlHVvwJK90dsFVJ9a8Ch73NYlMNKU-WjYWYMrFNDN9tUr1mjJMvbyOlfzqufqTM_OXfjSdLrKODntzGeyo378gef4v6O6B3dqn5RsVUJ0HxZMvgK3f7tC4fsKLLcrJVZufp660gM4HCoUIZePSexxtIJUWWDfDBG5Jrn5KqpKMZ2X2MZxTrBHKPrkXBTn09LMSrKxLc4wGDgqji_JiVDqaF6-eQgHO-9G2z2_vqvBV3ES4pppQpqkWulAZiqKE1xJJhGNIqVZV4Shkiqc6CAUoUyZTDPRNSyLsokUWjGLiB-twmI-zc0jILFJjI4F0xYqn6pU0FiZCX4llZRidObBRjNnXNVA5vY-jTNeQTBTjjzkjocevGxJLyr0juuIXruJbylEcWrT3VLGDwfv-WiYpMNx0Od9D9YbyeC1ppcc3S8MUGgYMQ9etI9RR-2PF5Eb5C7SJBg5oiNLPVirJKptjCYssnEbjsrJxd_7ybd2P7rC438nfQ63eqP9Pb73YdB_Asvo7dUp5-uwOCvm5il6VDP5zCnOTykjGa0
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3rb9MwED-NTaAJicd4BQYEhND2IV3ixHEjPk0bZaxQTXTTKoRk-RX2TKu0RYO_nrPzgKEhIb7ZyiWxz3e-u-TuZ4BXLBSpzR8MdMriIJGaBJKSLMjRGBsp0cNPbL3zx0G6c5DsjuhoAd40tTAVPkT7wc1qhtuvrYJPdL7xCzRUnEw6aH-y7jVYStKwa0V6-1OLHWWrbV3-ImE0QLtOG1ihkGy0t14yRkuWrxdXeZqXHVdneXq34Usz5irh5LQzn8mO-vEHnON_TuoO3Ko9Un-zEqG7sGCKFbj52wEK31dgud0nsXP989i17sHhUKEAuWxM3xajlX6VA_bN-KLQfmG-iqpTjudTfMdx4eOAUPD9c1Gej6dmNvXXtsQZhgJH5fGFfyKUOppP1-_DQe_t_tZOUJ_UEKgkjXDHNBFJmVY6lJmKcTVYmMckjpWmXRFFSqoo12EkIsmoZJnoGprFWS6FVtTi4ccPYLEYF-YR-IlJjU4E1RYonygmSKJMjk9hkhCMzTxYa5aMqxrG3J6mccYrAGbCkYfc8dCDly3ppMLuuIrotVv3lkKUpzbZjVF-OHjH94cpG47CPu97sNoIBq_1fMrR-cLwhEQx9eBFexk11P52EYVB7iINSidGjRnx4GElUO3LSEpjG7XhrJxY_H2cfHN3zzUe_zvpc7ixt93jH94P-k9gGV29Ot98FRZn5dw8RXdqJp85tfkJ5lQYZQ
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Scratching+under+positive+and+negative+arousal+in+common+marmosets+%28Callithrix+jacchus%29&rft.jtitle=American+journal+of+primatology&rft.au=Neal%2C+Sarah+J&rft.au=Caine%2C+Nancy+G&rft.date=2016-02-01&rft.eissn=1098-2345&rft.volume=78&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=216&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002%2Fajp.22498&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F26530306&rft.externalDocID=26530306
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon