Scratching under positive and negative arousal in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)
Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anx...
Saved in:
Published in | American journal of primatology Vol. 78; no. 2; pp. 216 - 226 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Blackwell Publishing Ltd
01.02.2016
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor‐housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post‐induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post‐induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety–scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non‐human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species. Am. J. Primatol. 78:216–226, 2016. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor-housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post-induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post-induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety-scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non-human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species. Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor‐housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post‐induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post‐induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety‐provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety–scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non‐human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species. Am. J. Primatol. 78:216–226, 2016. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor-housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post-induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post-induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety-scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non-human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species.Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances. In addition, the existing literature has investigated scratching only in relation to negative arousal (i.e., anxiety), even though anxiety and excitement (positive arousal) share important physiological and behavioral correlates, including increased heart rate, blood pressure, and locomotion. In the current study, we scored all instances of scratching in 11 outdoor-housed captive common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) during three contexts that were intended to be negatively arousing and three contexts that were intended to be positively arousing during a baseline, manipulation, and post-induction period. Summed across the three negative arousal contexts, the results showed that subjects exhibited significantly lower scratching rates during the manipulation than during either the baseline or post-induction periods, and the pattern of means was the same for all three of those contexts. Under the three contexts of positive arousal, subjects exhibited different patterns of scratching rates during the manipulation periods (play = increases, foraging = decreases, food anticipation = no change). Data from the current study, and a close examination of data from studies showing no change in scratching under anxiety-provoking circumstances, suggest that the anxiety-scratching relationship may be more complex than has been reported previously. Our results raise a potential concern about the unchallenged use of scratching as a behavioral indicator of anxiety in captive non-human primates, with important implications for welfare and management of these species. |
Author | Caine, Nancy G. Neal, Sarah J. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Sarah J. surname: Neal fullname: Neal, Sarah J. email: Correspondence to: Sarah J. Neal, University of Wisconsin, Madison, 1202 W. Johnson St., Madison, WI 53706. , sneal2@wisc.edu organization: California State University San Marcos, California, San Marcos – sequence: 2 givenname: Nancy G. surname: Caine fullname: Caine, Nancy G. organization: California State University San Marcos, California, San Marcos |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26530306$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kU9P3DAQxa2KChbKoV-gstQLHAJjO7aTI1oVaEFtpaWiN8txHNbbxN7aCX--fU0XOCC1p9FIv_c0894u2vLBW4TeEzgiAPRYr9ZHlJZ19QbNCNRVQVnJt9AMqOQF5YLvoN2UVgCElIJvox0qOAMGYoauFybq0Sydv8GTb23E65Dc6G4t1r7F3t7ozRLDlHSPnccmDEPweNBxCMmOCR_Mdd-7cRndPV5pY5ZTOnyH3na6T3b_ae6hH6efrubnxeW3s8_zk8vClIJURW0JFbI1LTS1YaUACR2jjJmWV5oQ0xjStUA0aSRvZK0ry2tWd41uDadQcraHDja-6xh-TzaNanDJ2L7X3uaLFZECKgG8phn9-ApdhSn6fF2muJSUEvZo-OGJmprBtmodXf70QT1HloHDDWBiSCna7gUhoB7rULkO9beOzB6_Yo0bc6DBj1G7_n-KO9fbh39bq5Mv358VxUbh0mjvXxQ6_lJCMsnV9dczdbUQcvETLtQF-wPJgqmN |
CODEN | AJPTDU |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23138 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_23677 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2017_12_005 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_applanim_2019_03_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2017_06_001 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0250913 crossref_primary_10_3390_ani12213002 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_39214_2 crossref_primary_10_3390_jzbg2020012 crossref_primary_10_7120_09627286_27_1_081 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_025_92251_x crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_10754_8 crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2021_0484 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2024_105950 |
Cites_doi | 10.1002/ajp.1350130405 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00349-8 10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00065-X 10.1038/sj.mp.4002135 10.3109/10253890.2010.505272 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1997)23:5<315::AID-AB2>3.0.CO;2-H 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.018 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.007 10.1159/000156561 10.1896/044.015.0209 10.1007/s10071-007-0125-0 10.1002/ajp.20290 10.1002/ajp.22167 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00153-9 10.1007/BF02381606 10.1016/0163-1047(91)90469-7 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318244fbde 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.047 10.1002/ajp.22363 10.1159/000078307 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.016 10.1007/BF02629625 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00527.x 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00058.x 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.008 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1469 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.04.009 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.011 10.1097/00006842-198108000-00007 10.1163/156853997X00386 10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.20 10.1007/s10329-010-0228-0 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90089-9 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007 10.3377/004.047.0111 10.1002/ajp.1034 10.1146/annurev.an.09.100180.002351 10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00376-0 10.1002/ajp.1350190105 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_07 10.1163/1568539042360189 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80592-5 10.1080/0300443941020107 10.1086/393866 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199807)33:1<21::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-U 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.016 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1976.tb00922.x 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00174-5 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.012 10.1163/156853990X00518 10.1002/ajpa.1330330212 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.02.016 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90258-4 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0513 10.1007/s00726-006-0335-9 10.1002/ajp.22057 10.1002/(SICI)1522-7154(1996)2:4<186::AID-ANXI5>3.0.CO;2-M 10.1016/S0196-9781(02)00020-7 10.1007/s10329-012-0294-6 10.1111/eth.12008 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00037-2 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00453.x 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.06.003 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03987.x 10.1097/00006842-199905000-00011 10.1111/eth.12119 10.1002/ajp.1350110204 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. – notice: 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
DBID | BSCLL AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7SN 7TK 8BJ 8FD C1K FQK FR3 JBE K9. P64 RC3 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1002/ajp.22498 |
DatabaseName | Istex CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management International Bibliography of the Social Sciences Engineering Research Database International Bibliography of the Social Sciences ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Genetics Abstracts International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS) Technology Research Database Animal Behavior Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Engineering Research Database Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anthropology Zoology Psychology |
EISSN | 1098-2345 |
EndPage | 226 |
ExternalDocumentID | 3924352671 26530306 10_1002_ajp_22498 AJP22498 ark_67375_WNG_TS67SX0K_K |
Genre | article Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ .3N .GA .Y3 05W 0R~ 10A 1L6 1OB 1OC 1ZS 23M 31~ 33P 36B 3SF 3WU 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51X 52M 52N 52O 52P 52S 52T 52U 52W 52X 53G 5GY 5RE 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A03 AAESR AAEVG AAHHS AANLZ AAONW AASGY AAXRX AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABEML ABIJN ABIVO ABPVW ACAHQ ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFS ACPOU ACPRK ACSCC ACXBN ACXQS ADBBV ADEOM ADIYS ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADOZA ADXAS ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEIGN AEIMD AENEX AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFFPM AFGKR AFPWT AFRAH AFZJQ AHBTC AHMBA AITYG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE AJXKR ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN AMBMR AMYDB ASPBG ATUGU AUFTA AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BHBCM BMNLL BMXJE BNHUX BROTX BRXPI BSCLL BY8 CS3 D-E D-F DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRSTM DVXWH EBS ECGQY EJD F00 F01 F04 F5P FEDTE G-S G.N GNP GODZA H.T H.X HBH HF~ HGLYW HHY HHZ HVGLF HZ~ IX1 J0M JPC KQQ LATKE LAW LC2 LC3 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES M66 MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRSTM MSFUL MSSTM MXFUL MXSTM N04 N05 N9A NF~ NNB O66 O9- OIG P2P P2W P2X P4D PALCI PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 QRW R.K RIWAO RJQFR ROL RWI RWV RX1 RYL SAMSI SUPJJ TN5 UB1 V2E VQA W8V W99 WBKPD WIB WIH WIK WJL WNSPC WOHZO WQJ WRC WTM WXSBR WYISQ XG1 XV2 ZCG ZZTAW ~02 ~IA ~WT AAHQN AAMNL AANHP AAYCA ACRPL ACYXJ ADNMO AFWVQ ALVPJ AAYXX AEYWJ AGHNM AGQPQ AGYGG CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7SN 7TK 8BJ 8FD AAMMB AEFGJ AGXDD AIDQK AIDYY C1K FQK FR3 JBE K9. P64 RC3 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c4618-9e1267dcd0b9c346070f3233cd58a11cbc1fd01a1b75b79a8e5939fbadc520453 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 0275-2565 1098-2345 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 09:38:32 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 10:30:33 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:05:56 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:36 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:53:16 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:37:00 EST 2025 Wed Oct 30 10:05:32 EDT 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 2 |
Keywords | anxiety callitrichid excitement scratching arousal |
Language | English |
License | http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4618-9e1267dcd0b9c346070f3233cd58a11cbc1fd01a1b75b79a8e5939fbadc520453 |
Notes | ArticleID:AJP22498 ark:/67375/WNG-TS67SX0K-K istex:08A439C206E7A6C9ADE800A114A34CA4DD6424E4 ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
PMID | 26530306 |
PQID | 1757722135 |
PQPubID | 866340 |
PageCount | 11 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_1760860592 proquest_journals_1757722135 pubmed_primary_26530306 crossref_primary_10_1002_ajp_22498 crossref_citationtrail_10_1002_ajp_22498 wiley_primary_10_1002_ajp_22498_AJP22498 istex_primary_ark_67375_WNG_TS67SX0K_K |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | February 2016 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-02-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2016 text: February 2016 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: New York |
PublicationTitle | American journal of primatology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Am. J. Primatol. |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | Blackwell Publishing Ltd Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
References | Braesicke K, Parkinson JA, Reekir Y, et al. 2005. Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis. European Journal of Neuroscience 21:1733-1740. Schlafani A, Norscia I, Antonacci D, Palagi E. 2012. Scratching around mating: factors affecting anxiety in wild Lemur catta. Primates 53:247-254. Mellou E. 2006. Play theories: a contemporary review. Early Child Development and Care 102:91-100. Polizzi di Sorrentino E, Schino G, Tiddi B, Aureli F. 2012. Scratching as a window into the emotional responses of wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Ethology 118:1072-1084. Buckley V, Semple S. 2012. Evidence that displacement activities facilitate behavioural transitions in ring-tailed lemurs. Behavioural Processes 90:433-435. Omedes A. 1981. Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets Callithrix a. argentata and Callithrix a. melanura. Miscellania Zoologica 7:193-211. Castles DL, Whiten A. 1998. Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. II. Stress and self-directed behaviour. Ethology 104:148-160. Barros M, Giorgetti M, Souto AAV, et al. 2007. Persistent anxiety-like behavior in marmosets following a recent predatory stress condition: reversal by diazepam. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 84:705-711. Maestripieri D. 1993. Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) I. Measurement of anxiety and identification of anxiety-eliciting situations. Ethology 95:19-31. Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Schino G. 1991. Conflict, displacement activities, and menstrual cycle in long-tailed macaques. Primates 32:115-118. Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2002. Anxiolytic-like effects of Substance P fragment (SP1-7) in non-human primates (Callithrix jacchus). Peptides 23:967-973. Aureli F, van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM. 1989. Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology 19:39-51. Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M. 1989. On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiology & Behavior 45:667-670. Aureli F. 1997. Post-conflict anxiety in non-human primates: the mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution. Aggressive Behavior 23:315-328. Judge PG, Griffaton NS, Fincke AM. 2006. Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) during crowding: a tension-reduction strategy. American Journal of Primatology 68:993-1006. Judge PG, Evans DW, Schroepfer KK, Gross AC. 2011. Perseveration on a reversal-learning task correlates with rates of self-directed behavior in nonhuman primates. Behavioural Brain Research 222:57-65. Nakayama K. 2004. Observing conspecifics scratching induces a contagion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Journal of Comparative Psychology 118:20-24. Judge PG, Bachman KA. 2013. Witnessing reconciliation reduces arousal of bystanders in a baboon group (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). Animal Behaviour 85:881-889. Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD, Hyatt CW. 2001. Effects of cognitive challenge on self-directed behaviors by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 55:1-14. Waldstein SR, Kop WJ, Schmidt LA, et al. 2000. Frontal electrocortical and cardiovascular reactivity during happiness and anger. Biological Psychology 55:3-23. Smith TE, McGreer-Whitworth B, French JA. 1998. Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel-cage housing in black tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Hormones and Behavior 34:211-222. Norscia I, Palagi E. 2011. When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. Primates 52:101-104. Ebner K, Singewald N. 2006. The role of substance P in stress and anxiety responses. Amino Acids 31:251-272. Held SDE, Spinka M. 2011. Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81:891-899. Kaburu SSK, MacLarnon A, Majolo B, Qarro M, Semple S. 2012. Dominance rank and self-scratching among wild female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). African Zoololgy 47:74-79. Aureli F, Yates K. 2010. Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques. Biology Letters 6:27-29. Cross N, Rogers LJ. 2006. Mobbing vocalizations as a coping response in the common marmoset. Hormones and Behavior 49:237-245. Manson JH, Perry S. 2000. Correlates of self-directed behaviour in wild white-faced Capuchins. Ethology 106:301-317. Silva IO, Alvarenga ABB, Boere V. 2008. Occasional field observations of the predation on mice, dove and ants by black-tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix penicillata). Neotropical Primates 15:59-62. Cilia J, Piper DC. 1996. Marmoset conspecific confrontation: an ethologically-based model of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 58:85-91. Bassett L, Buchanan-Smith HM, McKinley J, Smith T. 2003. Effects of training on stress-related behavior of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping with routine husbandry procedures. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6:221-233. Mootnick AR, Cunningham C, Baker E. 2012. Phylogenetic separation in limb use in captive gibbons (Hylobatidae): a comparison across the primate order. American Journal of Primatology 74:1035-1043. Waitt C, Buchanan-Smith HM. 2001. What time is feeding?: how delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump-tailed macaque behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 75:75-85. Troisi A, Schino G, D'Antoni M, et al. 1991. Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers. Behavioral and Neural Biology 56:307-313. Easley SP, Coelho AM Jr., Taylor LL. 1987. Scratching dominance, tension, and displacement in male baboons. American Journal of Primatology 13:397-411. Engel SR, Creson TK, Hao Y, et al. 2009. The extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway contributed to the control of behavioral excitement. Molecular Psychiatry 14:448-461. Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2004. Multibehavioral analysis of fear and anxiety before, during, and after experimentally induced predatory stress in Callithrix penicillata. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 78:357-367. Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A. 1991. A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Animal Behaviour 44:967-979. Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Freeman H, et al. 2006. Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal. Emotion 65:53-559. Semple S, Harrison C, Lehmann J. 2013. Grooming and anxiety in Barbary macaques. Ethology 119:779-785. Spinka M, Newberry RC, Bekoff M. 2001. Mammilian play: training for the unexpected. The Quarterly Review of Biology 76:141-168. Barros M, Boere V, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2000. Measuring fear and anxiety in the marmoset (Callithrix penicillata) with a novel predator confrontation model: effects of diazepam. Behavioural Brain Research 108:205-211. Dettling A, Pryce CR, Martin RD, Dobeli M. 1998. Physiological responses to parental separation and a strange situation are related to parental care received in juvenile Goeldi's monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Developmental Psychobiology 33:21-31. Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ. 1998. Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia 36:1265-1273. Schino G, Perretta G, Taglioni AM, Monaco V, Troisi A. 1996. Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety 2:186-191. Baker KC, Aureli F. 1997. Behavioural indicators of anxiety: an empirical test in chimpanzees. Behaviour 134:1031-1050. Saltzman W, Abbott DH. 2009. Effects of elevated circulating cortisol concentration on maternal behavior in common marmoset mothers (Callithrix jacchus). Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:1222-1234. Judge PG, Mullen SH. 2005. Quadratic post-conflict affiliation among bystanders in a hamadryas baboon group. Animal Behaviour 69:1345-1355. Schino G, Maestripieri D, Scucchi S, Turillazzi PG. 1990. Social tension in familiar and unfamiliar pairs of long-tailed macaques. Behaviour 113:264-272. O'Neill-Wagner PL, Bolig R, Price CS. 1994. Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups?. Communication and Cognition 27:261-272. de Boer RA, Overduin-de Vries AM, Louwerse AL, Sterck EHM. 2013. The behavioral context of visual displays in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology 75:1084-1095. Schwartz GE, Weinberger DA, Singer JA. 1981. Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine 43:343-364. Peignot P, Jankowski F, Anderson JR. 2004. On self-scratching in captive mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Folia Primatologica 75:160-164. Schino G, Troisi A, Perretta G, Monaco V. 1991. Measuring anxiety in non-human primates: effect of lorazepam on macaque scratching. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 38:889-891. Dettmer AM, Novak MA, Suomi SJ, Meyer JS. 2012. Physiological and behavioral adaptation to relocation stress in differentially reared rhesus monkeys: hair cortisol as a biomarker for anxiety-related responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:191-199. Pearson BL, Reeder D, Judge PG. 2015. Crowding increases salivary cortisol but not self-directed behavior in captive baboons. American Journal of Primatology 77:462-467. Diezinger F, Anderson JR. 1986. Starting from scratch: a first look at a "displacement activity" in group-living rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 11:117-124. Parr LA, Hopkins WD. 2000. Brain temperature asymmetries and emotional perception in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Physiology & Behavior 71:363-371. Kato Y, Gokan H, Oh-Nishi A, et al. 2014. Vocalizations associated with anxiety and fear in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Behavioural Brain Research 275:43-52. Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD. 2004. Behavioral evidence for the cutaneous expression of emotion in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Behavior 141:979-997. Palagi E, Norscia I. 2011. Scratching aro 1989; 45 2006; 31 1991; 56 1991; 57 2015; 77 1970; 33 2011; 52 2005; 21 1994; 27 2011; 14 1981; 43 2012; 53 2001; 42 2005; 69 2009; 14 2004; 75 1974; 40 2006; 68 2013; 119 2006; 65 1991; 44 2003; 6 2000; 55 2004; 78 1980 2001; 55 1996; 2 2010; 6 1987; 13 1991; 38 2010 1991; 32 1986; 11 2004; 141 2011; 81 1997; 23 1997; 134 2013; 85 1981; 7 2008; 15 2006 2000; 71 2008; 11 2012; 37 1999; 61 1996; 58 2014; 275 2012; 74 2014; 87 1999 2009; 34 2012; 90 1993; 95 2002; 23 2006; 49 2000; 106 2000; 108 2013; 75 1998; 104 2007; 84 2012; 47 1990; 113 2004; 118 1989; 19 2011; 222 2012; 118 1998; 34 1998; 33 1998; 36 2001; 75 2006; 102 2001; 76 e_1_2_6_51_1 e_1_2_6_74_1 e_1_2_6_53_1 e_1_2_6_32_1 e_1_2_6_70_1 e_1_2_6_30_1 e_1_2_6_72_1 O'Neill‐Wagner PL (e_1_2_6_55_1) 1994; 27 e_1_2_6_19_1 e_1_2_6_36_1 e_1_2_6_59_1 e_1_2_6_11_1 e_1_2_6_17_1 e_1_2_6_15_1 e_1_2_6_38_1 e_1_2_6_57_1 e_1_2_6_62_1 e_1_2_6_64_1 e_1_2_6_43_1 e_1_2_6_20_1 e_1_2_6_41_1 e_1_2_6_60_1 e_1_2_6_9_1 e_1_2_6_5_1 e_1_2_6_7_1 e_1_2_6_24_1 e_1_2_6_49_1 e_1_2_6_3_1 e_1_2_6_22_1 e_1_2_6_66_1 e_1_2_6_28_1 e_1_2_6_45_1 e_1_2_6_26_1 e_1_2_6_47_1 e_1_2_6_68_1 e_1_2_6_52_1 Omedes A (e_1_2_6_54_1) 1981; 7 e_1_2_6_73_1 e_1_2_6_75_1 e_1_2_6_10_1 e_1_2_6_31_1 e_1_2_6_50_1 e_1_2_6_71_1 Biben M (e_1_2_6_13_1) 1999 Hopkins WD (e_1_2_6_34_1) 2006; 65 e_1_2_6_14_1 e_1_2_6_35_1 e_1_2_6_12_1 e_1_2_6_33_1 e_1_2_6_18_1 e_1_2_6_39_1 e_1_2_6_56_1 e_1_2_6_16_1 e_1_2_6_37_1 e_1_2_6_58_1 e_1_2_6_63_1 e_1_2_6_42_1 e_1_2_6_65_1 e_1_2_6_21_1 e_1_2_6_40_1 e_1_2_6_61_1 e_1_2_6_8_1 e_1_2_6_4_1 e_1_2_6_6_1 e_1_2_6_25_1 e_1_2_6_48_1 e_1_2_6_23_1 e_1_2_6_2_1 e_1_2_6_29_1 e_1_2_6_44_1 e_1_2_6_67_1 e_1_2_6_27_1 e_1_2_6_46_1 e_1_2_6_69_1 |
References_xml | – reference: Peignot P, Jankowski F, Anderson JR. 2004. On self-scratching in captive mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Folia Primatologica 75:160-164. – reference: Judge PG, Evans DW, Schroepfer KK, Gross AC. 2011. Perseveration on a reversal-learning task correlates with rates of self-directed behavior in nonhuman primates. Behavioural Brain Research 222:57-65. – reference: Judge PG, Griffaton NS, Fincke AM. 2006. Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) during crowding: a tension-reduction strategy. American Journal of Primatology 68:993-1006. – reference: Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M. 1989. On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiology & Behavior 45:667-670. – reference: Hook-Costigan MA, Rogers LJ. 1998. Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia 36:1265-1273. – reference: Waitt C, Buchanan-Smith HM. 2001. What time is feeding?: how delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump-tailed macaque behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 75:75-85. – reference: Cross N, Rogers LJ. 2006. Mobbing vocalizations as a coping response in the common marmoset. Hormones and Behavior 49:237-245. – reference: Dettling A, Pryce CR, Martin RD, Dobeli M. 1998. Physiological responses to parental separation and a strange situation are related to parental care received in juvenile Goeldi's monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Developmental Psychobiology 33:21-31. – reference: Schino G, Perretta G, Taglioni AM, Monaco V, Troisi A. 1996. Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety 2:186-191. – reference: Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Freeman H, et al. 2006. Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal. Emotion 65:53-559. – reference: Barros M, Boere V, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2000. Measuring fear and anxiety in the marmoset (Callithrix penicillata) with a novel predator confrontation model: effects of diazepam. Behavioural Brain Research 108:205-211. – reference: Polizzi di Sorrentino E, Schino G, Tiddi B, Aureli F. 2012. Scratching as a window into the emotional responses of wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Ethology 118:1072-1084. – reference: Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Schino G. 1991. Conflict, displacement activities, and menstrual cycle in long-tailed macaques. Primates 32:115-118. – reference: Bassett L, Buchanan-Smith HM, McKinley J, Smith T. 2003. Effects of training on stress-related behavior of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping with routine husbandry procedures. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6:221-233. – reference: Duboscq J, Agil M, Engelhardt A, Thierry B. 2014. The function of postconflict interactions: new prospects from the study of a tolerant species of primate. Animal Behaviour 87:107-120. – reference: D'Anna KL, Hoffman MC, Zerbe GO, et al. 2012. Acculturation, maternal cortisol release and birth outcomes in women of Mexican descent. Psychosomatic Medicine 74:296-304. – reference: Baldwin JD, Baldwin JI. 1974. Effects of food ecology on social play: a laboratory simulation. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 40:1-14. – reference: Barros M, Giorgetti M, Souto AAV, et al. 2007. Persistent anxiety-like behavior in marmosets following a recent predatory stress condition: reversal by diazepam. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 84:705-711. – reference: Castles DL, Whiten A. 1998. Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. II. Stress and self-directed behaviour. Ethology 104:148-160. – reference: Nakayama K. 2004. Observing conspecifics scratching induces a contagion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Journal of Comparative Psychology 118:20-24. – reference: O'Neill-Wagner PL, Bolig R, Price CS. 1994. Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups?. Communication and Cognition 27:261-272. – reference: Braesicke K, Parkinson JA, Reekir Y, et al. 2005. Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis. European Journal of Neuroscience 21:1733-1740. – reference: Palagi E, Norscia I. 2011. Scratching around stress: hierarchy and reconciliation make the difference in wild brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus). Stress 14:93-97. – reference: Mootnick AR, Cunningham C, Baker E. 2012. Phylogenetic separation in limb use in captive gibbons (Hylobatidae): a comparison across the primate order. American Journal of Primatology 74:1035-1043. – reference: Kaburu SSK, MacLarnon A, Majolo B, Qarro M, Semple S. 2012. Dominance rank and self-scratching among wild female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). African Zoololgy 47:74-79. – reference: Schino G, Troisi A, Perretta G, Monaco V. 1991. Measuring anxiety in non-human primates: effect of lorazepam on macaque scratching. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior 38:889-891. – reference: Smith TE, McGreer-Whitworth B, French JA. 1998. Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel-cage housing in black tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Hormones and Behavior 34:211-222. – reference: Manson JH, Perry S. 2000. Correlates of self-directed behaviour in wild white-faced Capuchins. Ethology 106:301-317. – reference: Clara E, Tommasi L, Rogers LJ. 2008. Social mobbing calls in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): effects of experience and associated cortisol levels. Animal Cognition 11:349-358. – reference: Parr LA, Hopkins WD. 2000. Brain temperature asymmetries and emotional perception in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Physiology & Behavior 71:363-371. – reference: Silva IO, Alvarenga ABB, Boere V. 2008. Occasional field observations of the predation on mice, dove and ants by black-tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix penicillata). Neotropical Primates 15:59-62. – reference: Semple S, Harrison C, Lehmann J. 2013. Grooming and anxiety in Barbary macaques. Ethology 119:779-785. – reference: Buckley V, Semple S. 2012. Evidence that displacement activities facilitate behavioural transitions in ring-tailed lemurs. Behavioural Processes 90:433-435. – reference: Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2002. Anxiolytic-like effects of Substance P fragment (SP1-7) in non-human primates (Callithrix jacchus). Peptides 23:967-973. – reference: Baker KC, Aureli F. 1997. Behavioural indicators of anxiety: an empirical test in chimpanzees. Behaviour 134:1031-1050. – reference: Dettmer AM, Novak MA, Suomi SJ, Meyer JS. 2012. Physiological and behavioral adaptation to relocation stress in differentially reared rhesus monkeys: hair cortisol as a biomarker for anxiety-related responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37:191-199. – reference: Loy J. 1970. Behavioral responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 33:263-271. – reference: Jacob RG, Thayer JF, Manuck SB, et al. 1999. Ambulatory blood pressure responses and the circumplex model of mood: a 4-day study. Psychosomatic Medicine 61:319-333. – reference: Judge PG, Bachman KA. 2013. Witnessing reconciliation reduces arousal of bystanders in a baboon group (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). Animal Behaviour 85:881-889. – reference: Easley SP, Coelho AM Jr., Taylor LL. 1987. Scratching dominance, tension, and displacement in male baboons. American Journal of Primatology 13:397-411. – reference: Omedes A. 1981. Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets Callithrix a. argentata and Callithrix a. melanura. Miscellania Zoologica 7:193-211. – reference: Pearson BL, Reeder D, Judge PG. 2015. Crowding increases salivary cortisol but not self-directed behavior in captive baboons. American Journal of Primatology 77:462-467. – reference: Aureli F. 1997. Post-conflict anxiety in non-human primates: the mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution. Aggressive Behavior 23:315-328. – reference: Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A. 1991. A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Animal Behaviour 44:967-979. – reference: Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD. 2004. Behavioral evidence for the cutaneous expression of emotion in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Behavior 141:979-997. – reference: de Boer RA, Overduin-de Vries AM, Louwerse AL, Sterck EHM. 2013. The behavioral context of visual displays in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology 75:1084-1095. – reference: Diezinger F, Anderson JR. 1986. Starting from scratch: a first look at a "displacement activity" in group-living rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 11:117-124. – reference: Aureli F, Yates K. 2010. Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques. Biology Letters 6:27-29. – reference: Judge PG, Mullen SH. 2005. Quadratic post-conflict affiliation among bystanders in a hamadryas baboon group. Animal Behaviour 69:1345-1355. – reference: Mellou E. 2006. Play theories: a contemporary review. Early Child Development and Care 102:91-100. – reference: Schino G, Maestripieri D, Scucchi S, Turillazzi PG. 1990. Social tension in familiar and unfamiliar pairs of long-tailed macaques. Behaviour 113:264-272. – reference: Waldstein SR, Kop WJ, Schmidt LA, et al. 2000. Frontal electrocortical and cardiovascular reactivity during happiness and anger. Biological Psychology 55:3-23. – reference: Schlafani A, Norscia I, Antonacci D, Palagi E. 2012. Scratching around mating: factors affecting anxiety in wild Lemur catta. Primates 53:247-254. – reference: Norscia I, Palagi E. 2011. When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets. Primates 52:101-104. – reference: Barros M, de Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Tomaz C. 2004. Multibehavioral analysis of fear and anxiety before, during, and after experimentally induced predatory stress in Callithrix penicillata. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 78:357-367. – reference: Kato Y, Gokan H, Oh-Nishi A, et al. 2014. Vocalizations associated with anxiety and fear in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Behavioural Brain Research 275:43-52. – reference: Engel SR, Creson TK, Hao Y, et al. 2009. The extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway contributed to the control of behavioral excitement. Molecular Psychiatry 14:448-461. – reference: Saltzman W, Abbott DH. 2009. Effects of elevated circulating cortisol concentration on maternal behavior in common marmoset mothers (Callithrix jacchus). Psychoneuroendocrinology 34:1222-1234. – reference: Aureli F, van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM. 1989. Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology 19:39-51. – reference: Maestripieri D. 1993. Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) I. Measurement of anxiety and identification of anxiety-eliciting situations. Ethology 95:19-31. – reference: Held SDE, Spinka M. 2011. Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81:891-899. – reference: Cilia J, Piper DC. 1996. Marmoset conspecific confrontation: an ethologically-based model of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 58:85-91. – reference: Elder CM, Menzel CR. 2001. Dissociation of cortisol and behavioral indicators of stress in an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) during a computerized task. Primates 42:345-357. – reference: Spinka M, Newberry RC, Bekoff M. 2001. Mammilian play: training for the unexpected. The Quarterly Review of Biology 76:141-168. – reference: Schwartz GE, Weinberger DA, Singer JA. 1981. Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine 43:343-364. – reference: Pavani S, Maestripieri D, Schino G, Turillazzi PG, Scucchi S. 1991. Factors influencing scratching behaviour in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Folia Primatologica 57:34-38. – reference: Leavens DA, Aureli F, Hopkins WD, Hyatt CW. 2001. Effects of cognitive challenge on self-directed behaviors by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 55:1-14. – reference: Ebner K, Singewald N. 2006. The role of substance P in stress and anxiety responses. Amino Acids 31:251-272. – reference: Troisi A, Schino G, D'Antoni M, et al. 1991. Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers. Behavioral and Neural Biology 56:307-313. – volume: 113 start-page: 264 year: 1990 end-page: 272 article-title: Social tension in familiar and unfamiliar pairs of long‐tailed macaques publication-title: Behaviour – volume: 44 start-page: 967 year: 1991 end-page: 979 article-title: A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates publication-title: Animal Behaviour – volume: 15 start-page: 59 year: 2008 end-page: 62 article-title: Occasional field observations of the predation on mice, dove and ants by black‐tufted‐ear marmosets ( ) publication-title: Neotropical Primates – volume: 14 start-page: 448 year: 2009 end-page: 461 article-title: The extracellular signal‐regulated kinase pathway contributed to the control of behavioral excitement publication-title: Molecular Psychiatry – start-page: 191 year: 1999 end-page: 208 – volume: 75 start-page: 1084 year: 2013 end-page: 1095 article-title: The behavioral context of visual displays in common marmosets ( ) publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 36 start-page: 1265 year: 1998 end-page: 1273 article-title: Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 104 start-page: 148 year: 1998 end-page: 160 article-title: Post‐conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons. II. Stress and self‐directed behaviour publication-title: Ethology – volume: 106 start-page: 301 year: 2000 end-page: 317 article-title: Correlates of self‐directed behaviour in wild white‐faced Capuchins publication-title: Ethology – volume: 108 start-page: 205 year: 2000 end-page: 211 article-title: Measuring fear and anxiety in the marmoset ( ) with a novel predator confrontation model: effects of diazepam publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research – volume: 85 start-page: 881 year: 2013 end-page: 889 article-title: Witnessing reconciliation reduces arousal of bystanders in a baboon group ( ) publication-title: Animal Behaviour – volume: 68 start-page: 993 year: 2006 end-page: 1006 article-title: Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons ( during crowding: a tension‐reduction strategy publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 21 start-page: 1733 year: 2005 end-page: 1740 article-title: Autonomic arousal in an appetitive context in primates: a behavioural and neural analysis publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 55 start-page: 3 year: 2000 end-page: 23 article-title: Frontal electrocortical and cardiovascular reactivity during happiness and anger publication-title: Biological Psychology – volume: 275 start-page: 43 year: 2014 end-page: 52 article-title: Vocalizations associated with anxiety and fear in the common marmoset ( ) publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research – volume: 23 start-page: 315 year: 1997 end-page: 328 article-title: Post‐conflict anxiety in non‐human primates: the mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution publication-title: Aggressive Behavior – volume: 23 start-page: 967 year: 2002 end-page: 973 article-title: Anxiolytic‐like effects of Substance P fragment (SP ) in non‐human primates ( ) publication-title: Peptides – volume: 74 start-page: 296 year: 2012 end-page: 304 article-title: Acculturation, maternal cortisol release and birth outcomes in women of Mexican descent publication-title: Psychosomatic Medicine – volume: 13 start-page: 397 year: 1987 end-page: 411 article-title: Scratching dominance, tension, and displacement in male baboons publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 31 start-page: 251 year: 2006 end-page: 272 article-title: The role of substance P in stress and anxiety responses publication-title: Amino Acids – volume: 11 start-page: 117 year: 1986 end-page: 124 article-title: Starting from scratch: a first look at a “displacement activity” in group‐living rhesus monkeys publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 222 start-page: 57 year: 2011 end-page: 65 article-title: Perseveration on a reversal‐learning task correlates with rates of self‐directed behavior in nonhuman primates publication-title: Behavioural Brain Research – volume: 47 start-page: 74 year: 2012 end-page: 79 article-title: Dominance rank and self‐scratching among wild female Barbary macaques ( ) publication-title: African Zoololgy – volume: 57 start-page: 34 year: 1991 end-page: 38 article-title: Factors influencing scratching behaviour in long‐tailed macaques ( ) publication-title: Folia Primatologica – volume: 75 start-page: 160 year: 2004 end-page: 164 article-title: On self‐scratching in captive mandrills ( ) publication-title: Folia Primatologica – volume: 32 start-page: 115 year: 1991 end-page: 118 article-title: Conflict, displacement activities, and menstrual cycle in long‐tailed macaques publication-title: Primates – volume: 141 start-page: 979 year: 2004 end-page: 997 article-title: Behavioral evidence for the cutaneous expression of emotion in a chimpanzee ( ) publication-title: Behavior – volume: 69 start-page: 1345 year: 2005 end-page: 1355 article-title: Quadratic post‐conflict affiliation among bystanders in a hamadryas baboon group publication-title: Animal Behaviour – volume: 74 start-page: 1035 year: 2012 end-page: 1043 article-title: Phylogenetic separation in limb use in captive gibbons ( ): a comparison across the primate order publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 14 start-page: 93 year: 2011 end-page: 97 article-title: Scratching around stress: hierarchy and reconciliation make the difference in wild brown lemurs ( ) publication-title: Stress – volume: 119 start-page: 779 year: 2013 end-page: 785 article-title: Grooming and anxiety in Barbary macaques publication-title: Ethology – volume: 65 start-page: 53 year: 2006 end-page: 559 article-title: Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees ( ): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal publication-title: Emotion – volume: 102 start-page: 91 year: 2006 end-page: 100 article-title: Play theories: a contemporary review publication-title: Early Child Development and Care – volume: 34 start-page: 211 year: 1998 end-page: 222 article-title: Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel‐cage housing in black tufted‐ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli) publication-title: Hormones and Behavior – volume: 77 start-page: 462 year: 2015 end-page: 467 article-title: Crowding increases salivary cortisol but not self‐directed behavior in captive baboons publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 118 start-page: 1072 year: 2012 end-page: 1084 article-title: Scratching as a window into the emotional responses of wild tufted capuchin monkeys publication-title: Ethology – volume: 43 start-page: 343 year: 1981 end-page: 364 article-title: Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise publication-title: Psychosomatic Medicine – start-page: 471 year: 1980 end-page: 495 – volume: 58 start-page: 85 year: 1996 end-page: 91 article-title: Marmoset conspecific confrontation: an ethologically‐based model of anxiety publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior – volume: 52 start-page: 101 year: 2011 end-page: 104 article-title: When play is a family business: adult play, hierarchy, and possible stress reduction in common marmosets publication-title: Primates – volume: 49 start-page: 237 year: 2006 end-page: 245 article-title: Mobbing vocalizations as a coping response in the common marmoset publication-title: Hormones and Behavior – volume: 42 start-page: 345 year: 2001 end-page: 357 article-title: Dissociation of cortisol and behavioral indicators of stress in an orangutan ( ) during a computerized task publication-title: Primates – volume: 27 start-page: 261 year: 1994 end-page: 272 article-title: Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups publication-title: Communication and Cognition – volume: 37 start-page: 191 year: 2012 end-page: 199 article-title: Physiological and behavioral adaptation to relocation stress in differentially reared rhesus monkeys: hair cortisol as a biomarker for anxiety‐related responses publication-title: Psychoneuroendocrinology – volume: 87 start-page: 107 year: 2014 end-page: 120 article-title: The function of postconflict interactions: new prospects from the study of a tolerant species of primate publication-title: Animal Behaviour – volume: 81 start-page: 891 year: 2011 end-page: 899 article-title: Animal play and animal welfare publication-title: Animal Behaviour – volume: 40 start-page: 1 year: 1974 end-page: 14 article-title: Effects of food ecology on social play: a laboratory simulation publication-title: Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie – volume: 134 start-page: 1031 year: 1997 end-page: 1050 article-title: Behavioural indicators of anxiety: an empirical test in chimpanzees publication-title: Behaviour – volume: 76 start-page: 141 year: 2001 end-page: 168 article-title: Mammilian play: training for the unexpected publication-title: The Quarterly Review of Biology – volume: 118 start-page: 20 year: 2004 end-page: 24 article-title: Observing conspecifics scratching induces a contagion of scratching in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) publication-title: Journal of Comparative Psychology – year: 2010 – volume: 90 start-page: 433 year: 2012 end-page: 435 article-title: Evidence that displacement activities facilitate behavioural transitions in ring‐tailed lemurs publication-title: Behavioural Processes – volume: 61 start-page: 319 year: 1999 end-page: 333 article-title: Ambulatory blood pressure responses and the circumplex model of mood: a 4‐day study publication-title: Psychosomatic Medicine – volume: 19 start-page: 39 year: 1989 end-page: 51 article-title: Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long‐tailed macaques ( ) publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 53 start-page: 247 year: 2012 end-page: 254 article-title: Scratching around mating: factors affecting anxiety in wild publication-title: Primates – volume: 11 start-page: 349 year: 2008 end-page: 358 article-title: Social mobbing calls in common marmosets ( ): effects of experience and associated cortisol levels publication-title: Animal Cognition – volume: 56 start-page: 307 year: 1991 end-page: 313 article-title: Scratching as a behavioral index of anxiety in macaque mothers publication-title: Behavioral and Neural Biology – volume: 33 start-page: 21 year: 1998 end-page: 31 article-title: Physiological responses to parental separation and a strange situation are related to parental care received in juvenile Goeldi's monkeys ( ) publication-title: Developmental Psychobiology – volume: 55 start-page: 1 year: 2001 end-page: 14 article-title: Effects of cognitive challenge on self‐directed behaviors by chimpanzees ( ) publication-title: American Journal of Primatology – volume: 71 start-page: 363 year: 2000 end-page: 371 article-title: Brain temperature asymmetries and emotional perception in chimpanzees, publication-title: Physiology & Behavior – volume: 7 start-page: 193 year: 1981 end-page: 211 article-title: Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets and publication-title: Miscellania Zoologica – year: 2006 – volume: 6 start-page: 27 year: 2010 end-page: 29 article-title: Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques publication-title: Biology Letters – volume: 84 start-page: 705 year: 2007 end-page: 711 article-title: Persistent anxiety‐like behavior in marmosets following a recent predatory stress condition: reversal by diazepam publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior – volume: 34 start-page: 1222 year: 2009 end-page: 1234 article-title: Effects of elevated circulating cortisol concentration on maternal behavior in common marmoset mothers ( ) publication-title: Psychoneuroendocrinology – volume: 6 start-page: 221 year: 2003 end-page: 233 article-title: Effects of training on stress‐related behavior of the common marmoset ( ) in relation to coping with routine husbandry procedures publication-title: Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science – volume: 38 start-page: 889 year: 1991 end-page: 891 article-title: Measuring anxiety in non‐human primates: effect of lorazepam on macaque scratching publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior – volume: 75 start-page: 75 year: 2001 end-page: 85 article-title: What time is feeding?: how delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump‐tailed macaque behavior publication-title: Applied Animal Behaviour Science – volume: 95 start-page: 19 year: 1993 end-page: 31 article-title: Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques ( ) I. Measurement of anxiety and identification of anxiety‐eliciting situations publication-title: Ethology – volume: 33 start-page: 263 year: 1970 end-page: 271 article-title: Behavioral responses of free‐ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage publication-title: American Journal of Physical Anthropology – volume: 2 start-page: 186 year: 1996 end-page: 191 article-title: Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety publication-title: Anxiety – volume: 78 start-page: 357 year: 2004 end-page: 367 article-title: Multibehavioral analysis of fear and anxiety before, during, and after experimentally induced predatory stress in publication-title: Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior – volume: 45 start-page: 667 year: 1989 end-page: 670 article-title: On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque publication-title: Physiology & Behavior – ident: e_1_2_6_28_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350130405 – ident: e_1_2_6_57_1 doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00349-8 – ident: e_1_2_6_75_1 doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00065-X – ident: e_1_2_6_31_1 doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002135 – ident: e_1_2_6_56_1 doi: 10.3109/10253890.2010.505272 – ident: e_1_2_6_2_1 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1997)23:5<315::AID-AB2>3.0.CO;2-H – ident: e_1_2_6_27_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.10.018 – ident: e_1_2_6_21_1 doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.007 – ident: e_1_2_6_58_1 doi: 10.1159/000156561 – ident: e_1_2_6_70_1 doi: 10.1896/044.015.0209 – ident: e_1_2_6_20_1 doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0125-0 – ident: e_1_2_6_36_1 – ident: e_1_2_6_39_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.20290 – ident: e_1_2_6_23_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.22167 – ident: e_1_2_6_8_1 doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00153-9 – ident: e_1_2_6_68_1 doi: 10.1007/BF02381606 – ident: e_1_2_6_73_1 doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(91)90469-7 – ident: e_1_2_6_22_1 doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318244fbde – ident: e_1_2_6_42_1 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.047 – ident: e_1_2_6_59_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.22363 – ident: e_1_2_6_60_1 doi: 10.1159/000078307 – ident: e_1_2_6_38_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.016 – ident: e_1_2_6_30_1 doi: 10.1007/BF02629625 – ident: e_1_2_6_49_1 doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00527.x – ident: e_1_2_6_18_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00058.x – volume: 65 start-page: 53 year: 2006 ident: e_1_2_6_34_1 article-title: Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal publication-title: Emotion – ident: e_1_2_6_10_1 doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.04.008 – ident: e_1_2_6_71_1 doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1469 – ident: e_1_2_6_17_1 doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.04.009 – ident: e_1_2_6_37_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.02.011 – ident: e_1_2_6_66_1 doi: 10.1097/00006842-198108000-00007 – ident: e_1_2_6_7_1 doi: 10.1163/156853997X00386 – ident: e_1_2_6_52_1 doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.20 – start-page: 191 volume-title: Saimiri year: 1999 ident: e_1_2_6_13_1 – ident: e_1_2_6_53_1 doi: 10.1007/s10329-010-0228-0 – ident: e_1_2_6_14_1 doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90089-9 – ident: e_1_2_6_32_1 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007 – ident: e_1_2_6_15_1 – ident: e_1_2_6_41_1 doi: 10.3377/004.047.0111 – ident: e_1_2_6_44_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.1034 – ident: e_1_2_6_43_1 doi: 10.1146/annurev.an.09.100180.002351 – ident: e_1_2_6_19_1 doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00376-0 – ident: e_1_2_6_4_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350190105 – ident: e_1_2_6_12_1 doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_07 – ident: e_1_2_6_45_1 doi: 10.1163/1568539042360189 – ident: e_1_2_6_48_1 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80592-5 – ident: e_1_2_6_50_1 doi: 10.1080/0300443941020107 – ident: e_1_2_6_72_1 doi: 10.1086/393866 – ident: e_1_2_6_24_1 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199807)33:1<21::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-U – ident: e_1_2_6_40_1 doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.016 – ident: e_1_2_6_6_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1976.tb00922.x – ident: e_1_2_6_74_1 doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00174-5 – volume: 7 start-page: 193 year: 1981 ident: e_1_2_6_54_1 article-title: Behavioural repertoire of adult silvery marmosets Callithrix a. argentata and Callithrix a. melanura publication-title: Miscellania Zoologica – ident: e_1_2_6_62_1 doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.012 – ident: e_1_2_6_63_1 doi: 10.1163/156853990X00518 – ident: e_1_2_6_46_1 doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330330212 – ident: e_1_2_6_11_1 doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.02.016 – ident: e_1_2_6_64_1 doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90258-4 – ident: e_1_2_6_3_1 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0513 – ident: e_1_2_6_29_1 doi: 10.1007/s00726-006-0335-9 – ident: e_1_2_6_51_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.22057 – ident: e_1_2_6_65_1 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-7154(1996)2:4<186::AID-ANXI5>3.0.CO;2-M – ident: e_1_2_6_9_1 doi: 10.1016/S0196-9781(02)00020-7 – ident: e_1_2_6_67_1 doi: 10.1007/s10329-012-0294-6 – ident: e_1_2_6_5_1 – ident: e_1_2_6_61_1 doi: 10.1111/eth.12008 – ident: e_1_2_6_33_1 doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00037-2 – ident: e_1_2_6_47_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00453.x – ident: e_1_2_6_25_1 doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.06.003 – ident: e_1_2_6_16_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03987.x – ident: e_1_2_6_35_1 doi: 10.1097/00006842-199905000-00011 – ident: e_1_2_6_69_1 doi: 10.1111/eth.12119 – volume: 27 start-page: 261 year: 1994 ident: e_1_2_6_55_1 article-title: Do play activity levels tell us something about psychosocial welfare in captive monkey groups publication-title: Communication and Cognition – ident: e_1_2_6_26_1 doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350110204 |
SSID | ssj0011465 |
Score | 2.2073786 |
Snippet | Scratching has been widely used as an indicator of anxiety in many primate species. However, a handful of studies have shown no change in scratching under... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref wiley istex |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 216 |
SubjectTerms | Animal behavior Animals Anxiety Arousal Blood pressure Callithrix - physiology callitrichid Correlation analysis excitement Female Heart rate Male Monkeys & apes scratching Studies |
Title | Scratching under positive and negative arousal in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) |
URI | https://api.istex.fr/ark:/67375/WNG-TS67SX0K-K/fulltext.pdf https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002%2Fajp.22498 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26530306 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1757722135 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1760860592 |
Volume | 78 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1ba9VAEB5KRSiCl3qLVllFpD7kNNlkdxt8KsVaWiziaelBhGVvsdeckpwj1V_v7OailQriW0ImyV5mdr5JZr4FeCUSxX3-YGy5yOJcWxprRou4RGfstEaEn_t65w97fPsg35mwyQK87WthWn6I4YObt4ywXnsDV7pZ-0Uaqk4uRuh_Cl_o63O1PCD6NFBH-WLbkL5IBYvRrbOeVSiha8OdV3zRDT-sl9cBzau4NTierTvwpW9ym29yOprP9Mj8-IPN8T_7dBdud4CUbLQadA8WXLUMt37bP-H7MiwNyySe3Pw8DUf34XBsUH9CMibxtWg1aVPAvjmiKksq91W1J_V03uA7jiuCDUK9J-eqPp82btaQ1U11hpHAUX18SU6UMUfz5s0DONh6t7-5HXcbNcQm5ykumC6lXFhjE12YLOe4jJQZzTJj2bpKU6NNWtokVakWTItCrTtWZEWplTXM0-FnD2GxmlbuMZDccWdzxaznyadGKJobV-JThKYUQ7MIVvspk6ZjMfebaZzJln-ZShxDGcYwgpeD6EVL3XGd0Osw74OEqk99rptg8nDvvdwfczGeJLtyN4KVXjFkZ-aNROyF0QlNMxbBi-EyGqj_66Iqh6OLMhzDRkSxNIJHrUINL6OcZT5ow14Ftfh7O-XGzsdw8OTfRZ_CEsK7Lsd8BRZn9dw9Qwg108-DrfwE4UsVkg |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3rb9MwED-NTYgJicdgIzDAIITGh3SJEyeNxJdpYpR1qxDttAoJWX6VPdMpadHgr-fsPGBoSIhvtnKJ7fOd7845_wzwKg1EYvMHfZ2kkR9LTX3JaOZP0BgbKdHDj-155_1B0juId8dsvABvm7MwFT5Eu-FmNcOt11bB7Yb05i_UUHFy0UEDlHVvwJK90dsFVJ9a8Ch73NYlMNKU-WjYWYMrFNDN9tUr1mjJMvbyOlfzqufqTM_OXfjSdLrKODntzGeyo378gef4v6O6B3dqn5RsVUJ0HxZMvgK3f7tC4fsKLLcrJVZufp660gM4HCoUIZePSexxtIJUWWDfDBG5Jrn5KqpKMZ2X2MZxTrBHKPrkXBTn09LMSrKxLc4wGDgqji_JiVDqaF6-eQgHO-9G2z2_vqvBV3ES4pppQpqkWulAZiqKE1xJJhGNIqVZV4Shkiqc6CAUoUyZTDPRNSyLsokUWjGLiB-twmI-zc0jILFJjI4F0xYqn6pU0FiZCX4llZRidObBRjNnXNVA5vY-jTNeQTBTjjzkjocevGxJLyr0juuIXruJbylEcWrT3VLGDwfv-WiYpMNx0Od9D9YbyeC1ppcc3S8MUGgYMQ9etI9RR-2PF5Eb5C7SJBg5oiNLPVirJKptjCYssnEbjsrJxd_7ybd2P7rC438nfQ63eqP9Pb73YdB_Asvo7dUp5-uwOCvm5il6VDP5zCnOTykjGa0 |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3rb9MwED-NTaAJicd4BQYEhND2IV3ixHEjPk0bZaxQTXTTKoRk-RX2TKu0RYO_nrPzgKEhIb7ZyiWxz3e-u-TuZ4BXLBSpzR8MdMriIJGaBJKSLMjRGBsp0cNPbL3zx0G6c5DsjuhoAd40tTAVPkT7wc1qhtuvrYJPdL7xCzRUnEw6aH-y7jVYStKwa0V6-1OLHWWrbV3-ImE0QLtOG1ihkGy0t14yRkuWrxdXeZqXHVdneXq34Usz5irh5LQzn8mO-vEHnON_TuoO3Ko9Un-zEqG7sGCKFbj52wEK31dgud0nsXP989i17sHhUKEAuWxM3xajlX6VA_bN-KLQfmG-iqpTjudTfMdx4eOAUPD9c1Gej6dmNvXXtsQZhgJH5fGFfyKUOppP1-_DQe_t_tZOUJ_UEKgkjXDHNBFJmVY6lJmKcTVYmMckjpWmXRFFSqoo12EkIsmoZJnoGprFWS6FVtTi4ccPYLEYF-YR-IlJjU4E1RYonygmSKJMjk9hkhCMzTxYa5aMqxrG3J6mccYrAGbCkYfc8dCDly3ppMLuuIrotVv3lkKUpzbZjVF-OHjH94cpG47CPu97sNoIBq_1fMrR-cLwhEQx9eBFexk11P52EYVB7iINSidGjRnx4GElUO3LSEpjG7XhrJxY_H2cfHN3zzUe_zvpc7ixt93jH94P-k9gGV29Ot98FRZn5dw8RXdqJp85tfkJ5lQYZQ |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Scratching+under+positive+and+negative+arousal+in+common+marmosets+%28Callithrix+jacchus%29&rft.jtitle=American+journal+of+primatology&rft.au=Neal%2C+Sarah+J&rft.au=Caine%2C+Nancy+G&rft.date=2016-02-01&rft.eissn=1098-2345&rft.volume=78&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=216&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002%2Fajp.22498&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F26530306&rft.externalDocID=26530306 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0275-2565&client=summon |