The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”?

If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired [1, 2]. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face recognition, it has been suggested that the inversion effect in biological motion is due to impaired configural processing in a highly tra...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inCurrent biology Vol. 16; no. 8; pp. 821 - 824
Main Authors Troje, Nikolaus F., Westhoff, Cord
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published England Elsevier Inc 18.04.2006
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0960-9822
1879-0445
DOI10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022

Cover

Loading…
Abstract If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired [1, 2]. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face recognition, it has been suggested that the inversion effect in biological motion is due to impaired configural processing in a highly trained expert system [3–5]. Here, we present data that are incompatible with this view. We show that observers can readily retrieve information about direction from scrambled point-light displays of humans and animals. Even though all configural information is entirely disrupted, perception of these displays is still subject to a significant inversion effect. Inverting only parts of the display reveals that the information about direction, as well as the associated inversion effect, is entirely carried by the local motion of the feet. We interpret our findings in terms of a visual filter that is tuned to the characteristic motion of the limbs of an animal in locomotion and hypothesize that this mechanism serves as a general detection system for the presence of articulated terrestrial animals.
AbstractList If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired [1, 2]. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face recognition, it has been suggested that the inversion effect in biological motion is due to impaired configural processing in a highly trained expert system [3–5]. Here, we present data that are incompatible with this view. We show that observers can readily retrieve information about direction from scrambled point-light displays of humans and animals. Even though all configural information is entirely disrupted, perception of these displays is still subject to a significant inversion effect. Inverting only parts of the display reveals that the information about direction, as well as the associated inversion effect, is entirely carried by the local motion of the feet. We interpret our findings in terms of a visual filter that is tuned to the characteristic motion of the limbs of an animal in locomotion and hypothesize that this mechanism serves as a general detection system for the presence of articulated terrestrial animals.
If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face recognition, it has been suggested that the inversion effect in biological motion is due to impaired con-figural processing in a highly trained expert system. Here, we present data that are incompatible with this view. We show that observers can readily retrieve information about direction from scrambled point-light displays of humans and animals. Even though all configural information is entirely disrupted, perception of these displays is still subject to a significant inversion effect. Inverting only parts of the display reveals that the information about direction, as well as the associated inversion effect, is entirely carried by the local motion of the feet. We interpret our findings in terms of a visual filter that is tuned to the characteristic motion of the limbs of an animal in locomotion and hypothesize that this mechanism serves as a general detection system for the presence of articulated terrestrial animals.
If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face recognition, it has been suggested that the inversion effect in biological motion is due to impaired configural processing in a highly trained expert system. Here, we present data that are incompatible with this view. We show that observers can readily retrieve information about direction from scrambled point-light displays of humans and animals. Even though all configural information is entirely disrupted, perception of these displays is still subject to a significant inversion effect. Inverting only parts of the display reveals that the information about direction, as well as the associated inversion effect, is entirely carried by the local motion of the feet. We interpret our findings in terms of a visual filter that is tuned to the characteristic motion of the limbs of an animal in locomotion and hypothesize that this mechanism serves as a general detection system for the presence of articulated terrestrial animals.
If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face recognition, it has been suggested that the inversion effect in biological motion is due to impaired configural processing in a highly trained expert system. Here, we present data that are incompatible with this view. We show that observers can readily retrieve information about direction from scrambled point-light displays of humans and animals. Even though all configural information is entirely disrupted, perception of these displays is still subject to a significant inversion effect. Inverting only parts of the display reveals that the information about direction, as well as the associated inversion effect, is entirely carried by the local motion of the feet. We interpret our findings in terms of a visual filter that is tuned to the characteristic motion of the limbs of an animal in locomotion and hypothesize that this mechanism serves as a general detection system for the presence of articulated terrestrial animals.If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face recognition, it has been suggested that the inversion effect in biological motion is due to impaired configural processing in a highly trained expert system. Here, we present data that are incompatible with this view. We show that observers can readily retrieve information about direction from scrambled point-light displays of humans and animals. Even though all configural information is entirely disrupted, perception of these displays is still subject to a significant inversion effect. Inverting only parts of the display reveals that the information about direction, as well as the associated inversion effect, is entirely carried by the local motion of the feet. We interpret our findings in terms of a visual filter that is tuned to the characteristic motion of the limbs of an animal in locomotion and hypothesize that this mechanism serves as a general detection system for the presence of articulated terrestrial animals.
Author Westhoff, Cord
Troje, Nikolaus F.
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Nikolaus F.
  surname: Troje
  fullname: Troje, Nikolaus F.
  email: troje@post.queensu.ca
  organization: Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, K7L3N6
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Cord
  surname: Westhoff
  fullname: Westhoff, Cord
  organization: Department of Psychology, Ruhr-Universität Bochum, 44780 Bochum, Germany
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16631591$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqFkc1u1DAURq2qqJ3-PEA3yCt2Sa8dx3FggaBModIgumgXXVmOcw0eZeKpnRmJXR8EXq5PQsIUFizK6lq637mSz3dE9vvQIyFnDHIGTJ4vc7tpcg4gcyhy4HyPzJiq6gyEKPfJDGoJWa04PyRHKS0BGFe1PCCHTMqClTWbkbubb0iv-i3G5ENP586hHajv6XsfuvDVW9PRz2GYdtcYLa6n52s63_oWe4vUhUgNfXz4sfAO6QccRjzEx4efb0_IC2e6hKdP85jcXs5vLj5liy8fry7eLTIrhBoy2SjDoIGGgYASi4ZXVjDJOTrrANq2FKKouGMOa1MzMKV0wiGOH7C8LFRxTF7t7q5juN9gGvTKJ4tdZ3oMm6RlpZQqJPw3yOpKcvE7-PIpuGlW2Op19CsTv-s_1sZAtQvYGFKK6LT1g5nMDNH4TjPQUz96qcd-9NSPhkKP_Ywk-4f8e_wZ5s2OwdHi1mPUyfpJfuvjaFu3wT9D_wIkbagg
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12811
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2022_119403
crossref_primary_10_1038_s42003_025_07861_y
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_90313_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_06_001
crossref_primary_10_1162_NECO_a_00670
crossref_primary_10_1167_19_4_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2006_04_008
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1115515109
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0007414
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_dcn_2013_01_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2016_09_010
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_isci_2023_107658
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep36742
crossref_primary_10_3389_fspor_2024_1468209
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_015_0951_4
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_22_12_10
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_023_02362_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2016_02_015
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2023_108718
crossref_primary_10_1111_bjop_12680
crossref_primary_10_1068_p6140
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0016232
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2020_01_010
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00146_010_0314_2
crossref_primary_10_1111_eth_13016
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0160468
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2017_08_004
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_21_5_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2023_105655
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2013_09_026
crossref_primary_10_7717_peerj_15694
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2016_09_018
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_0707753104
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0037085
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jrp_2010_04_002
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02217_6
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2023_103126
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2022_108307
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psyneuen_2016_07_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2023_108106
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep44012
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0039145
crossref_primary_10_1242_bio_011270
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2021_101615
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2009_11_014
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2015_07_029
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_8624_2009_01317_x
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_9280_2008_02111_x
crossref_primary_10_1177_20416695221105911
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_4133_13_2015
crossref_primary_10_3390_biomimetics7040193
crossref_primary_10_3390_electronics8101169
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_32100
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_019_47377_0
crossref_primary_10_3390_biomimetics9010027
crossref_primary_10_1068_i0612
crossref_primary_10_1167_19_14_25
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_012_0273_2
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2703_10_2011
crossref_primary_10_1111_opo_12351
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_018_5233_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2016_11_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2017_11_005
crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12565
crossref_primary_10_1080_17470919_2011_614003
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_013_0485_2
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2022_981330
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2014_07_017
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2008_02_014
crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976221091211
crossref_primary_10_1163_22134808_00002471
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2013_03_013
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2014_10_006
crossref_primary_10_1162_imag_a_00048
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2012_03_005
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41586_022_04925_5
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnint_2015_00049
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2018_1516708
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2013_01_002
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2018_03_013
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep24683
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0138502
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11434_011_4528_6
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_rasd_2014_12_004
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2023_e18161
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1532_7078_2009_00003_x
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2024_1279947
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2024_108380
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0061751
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_80678_7
crossref_primary_10_1080_07351690_2018_1405672
crossref_primary_10_1126_sciadv_abn8464
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhm242
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_01455
crossref_primary_10_1177_0301006616652026
crossref_primary_10_1093_texcom_tgac041
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neucli_2013_10_125
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01843_z
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_015_0994_1
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_018_1068_6
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0099902
crossref_primary_10_1002_aur_1595
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_paid_2011_06_014
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_023_02729_x
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_023_01764_3
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhx151
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13025
crossref_primary_10_1177_20416695241246755
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_023_02308_z
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_89873_3
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2017_09_012
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0234026
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2022_119506
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_019_01901_6
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_0707021105
crossref_primary_10_1177_09637214221128252
crossref_primary_10_1068_i0485
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11229_020_02882_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2015_04_032
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0139618
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_022_30347_y
crossref_primary_10_22172_cogbio_2013_25_1_002
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_98701_5
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2024_120889
crossref_primary_10_3758_CABN_9_3_237
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_41636_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2007_09_017
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01343
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15586
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_022_02106_z
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_89873
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2016_11_006
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10803_011_1267_0
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2009_10_021
crossref_primary_10_1080_09297049_2014_945407
crossref_primary_10_1186_1471_2202_10_39
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_015_0876_y
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_20_9_7
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0263902
crossref_primary_10_1177_2041669517750171
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2009_08_008
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2020_09_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2013_08_041
crossref_primary_10_1177_03010066221145320
crossref_primary_10_1177_23312165231212032
crossref_primary_10_1177_0301006616652054
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1714655115
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11097_020_09717_8
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bandc_2009_08_010
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12369_019_00525_y
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2017_06_010
crossref_primary_10_1038_srep42576
crossref_primary_10_4236_psych_2012_31014
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1722625115
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01396
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_020_02163_3
crossref_primary_10_34133_2022_9829016
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_011_0133_7
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2017_00505
crossref_primary_10_1097_WNR_0b013e328330a867
crossref_primary_10_1167_jov_20_10_21
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_012_3209_2
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2023_119893
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1460_9568_2007_05379_x
crossref_primary_10_1017_S003329171900031X
crossref_primary_10_1126_science_1243091
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2015_04_014
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2013_11_010
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_830555
crossref_primary_10_1111_jir_12623
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_018_2476_8
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_024_53968_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2013_07_004
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00420_2011
crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_22543
crossref_primary_10_1515_prolas_2017_0056
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2016_03_005
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_61252_3
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2009_00914_x
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_012_0284_0
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2014_10_006
crossref_primary_10_1068_p7077
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1460_9568_2007_06009_x
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_019_01313_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2022_108395
crossref_primary_10_1080_10407413_2014_957969
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1460_9568_2008_06559_x
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0018802
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_98701
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2011_11_025
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797610376072
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnint_2015_00024
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2014_00583
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2006_03_052
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_2010_21593
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1600_0838_2012_01455_x
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0028391
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_018_20808_0
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2023_2288361
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_69468_3
crossref_primary_10_1080_17588928_2011_597848
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cortex_2020_12_015
crossref_primary_10_1111_brv_12512
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_018_1112_x
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2900_13_2014
crossref_primary_10_1145_3086591
crossref_primary_10_2298_PSI1101005T
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0115117
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_010_2378_0
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10071_009_0306_0
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_010_0018_1
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2016_11_009
crossref_primary_10_1177_1747021820903042
crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218211019673
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_017_0883_9
crossref_primary_10_1111_bjop_12527
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_023_01827_7
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_21000_1
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797611417257
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797612467212
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2024_1459550
crossref_primary_10_1177_0301006617718716
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00369_2014
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2014_12_015
crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0534_19_2020
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_017_10463_2
crossref_primary_10_2352_J_Percept_Imaging_2020_3_1_010402
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12209
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_020_73431_3
crossref_primary_10_1080_09298215_2021_1977338
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2008_09_016
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_beproc_2021_104425
crossref_primary_10_2478_v10053_008_0006_3
crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_90313
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2025_101565
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2022_108142
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pbio_3001172
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1167809
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2012_05_008
crossref_primary_10_1177_2041669517707767
crossref_primary_10_1111_jpr_12145
crossref_primary_10_3389_fncir_2014_00119
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2018_09_017
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0954579408000515
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_plrev_2023_06_005
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuropsychologia_2009_07_026
crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_247061
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797618778498
crossref_primary_10_1002_advs_202411562
crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1467_7687_2010_00985_x
crossref_primary_10_1177_1362361313499455
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0059196
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2011_07_051
crossref_primary_10_1080_13506285_2011_614381
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2017_0263
crossref_primary_10_1002_pchj_53
crossref_primary_10_1186_s41235_018_0117_4
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0112539
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2008_1363
crossref_primary_10_1111_nyas_13727
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0116910
crossref_primary_10_3390_ani9090661
crossref_primary_10_3233_JHD_180337
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2008_05_002
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_011_2883_9
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10803_021_05352_7
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11031_024_10105_x
crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00892_2009
crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_016_1118_3
crossref_primary_10_22172_cogbio_2012_24_4_004
crossref_primary_10_3390_sym9030039
crossref_primary_10_1177_03010066211000192
Cites_doi 10.1111/1467-9280.24471
10.1167/2.5.2
10.1068/p220015
10.1016/0022-0965(84)90001-8
10.3758/BF03212378
10.1080/713755777
10.3758/BF03208159
10.1111/1467-9280.14431
10.1016/j.visres.2005.02.001
10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00504.x
10.3758/BF03206167
10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01903-4
10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.052
10.3758/BF03206847
10.1111/1467-9280.00387
10.1207/s15326969eco0501_2
10.1167/3.4.1
10.1068/p3392
10.1037/0096-3445.112.4.585
10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.628
10.1068/p130283
10.1068/p3012
10.1016/j.cub.2004.02.028
10.3758/BF03208295
10.3758/BF03206833
10.3758/BF03212075
10.1037/0096-1523.13.2.155
10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2006 Elsevier Ltd
Copyright_xml – notice: 2006 Elsevier Ltd
DBID 6I.
AAFTH
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7TK
7X8
DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022
DatabaseName ScienceDirect Open Access Titles
Elsevier:ScienceDirect:Open Access
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Biology
EISSN 1879-0445
EndPage 824
ExternalDocumentID 16631591
10_1016_j_cub_2006_03_022
S0960982206012814
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
--K
-DZ
-~X
0R~
1RT
1~5
29F
2WC
4.4
457
4G.
53G
5GY
5VS
62-
6I.
6J9
7-5
AACTN
AAEDT
AAEDW
AAFTH
AAFWJ
AAIAV
AAIKJ
AAKRW
AALRI
AAQFI
AAQXK
AAUCE
AAVLU
AAXJY
AAXUO
ABJNI
ABMAC
ABMWF
ABVKL
ACGFO
ACGFS
ADBBV
ADEZE
ADJPV
ADMUD
AEFWE
AENEX
AEXQZ
AFTJW
AGHFR
AGHSJ
AGKMS
AGUBO
AHHHB
AHPSJ
AITUG
ALKID
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AMRAJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AZFZN
BAWUL
CAG
COF
CS3
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EBS
EJD
F5P
FCP
FDB
FEDTE
FGOYB
FIRID
G-2
HVGLF
HZ~
IHE
IXB
J1W
JIG
LX5
M3Z
M41
NCXOZ
O-L
O9-
OK1
OZT
P2P
R2-
RCE
RIG
ROL
RPZ
SCP
SDG
SES
SEW
SSZ
TR2
UHS
WQ6
XIH
XPP
Y6R
ZA5
ZGI
AAMRU
AAYWO
AAYXX
ABDGV
ABWVN
ACRPL
ACVFH
ADCNI
ADNMO
ADVLN
AEUPX
AFPUW
AGCQF
AGQPQ
AIGII
AKAPO
AKBMS
AKRWK
AKYEP
APXCP
CITATION
0SF
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7TK
7X8
EFKBS
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c448t-6b8a10b0b10405e3b27c41622efcf00dd544372f1fe9a910a56f4fee631c25383
IEDL.DBID IXB
ISSN 0960-9822
IngestDate Mon Jul 21 10:57:15 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 23:22:16 EDT 2025
Sat Sep 28 07:43:11 EDT 2024
Tue Jul 01 04:07:35 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:51:25 EDT 2025
Fri Feb 23 02:30:33 EST 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 8
Keywords SYSNEURO
Language English
License http://www.elsevier.com/open-access/userlicense/1.0
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c448t-6b8a10b0b10405e3b27c41622efcf00dd544372f1fe9a910a56f4fee631c25383
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
OpenAccessLink https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0960982206012814
PMID 16631591
PQID 19762460
PQPubID 23462
PageCount 4
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_67888360
proquest_miscellaneous_19762460
pubmed_primary_16631591
crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_cub_2006_03_022
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cub_2006_03_022
elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_cub_2006_03_022
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2006-04-18
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2006-04-18
PublicationDate_xml – month: 04
  year: 2006
  text: 2006-04-18
  day: 18
PublicationDecade 2000
PublicationPlace England
PublicationPlace_xml – name: England
PublicationTitle Current biology
PublicationTitleAlternate Curr Biol
PublicationYear 2006
Publisher Elsevier Inc
Publisher_xml – name: Elsevier Inc
References Troje (bib9) 2003; 32
Freire, Lee, Symons (bib13) 2000; 29
Maurer, Grand, Mondloch (bib15) 2002; 6
McConnell, Muchisky, Bingham (bib20) 1998; 60
Dittrich (bib4) 1993; 22
Barclay, Cutting, Kozlowski (bib10) 1978; 23
Knoblich, Flach (bib24) 2001; 12
Bertenthal, Pinto (bib8) 1994; 5
Proffitt, Bertenthal (bib3) 1990; 47
Runeson, Frykholm (bib21) 1983; 112
Leder, Bruce (bib14) 1998; 51
Jokisch, Troje (bib19) 2003; 3
Sumi (bib1) 1984; 13
Pavlova, Sokolov (bib2) 2000; 62
Shipley (bib11) 2003; 14
Shiffrar, Lichtey, Chatterjee (bib17) 1997; 59
Ikeda, Blake, Watanabe (bib7) 2005; 45
Troje (bib28) 2002; 2
Hecht, Kaiser, Banks (bib18) 1996; 58
Bingham (bib22) 1987; 13
Reed, Stone, Bozova, Tanaka (bib5) 2003; 14
Vallortigara, Regolin (bib27) 2006; 16
Johansson (bib6) 1973; 14
Bertenthal, Proffitt, Cutting (bib25) 1984; 37
Sekuler, Gaspar, Gold, Bennett (bib16) 2004; 14
Vallortigara, Regolin, Marconato (bib26) 2005; 3
Farah, Tanaka, Drain (bib12) 1995; 21
Bingham (bib23) 1993; 5
Pavlova (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib2) 2000; 62
Jokisch (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib19) 2003; 3
Hecht (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib18) 1996; 58
Bertenthal (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib8) 1994; 5
Vallortigara (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib27) 2006; 16
Troje (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib28) 2002; 2
McConnell (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib20) 1998; 60
Sumi (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib1) 1984; 13
Proffitt (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib3) 1990; 47
Shipley (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib11) 2003; 14
Freire (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib13) 2000; 29
Maurer (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib15) 2002; 6
Sekuler (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib16) 2004; 14
Shiffrar (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib17) 1997; 59
Dittrich (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib4) 1993; 22
Johansson (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib6) 1973; 14
Knoblich (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib24) 2001; 12
Troje (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib9) 2003; 32
Ikeda (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib7) 2005; 45
Bingham (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib22) 1987; 13
Leder (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib14) 1998; 51
Runeson (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib21) 1983; 112
Vallortigara (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib26) 2005; 3
Barclay (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib10) 1978; 23
Bingham (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib23) 1993; 5
Bertenthal (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib25) 1984; 37
Reed (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib5) 2003; 14
Farah (10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib12) 1995; 21
16713949 - Curr Biol. 2006 May 23;16(10):R376-7
References_xml – volume: 2
  start-page: 371
  year: 2002
  end-page: 387
  ident: bib28
  article-title: Decomposing biological motion: A framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns
  publication-title: J. Vis.
– volume: 12
  start-page: 467
  year: 2001
  end-page: 472
  ident: bib24
  article-title: Predicting the effects of actions: Interactions of perception and action
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
– volume: 58
  start-page: 1066
  year: 1996
  end-page: 1075
  ident: bib18
  article-title: Gravitational acceleration as a cue for absolute size and distance?
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1935
  year: 2005
  end-page: 1943
  ident: bib7
  article-title: Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor
  publication-title: Vision Res.
– volume: 37
  start-page: 213
  year: 1984
  end-page: 230
  ident: bib25
  article-title: Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions
  publication-title: J. Exp. Child Psychol.
– volume: 5
  start-page: 221
  year: 1994
  end-page: 225
  ident: bib8
  article-title: Global processing of biological motions
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
– volume: 21
  start-page: 628
  year: 1995
  end-page: 634
  ident: bib12
  article-title: What causes the face inversion effect?
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
– volume: 14
  start-page: 201
  year: 1973
  end-page: 211
  ident: bib6
  article-title: Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
– volume: 29
  start-page: 159
  year: 2000
  end-page: 170
  ident: bib13
  article-title: The face-inversion effect as a deficit in the encoding of configural information: Direct evidence
  publication-title: Perception
– volume: 13
  start-page: 283
  year: 1984
  end-page: 286
  ident: bib1
  article-title: Upside-down presentation of the Johansson moving light-spot pattern
  publication-title: Perception
– volume: 14
  start-page: 302
  year: 2003
  end-page: 308
  ident: bib5
  article-title: The body-inversion effect
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
– volume: 13
  start-page: 155
  year: 1987
  end-page: 177
  ident: bib22
  article-title: Kinematic form and scaling: Further investigations on the visual perception of lifted weight
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
– volume: 3
  start-page: 252
  year: 2003
  end-page: 264
  ident: bib19
  article-title: Biological motion as a cue for the perception of size
  publication-title: J. Vis.
– volume: 16
  start-page: R279
  year: 2006
  end-page: R280
  ident: bib27
  article-title: Natural-born physicists: A gravity bias in the interpretation of biological motion in visually-inexperienced chicks
  publication-title: Curr. Biol.
– volume: 6
  start-page: 255
  year: 2002
  end-page: 260
  ident: bib15
  article-title: The many faces of configural processing
  publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci.
– volume: 14
  start-page: 391
  year: 2004
  end-page: 396
  ident: bib16
  article-title: Inversion leads to quantitative, not qualitative, changes in face processing
  publication-title: Curr. Biol.
– volume: 22
  start-page: 15
  year: 1993
  end-page: 22
  ident: bib4
  article-title: Action categories and the perception of biological motion
  publication-title: Perception
– volume: 32
  start-page: 201
  year: 2003
  end-page: 210
  ident: bib9
  article-title: Reference frames for orientation anisotropies in face recognition and biological-motion perception
  publication-title: Perception
– volume: 23
  start-page: 145
  year: 1978
  end-page: 152
  ident: bib10
  article-title: Temporal and spatial factors in gait perception that influence gender recognition
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
– volume: 60
  start-page: 1175
  year: 1998
  end-page: 1187
  ident: bib20
  article-title: The use of time and trajectory forms as visual information about spatial scale in events
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
– volume: 14
  start-page: 377
  year: 2003
  end-page: 380
  ident: bib11
  article-title: The effect of object and event orientation on perception of biological motion
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
– volume: 51
  start-page: 449
  year: 1998
  end-page: 473
  ident: bib14
  article-title: Local and relational aspects of face distinctiveness
  publication-title: Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
– volume: 62
  start-page: 889
  year: 2000
  end-page: 899
  ident: bib2
  article-title: Orientation specificity in biological motion perception
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
– volume: 5
  start-page: 31
  year: 1993
  end-page: 64
  ident: bib23
  article-title: Scaling judgements of lifting weights: Lifter size and the role of the standard
  publication-title: Ecol. Psychol.
– volume: 59
  start-page: 51
  year: 1997
  end-page: 59
  ident: bib17
  article-title: The perception of biological motion across apertures
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
– volume: 3
  start-page: e208
  year: 2005
  ident: bib26
  article-title: Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns
  publication-title: PLoS Biol.
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1
  year: 1990
  end-page: 11
  ident: bib3
  article-title: Converging operations revisited: Assessing what infants perceive using discrimination measures
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
– volume: 112
  start-page: 585
  year: 1983
  end-page: 615
  ident: bib21
  article-title: Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.
– volume: 14
  start-page: 377
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib11
  article-title: The effect of object and event orientation on perception of biological motion
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.24471
– volume: 2
  start-page: 371
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib28
  article-title: Decomposing biological motion: A framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns
  publication-title: J. Vis.
  doi: 10.1167/2.5.2
– volume: 22
  start-page: 15
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib4
  article-title: Action categories and the perception of biological motion
  publication-title: Perception
  doi: 10.1068/p220015
– volume: 37
  start-page: 213
  year: 1984
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib25
  article-title: Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions
  publication-title: J. Exp. Child Psychol.
  doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90001-8
– volume: 14
  start-page: 201
  year: 1973
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib6
  article-title: Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03212378
– volume: 51
  start-page: 449
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib14
  article-title: Local and relational aspects of face distinctiveness
  publication-title: Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1080/713755777
– volume: 47
  start-page: 1
  year: 1990
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib3
  article-title: Converging operations revisited: Assessing what infants perceive using discrimination measures
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03208159
– volume: 14
  start-page: 302
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib5
  article-title: The body-inversion effect
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14431
– volume: 45
  start-page: 1935
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib7
  article-title: Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor
  publication-title: Vision Res.
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.02.001
– volume: 5
  start-page: 221
  year: 1994
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib8
  article-title: Global processing of biological motions
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00504.x
– volume: 60
  start-page: 1175
  year: 1998
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib20
  article-title: The use of time and trajectory forms as visual information about spatial scale in events
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206167
– volume: 6
  start-page: 255
  year: 2002
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib15
  article-title: The many faces of configural processing
  publication-title: Trends Cogn. Sci.
  doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01903-4
– volume: 16
  start-page: R279
  year: 2006
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib27
  article-title: Natural-born physicists: A gravity bias in the interpretation of biological motion in visually-inexperienced chicks
  publication-title: Curr. Biol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.052
– volume: 59
  start-page: 51
  year: 1997
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib17
  article-title: The perception of biological motion across apertures
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206847
– volume: 12
  start-page: 467
  year: 2001
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib24
  article-title: Predicting the effects of actions: Interactions of perception and action
  publication-title: Psychol. Sci.
  doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00387
– volume: 5
  start-page: 31
  year: 1993
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib23
  article-title: Scaling judgements of lifting weights: Lifter size and the role of the standard
  publication-title: Ecol. Psychol.
  doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco0501_2
– volume: 3
  start-page: 252
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib19
  article-title: Biological motion as a cue for the perception of size
  publication-title: J. Vis.
  doi: 10.1167/3.4.1
– volume: 32
  start-page: 201
  year: 2003
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib9
  article-title: Reference frames for orientation anisotropies in face recognition and biological-motion perception
  publication-title: Perception
  doi: 10.1068/p3392
– volume: 112
  start-page: 585
  year: 1983
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib21
  article-title: Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.112.4.585
– volume: 21
  start-page: 628
  year: 1995
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib12
  article-title: What causes the face inversion effect?
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.628
– volume: 13
  start-page: 283
  year: 1984
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib1
  article-title: Upside-down presentation of the Johansson moving light-spot pattern
  publication-title: Perception
  doi: 10.1068/p130283
– volume: 29
  start-page: 159
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib13
  article-title: The face-inversion effect as a deficit in the encoding of configural information: Direct evidence
  publication-title: Perception
  doi: 10.1068/p3012
– volume: 14
  start-page: 391
  year: 2004
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib16
  article-title: Inversion leads to quantitative, not qualitative, changes in face processing
  publication-title: Curr. Biol.
  doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.02.028
– volume: 23
  start-page: 145
  year: 1978
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib10
  article-title: Temporal and spatial factors in gait perception that influence gender recognition
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03208295
– volume: 58
  start-page: 1066
  year: 1996
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib18
  article-title: Gravitational acceleration as a cue for absolute size and distance?
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206833
– volume: 62
  start-page: 889
  year: 2000
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib2
  article-title: Orientation specificity in biological motion perception
  publication-title: Percept. Psychophys.
  doi: 10.3758/BF03212075
– volume: 13
  start-page: 155
  year: 1987
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib22
  article-title: Kinematic form and scaling: Further investigations on the visual perception of lifted weight
  publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.13.2.155
– volume: 3
  start-page: e208
  year: 2005
  ident: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022_bib26
  article-title: Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns
  publication-title: PLoS Biol.
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208
– reference: 16713949 - Curr Biol. 2006 May 23;16(10):R376-7
SSID ssj0012896
Score 2.3868752
Snippet If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired [1, 2]. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in...
If biological-motion point-light displays are presented upside down, adequate perception is strongly impaired. Reminiscent of the inversion effect in face...
SourceID proquest
pubmed
crossref
elsevier
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 821
SubjectTerms Animals
Female
Foot - physiology
Humans
Life
Locomotion - physiology
Male
Motion Perception
Pattern Recognition, Visual
SYSNEURO
Title The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”?
URI https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16631591
https://www.proquest.com/docview/19762460
https://www.proquest.com/docview/67888360
Volume 16
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LS8QwEA6LIngR367PHDwJdfNo0taL6Kosror4wPUU0jaBFamiuwdv_hD9c_4SJ2m74mE9eC3TEmbSmW8yk_kQ2tYQFxLG4kAbyHRCRqJA55IHeZImkZZ5aj1p3_mF7NyGpz3Ra6B2fRfGtVVWvr_06d5bV09alTZbz_1-69oPS4P45iaKsNiTWfMw9pf4eoejSgIkFL5eCcKBk64rm77HKxumVT2C7xLGxsWmcdjTx6CTWTRTgUd8UK5vDjVMMY-mSjrJtwV0DzbHbnCGPwLD5WBi3C9wKeHMgc89aw--HPWz7OGaWBQDfsUaf71_nPWtwUdm4E_0v94_9xfR7cnxTbsTVNQJQQb51iCQaawpSUkK2RYRhqcsygB6MWZsZgnJczf2LmKWWpNoQAxaSBtaYySnGQMfyJfQRPFUmBWEo5wLmgubJdRA9BfwXWHjWFIjaKyFaCJSK01l1VxxR2_xqOoGsgcFenZ8l1IRrkDPTbQzeuW5HKrxl3BYW0L92hkKnP5fr23VVlPwx7gyiC7M0_BVUUBgLJRkvIR05wLcSSyX5v5ZJQA0AIB09X-LWkPT5RFOGNB4HU0MXoZmA0DNIN1Ekwfdq7vupt-936QK9Mk
linkProvider Elsevier
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV07T8MwELYQCMGCeFOeHpiQQm0ndhIWxKsq0CIkilQmy0lsqQilFbQDW38I_Dl-CWcnKWIoA2t0jqw75-473-U-hA4VxIWYschTGjKdgJHQU5nwvSxO4lCJLDGOtK99J5qPwU2Xd2fQRfUvjG2rLH1_4dOdty6f1Ett1ge9Xv3BDUuD-GYnirDIklnPARoQ9mhfd88npQTIKFzBEqQ9K16VNl2TVzpKyoKEf0wYmxacpoFPF4Qay2ipRI_4rNjgCprR-SqaL_gk39fQExgd28kZ7g4MF5OJcS_HhYS1B2472h58P2loOcEVsygGAIsV_hp_tHpG40s9dFf6X-PP03X02LjqXDS9kjvBSyHhGnoiiRQlCUkg3SJc-wkLU8BejGmTGkKyzM69C5mhRscKIIPiwgRGa-HTlIET9DfQbN7P9RbCYeZzmnGTxlRD-OfwXm6iSFDNaaQ4ryFSKU2m5WBxy2_xIqsOsmcJeraEl0ISX4Kea-hosmRQTNX4SzioLCF_HQ0JXv-vZQeV1SR8MrYOonLdH71JChCMBYJMlxD2YsC3EpuFuX92CQgNECDd_t-mDtBCs9Nuydb13e0OWizucwKPRrtodvg60nuAcIbJvjvB31fC9k0
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+inversion+effect+in+biological+motion+perception%3A+evidence+for+a+%22life+detector%22%3F&rft.jtitle=Current+biology&rft.au=Troje%2C+Nikolaus+F&rft.au=Westhoff%2C+Cord&rft.date=2006-04-18&rft.issn=0960-9822&rft.volume=16&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=821&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.cub.2006.03.022&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0960-9822&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0960-9822&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0960-9822&client=summon