Dopamine transporter genotype is associated with a lateralized resistance to distraction during attention selection

Although lateral asymmetries in orienting behavior are evident across species and have been linked to interhemispheric asymmetries in dopamine signaling, the relative contribution of attentional versus motoric processes remains unclear. Here we took a cognitive genetic approach to adjudicate between...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inThe Journal of neuroscience Vol. 34; no. 47; pp. 15743 - 15750
Main Authors Newman, Daniel P, Cummins, Tarrant D R, Tong, Janette H S, Johnson, Beth P, Pickering, Hayley, Fanning, Peter, Wagner, Joseph, Goodrich, Jack T T, Hawi, Ziarih, Chambers, Christopher D, Bellgrove, Mark A
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States Society for Neuroscience 19.11.2014
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Although lateral asymmetries in orienting behavior are evident across species and have been linked to interhemispheric asymmetries in dopamine signaling, the relative contribution of attentional versus motoric processes remains unclear. Here we took a cognitive genetic approach to adjudicate between roles for dopamine in attentional versus response selection. A sample of nonclinical adult humans (N = 518) performed three cognitive tasks (spatial attentional competition, spatial cueing, and flanker tasks) that varied in the degree to which they required participants to resolve attentional or response competition. All participants were genotyped for two putatively functional tandem repeat polymorphisms of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1; SLC6A3), which are argued to influence the level of available synaptic dopamine and confer risk to disorders of inattention. DAT1 genotype modulated the task-specific effects of the various task-irrelevant stimuli across both the spatial competition and spatial cueing but not flanker tasks. Specifically, compared with individuals carrying one or two copies of the 10-repeat DAT1 allele, individuals without this allele demonstrated an immunity to distraction, such that response times were unaffected by increases in the number of distractor stimuli, particularly when these were presented predominantly in the left hemifield. All three genotype groups exhibited uniform costs of resolving leftward response selection in a standard flanker task. None of these significant effects could be explained by speed-accuracy trade-offs, suggesting that participants without the 10-repeat allele of the DAT1 tandem repeat polymorphism possess an enhanced attentional ability to suppress task-irrelevant stimuli in the left hemifield.
AbstractList Although lateral asymmetries in orienting behavior are evident across species and have been linked to interhemispheric asymmetries in dopamine signaling, the relative contribution of attentional versus motoric processes remains unclear. Here we took a cognitive genetic approach to adjudicate between roles for dopamine in attentional versus response selection. A sample of nonclinical adult humans (N = 518) performed three cognitive tasks (spatial attentional competition, spatial cueing, and flanker tasks) that varied in the degree to which they required participants to resolve attentional or response competition. All participants were genotyped for two putatively functional tandem repeat polymorphisms of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1; SLC6A3), which are argued to influence the level of available synaptic dopamine and confer risk to disorders of inattention. DAT1 genotype modulated the task-specific effects of the various task-irrelevant stimuli across both the spatial competition and spatial cueing but not flanker tasks. Specifically, compared with individuals carrying one or two copies of the 10-repeat DAT1 allele, individuals without this allele demonstrated an immunity to distraction, such that response times were unaffected by increases in the number of distractor stimuli, particularly when these were presented predominantly in the left hemifield. All three genotype groups exhibited uniform costs of resolving leftward response selection in a standard flanker task. None of these significant effects could be explained by speed-accuracy trade-offs, suggesting that participants without the 10-repeat allele of the DAT1 tandem repeat polymorphism possess an enhanced attentional ability to suppress task-irrelevant stimuli in the left hemifield.
Although lateral asymmetries in orienting behavior are evident across species and have been linked to interhemispheric asymmetries in dopamine signaling, the relative contribution of attentional versus motoric processes remains unclear. Here we took a cognitive genetic approach to adjudicate between roles for dopamine in attentional versus response selection. A sample of nonclinical adult humans (N = 518) performed three cognitive tasks (spatial attentional competition, spatial cueing, and flanker tasks) that varied in the degree to which they required participants to resolve attentional or response competition. All participants were genotyped for two putatively functional tandem repeat polymorphisms of the dopamine transporter gene (DATI; SLC6A3), which are argued to influence the level of available synaptic dopamine and confer risk to disorders of inattention. DATI genotype modulated the task-specific effects of the various task-irrelevant stimuli across both the spatial competition and spatial cueing but not flanker tasks. Specifically, compared with individuals carrying one or two copies of the 10-repeat DATI allele, individuals without this allele demonstrated an immunity to distraction, such that response times were unaffected by increases in the number of distractor stimuli, particularly when these were presented predominantly in the left hemifield. All three genotype groups exhibited uniform costs of resolving leftward response selection in a standard flanker task. None of these significant effects could be explained by speed-accuracy trade-offs, suggesting that participants without the 10-repeat allele of the DATI tandem repeat polymorphism possess an enhanced attentional ability to suppress task-irrelevant stimuli in the left hemifield.
Although lateral asymmetries in orienting behavior are evident across species and have been linked to interhemispheric asymmetries in dopamine signaling, the relative contribution of attentional versus motoric processes remains unclear. Here we took a cognitive genetic approach to adjudicate between roles for dopamine in attentional versus response selection. A sample of nonclinical adult humans ( N = 518) performed three cognitive tasks (spatial attentional competition, spatial cueing, and flanker tasks) that varied in the degree to which they required participants to resolve attentional or response competition. All participants were genotyped for two putatively functional tandem repeat polymorphisms of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1; SLC6A3 ), which are argued to influence the level of available synaptic dopamine and confer risk to disorders of inattention. DAT1 genotype modulated the task-specific effects of the various task-irrelevant stimuli across both the spatial competition and spatial cueing but not flanker tasks. Specifically, compared with individuals carrying one or two copies of the 10-repeat DAT1 allele, individuals without this allele demonstrated an immunity to distraction, such that response times were unaffected by increases in the number of distractor stimuli, particularly when these were presented predominantly in the left hemifield. All three genotype groups exhibited uniform costs of resolving leftward response selection in a standard flanker task. None of these significant effects could be explained by speed–accuracy trade-offs, suggesting that participants without the 10-repeat allele of the DAT1 tandem repeat polymorphism possess an enhanced attentional ability to suppress task-irrelevant stimuli in the left hemifield.
Author Tong, Janette H S
Johnson, Beth P
Newman, Daniel P
Cummins, Tarrant D R
Goodrich, Jack T T
Hawi, Ziarih
Pickering, Hayley
Fanning, Peter
Wagner, Joseph
Bellgrove, Mark A
Chambers, Christopher D
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Daniel P
  orcidid: 0000-0001-8240-1876
  surname: Newman
  fullname: Newman, Daniel P
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Tarrant D R
  surname: Cummins
  fullname: Cummins, Tarrant D R
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Janette H S
  surname: Tong
  fullname: Tong, Janette H S
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Beth P
  surname: Johnson
  fullname: Johnson, Beth P
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Hayley
  surname: Pickering
  fullname: Pickering, Hayley
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
– sequence: 6
  givenname: Peter
  orcidid: 0000-0002-6074-5130
  surname: Fanning
  fullname: Fanning, Peter
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
– sequence: 7
  givenname: Joseph
  surname: Wagner
  fullname: Wagner, Joseph
  organization: The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute and School of Psychology, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, Australia, and
– sequence: 8
  givenname: Jack T T
  surname: Goodrich
  fullname: Goodrich, Jack T T
  organization: The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute and School of Psychology, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, Australia, and
– sequence: 9
  givenname: Ziarih
  surname: Hawi
  fullname: Hawi, Ziarih
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia
– sequence: 10
  givenname: Christopher D
  surname: Chambers
  fullname: Chambers, Christopher D
  organization: Cardiff University, School of Psychology, Cardiff CF10 3AT, United Kingdom
– sequence: 11
  givenname: Mark A
  surname: Bellgrove
  fullname: Bellgrove, Mark A
  email: mark.bellgrove@monash.edu
  organization: Monash University, School of Psychological Sciences, Clayton, Victoria 3800, Australia, The University of Queensland, Queensland Brain Institute and School of Psychology, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, Australia, and mark.bellgrove@monash.edu
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25411502$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNqNkctOHDEQRa2IKAwkv4C8zKYHv929QYomhIdQkAKsLbe7ZjDqsTu2JxH5ejw8RskuK5erbl3d0jlAeyEGQOiIkjmVjB9ffj-9-3F9s7iYM850Q8WcESreoVmddg0ThO6hGWGaNEposY8Ocn4ghGhC9Qe0z6SgVBI2Q_lrnOzaB8Al2ZCnmAokvIIQy-ME2Gdsc47O2wID_u3LPbZ4rJ9kR_-nthJkn4sNrhpEPNQ6WVd8DHjYJB9W2JYC4bmRYYTn0Uf0fmnHDJ9e30N09-30dnHeXF2fXSy-XDVOCF2aQTLR9p1y3SBp1wtNXctoqzh3tOeEKslaZuVSDNBb1tpaSDXwpaWiE04CP0QnL77Tpl_D4GqOGttMya9tejTRevPvJPh7s4q_jFKkFVxVg8-vBin-3EAuZu2zg3G0AeImG6qZVlq28j-kqrLQLVddlaoXqUsx5wTLXSJKzBau2cE1W7iGCrOFWxeP_r5nt_ZGkz8BfX2mgw
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2018_00049
crossref_primary_10_1002_brb3_446
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00213_022_06077_w
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2016_06_032
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_10634_w
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00406_017_0786_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsycho_2017_07_019
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41597_022_01268_8
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jstrokecerebrovasdis_2017_02_012
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2019_05_068
crossref_primary_10_2139_ssrn_3899433
crossref_primary_10_1080_00207454_2017_1385614
Cites_doi 10.1093/cercor/11.1.73
10.1038/sj.mp.4001641
10.1002/ajmg.b.10948
10.1212/WNL.51.5.1395
10.3758/BF03211502
10.1038/217
10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00647.x
10.1073/pnas.0504789103
10.1093/cercor/bhs277
10.1016/B978-0-12-286480-3.50014-8
10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.027
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.012
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01863.1984
10.1086/319501
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-09-03082.1996
10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.583
10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
10.1038/sj.npp.1301637
10.1002/ajmg.b.30572
10.1212/WNL.37.11.1765
10.1177/0956797611417632
10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.74
10.1186/1471-2156-6-55
10.1038/sj.mp.4002022
10.1086/379378
10.1007/s002130050352
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-01-00167.1994
10.1093/brain/aws154
10.1016/0028-3932(89)90166-8
10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.269
10.1002/cne.1092
10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00099-8
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.005
10.1136/jnnp.66.1.57
10.1016/0006-8993(94)90079-5
10.1038/sj.npp.1300839
10.3758/BRM.42.1.205
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.015
10.1038/sj.tpj.6500026
10.1038/sj.mp.4001921
10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.120
10.1177/0894439311415604
10.1016/j.euroneuro.2004.06.004
10.1016/0028-3932(95)00037-4
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2014 the authors 0270-6474/14/3315743-08$15.00/0.
Copyright © 2014 the authors 0270-6474/14/3315743-08$15.00/0 2014
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2014 the authors 0270-6474/14/3315743-08$15.00/0.
– notice: Copyright © 2014 the authors 0270-6474/14/3315743-08$15.00/0 2014
DBID CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
7X8
7TK
5PM
DOI 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2327-14.2014
DatabaseName Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
CrossRef
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
CrossRef
MEDLINE - Academic
Neurosciences Abstracts
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
CrossRef

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1529-2401
EndPage 15750
ExternalDocumentID 10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2327_14_2014
25411502
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-~X
.55
18M
2WC
34G
39C
53G
5GY
5RE
5VS
AAFWJ
ABBAR
ABIVO
ACGUR
ACNCT
ADBBV
ADCOW
AENEX
AFCFT
AFHIN
AFOSN
AHWXS
AIZTS
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
BAWUL
BTFSW
CGR
CS3
CUY
CVF
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EBS
ECM
EIF
EJD
F5P
GX1
H13
HYE
H~9
KQ8
L7B
NPM
OK1
P0W
P2P
QZG
R.V
RHF
RHI
RPM
TFN
TR2
W8F
WH7
WOQ
X7M
XJT
YBU
YHG
YKV
YNH
YSK
AAYXX
CITATION
7X8
7TK
5PM
AETEA
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c447t-d5248b96c9d519b471c8218633c1b30165282a5f4deba28af4d56d3fa1494c5e3
IEDL.DBID RPM
ISSN 0270-6474
IngestDate Tue Sep 17 21:07:12 EDT 2024
Fri Oct 25 23:27:28 EDT 2024
Fri Oct 25 06:08:55 EDT 2024
Thu Sep 26 18:06:23 EDT 2024
Sat Sep 28 08:38:41 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 47
Keywords spatial attention
genetics
DAT1
dopamine
selective attention
individual differences
Language English
License Copyright © 2014 the authors 0270-6474/14/3315743-08$15.00/0.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c447t-d5248b96c9d519b471c8218633c1b30165282a5f4deba28af4d56d3fa1494c5e3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
Author contributions: C.D.C. and M.A.B. designed research; D.P.N., T.D.R.C., J.H.S.T., B.P.J., H.P., P.F., J.W., J.T.T.G., Z.H., C.D.C., and M.A.B. performed research; T.D.R.C., J.H.S.T., and Z.H. contributed unpublished reagents/analytic tools; D.P.N., C.D.C., and M.A.B. analyzed data; D.P.N., C.D.C., and M.A.B. wrote the paper.
ORCID 0000-0002-6074-5130
0000-0001-8240-1876
OpenAccessLink https://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneuro/34/47/15743.full.pdf
PMID 25411502
PQID 1627078369
PQPubID 23479
PageCount 8
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6608436
proquest_miscellaneous_1727675856
proquest_miscellaneous_1627078369
crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_2327_14_2014
pubmed_primary_25411502
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2014-Nov-19
2014-11-19
20141119
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2014-11-19
PublicationDate_xml – month: 11
  year: 2014
  text: 2014-Nov-19
  day: 19
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
PublicationTitle The Journal of neuroscience
PublicationTitleAlternate J Neurosci
PublicationYear 2014
Publisher Society for Neuroscience
Publisher_xml – name: Society for Neuroscience
References Ward (2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.43) 1996; 16
Cook (2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.10) 1995; 56
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.29
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.1
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.28
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.2
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.27
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.3
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.26
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.4
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.5
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.24
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.46
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.23
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.45
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.22
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.44
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.21
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.20
Posner (2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.34) 1984; 4
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.41
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.40
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.6
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.7
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.8
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.9
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.19
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.18
Iversen (2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.25) 1984; 107
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.17
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.39
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.16
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.38
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.15
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.37
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.14
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.36
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.13
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.35
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.12
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.11
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.33
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.32
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.31
2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.30
Voytko (2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.42) 1994; 14
References_xml – ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.12
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.1.73
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.11
  doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001641
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.30
  doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.10948
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.22
  doi: 10.1212/WNL.51.5.1395
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.15
  doi: 10.3758/BF03211502
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.14
  doi: 10.1038/217
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.29
  doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00647.x
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.23
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504789103
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.40
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs277
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.19
  doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-286480-3.50014-8
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.45
  doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.027
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.32
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.012
– volume: 4
  start-page: 1863
  year: 1984
  ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.34
  article-title: Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention
  publication-title: J Neurosci
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01863.1984
  contributor:
    fullname: Posner
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.39
  doi: 10.1086/319501
– volume: 16
  start-page: 3082
  year: 1996
  ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.43
  article-title: Covert orienting of attention in the rat and the role of striatal dopamine
  publication-title: J Neurosci
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-09-03082.1996
  contributor:
    fullname: Ward
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.16
  doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.583
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.33
  doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.3
  doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301637
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.7
  doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30572
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.17
  doi: 10.1212/WNL.37.11.1765
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.37
  doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.6
  doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.74
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.41
  doi: 10.1186/1471-2156-6-55
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.2
  doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002022
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.38
  doi: 10.1086/379378
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.44
  doi: 10.1007/s002130050352
– volume: 14
  start-page: 167
  year: 1994
  ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.42
  article-title: Basal forebrain lesions in monkeys disrupt attention but not learning and memory
  publication-title: J Neurosci
  doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-01-00167.1994
  contributor:
    fullname: Voytko
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.21
  doi: 10.1093/brain/aws154
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.9
  doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90166-8
– volume: 107
  start-page: 183
  year: 1984
  ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.25
  article-title: Behavioural effects of manipulation of basal ganglia neurotransmitters
  publication-title: Ciba Found Symp
  contributor:
    fullname: Iversen
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.26
  doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.269
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.27
  doi: 10.1002/cne.1092
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.24
  doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00099-8
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.46
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.005
– volume: 56
  start-page: 993
  year: 1995
  ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.10
  article-title: Association of attention-deficit disorder and the dopamine transporter gene
  publication-title: Am J Hum Genet
  contributor:
    fullname: Cook
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.35
  doi: 10.1136/jnnp.66.1.57
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.20
  doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90079-5
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.1
  doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300839
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.13
  doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.205
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.36
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.015
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.18
  doi: 10.1038/sj.tpj.6500026
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.31
  doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001921
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.4
  doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.120
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.5
  doi: 10.1177/0894439311415604
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.8
  doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2004.06.004
– ident: 2023041304141882000_34.47.15743.28
  doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00037-4
SSID ssj0007017
Score 2.2561753
Snippet Although lateral asymmetries in orienting behavior are evident across species and have been linked to interhemispheric asymmetries in dopamine signaling, the...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
crossref
pubmed
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
StartPage 15743
SubjectTerms Attention - physiology
Cognition - physiology
Cues
Dopamine Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins - genetics
Female
Functional Laterality - genetics
Functional Laterality - physiology
Genotype
Humans
Male
Orientation - physiology
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Space Perception - physiology
Young Adult
Title Dopamine transporter genotype is associated with a lateralized resistance to distraction during attention selection
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25411502
https://search.proquest.com/docview/1627078369
https://search.proquest.com/docview/1727675856
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6608436
Volume 34
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1JT9wwFH5iOPVSFegyZZErVb1lstjxchwNUKAFlU3iFiWOo47EZBATDvDrec9Jhk6ReujNShxl-Z7i73t-C8BXJPGWaEFQCWsCYVUZ5LFQgUki7dIk4nlBDv3TM3l0LU5u0ps1SPtcGB-0b4vpqL6djerpbx9beTezYR8nFv46nUgZacFlOICB4ryX6N3vV0W-zS7KLdRFQokuLRgFV3hyRuFxl5PjEfIIFcTkU4mpMw-qJGJGyeri9Ipx_h04-cdKdPgO3nYUko3bR92ANVdvwta4Rvk8e2TfmA_q9N7yLVjsoyieIZNkL1XM79l3V8_J98qOF6zHx5WMfLIsZz9zykq-nT7hoQu3IIKJlsGaOdv3VXZ9JgRr8xvZuGnagEl26Tvq4Og9XB8eXE2Ogq7PQmCFUE1QponQhZHWlMjnClyurKZWVZzbuOCU74S6LE8rUboiT3SOg1SWvEJQjbCp4x9gvZ7X7hMw0ivaOKsqzQUXwsiiksLZKHGo_FI9hLD_wNldW04jIxmC6GRLdDJCBzVJRugM4UuPQ4aWT9sZee3mD4sslgmVKuLS_GMO0jMvieQQPrbYLe_bgz4EtYLqcgJV3l49gwbpK3B3Bvj5v6_chjf0cpTUGJsdWG_uH9wuspum2IPBj3O95236GZr6-fg
link.rule.ids 230,315,733,786,790,891,27946,27947,53816,53818
linkProvider National Library of Medicine
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEB6VcoALr_IIz0VC3Pzc9dp7jFJKUpII0Rb1ZnnXaxHROFXjHOivZ2ZtB1IkJLitvLas1Yy93zf7zQzAOwTxhmCBVwmjPGHS0isikXoqDjObxCEvNAX0Z3M5PhPH58n5HiR9LowT7Ru98OuLpV8vvjlt5eXSBL1OLPg8G0kZZoLL4BbcTujgqyfp3Q84DV2jXSRcyIxEKrrEYKRcwfGcBHIno4mPSCL1IoqqRNSbB3kSYaN4d3v6A3PelE7-thcd3Yev_SpaCcp3f9No31zfKPD4z8t8APc6dMqG7fRD2LP1IzgY1sjMlz_Ye-b0oi4QfwDrQ-TbSwSp7FeB9Cv20dYrCuuyyZr1prclo3AvK9i0oITni8U1Xvpi14Rd0elYs2KHroCvS7JgbeokGzZNq8VkJ65ZD44ew9nRh9PR2OtaOHhGiLTxyiQWmVbSqBKhosad0GTUBYtzE2lOqVRI-YqkEqXVRZwVOEhkySv0FyVMYvkT2K9XtX0GjKhQpqxJq4wLLoSSupLCmjC2SCqTbABBb7n8sq3UkRPDQbPnW7PnZHakOzmZfQBvewPn-FHRSUlR29VmnUcypipIXKq_3IPIz7EtOYCnrVNs39t70wDSHXfZ3kBFvXdn0Alcce_O6M__-8k3cGd8Opvm08n80wu4Swul3MlIvYT95mpjXyGIavRr98n8BNssGwk
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEB5BkRAXXuURnouEuPm567X3GCWEprRRRalUcbG867WIaJyocQ701zOztkNTJA69rey1rNXMer9v_M0MwEcE8YZggVcJozxh0tIrIpF6Kg4zm8QhLzQF9I9n8uBMHJ4n59dafTnRvtFzv75Y-PX8p9NWrhYm6HViwcnxSMowE1wGq7IK7sK9hE6dnqh3H-E0dM12kXQhOxKp6JKDkXYFhzMSyZ2Opj6iidSLKLISUX8e5EqEj-LdI-of3HlTPnntPJo8gh_9SloZyi9_02jfXN0o8nirpT6Ghx1KZcN2yhO4Y-unsD-skaEvfrNPzOlGXUB-H9Zj5N0LBKvsb6H0S_bF1ksK77LpmvUuYEtGYV9WsKOCEp8v5ld46ZtdE4ZF52PNko1dIV-XbMHaFEo2bJpWk8lOXdMeHD2Ds8nn76MDr2vl4Bkh0sYrk1hkWkmjSoSMGk9Ek1E3LM5NpDmlVCH1K5JKlFYXcVbgIJElr9BvlDCJ5c9hr17W9iUwokSZsiatMi64EErqSgprwtgiuUyyAQS99fJVW7EjJ6aDps-3ps_J9Eh7cjL9AD70Rs5xc9Efk6K2y806j2RM1ZC4VP-ZgwjQsS45gBetY2zf23vUANIdl9lOoOLeu3fQEVyR787wr2795Hu4fzKe5EfT2dfX8IDWSSmUkXoDe83lxr5FLNXod27X_AG0fR2J
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Dopamine+transporter+genotype+is+associated+with+a+lateralized+resistance+to+distraction+during+attention+selection&rft.jtitle=The+Journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Newman%2C+Daniel+P&rft.au=Cummins%2C+Tarrant+D+R&rft.au=Tong%2C+Janette+H+S&rft.au=Johnson%2C+Beth+P&rft.date=2014-11-19&rft.eissn=1529-2401&rft.volume=34&rft.issue=47&rft.spage=15743&rft.epage=15750&rft_id=info:doi/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.2327-14.2014&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0270-6474&client=summon