Improving power in functional magnetic resonance imaging by moving beyond cluster-level inference
Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture of broad-scale effects distributed throughout the brain, suggesting that many focal reports may only reflect the tip of the iceberg of underly...
Saved in:
Published in | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS Vol. 119; no. 32; pp. 1 - 10 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
National Academy of Sciences
09.08.2022
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0027-8424 1091-6490 1091-6490 |
DOI | 10.1073/pnas.2203020119 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture of broad-scale effects distributed throughout the brain, suggesting that many focal reports may only reflect the tip of the iceberg of underlying effects. How focal versus broad-scale perspectives influence the inferences we make has not yet been comprehensively evaluated using real data. Here, we compare sensitivity and specificity across procedures representing multiple levels of inference using an empirical benchmarking procedure that resamples taskbased connectomes from the Human Connectome Project dataset (~1,000 subjects, 7 tasks, 3 resampling group sizes, 7 inferential procedures). Only broad-scale (network and whole brain) procedures obtained the traditional 80% statistical power level to detect an average effect, reflecting >20% more statistical power than focal (edge and cluster) procedures. Power also increased substantially for false discovery rate– compared with familywise error rate–controlling procedures. The downsides are fairly limited; the loss in specificity for broad-scale and FDR procedures was relatively modest compared to the gains in power. Furthermore, the broad-scale methods we introduce are simple, fast, and easy to use, providing a straightforward starting point for researchers. This also points to the promise of more sophisticated broad-scale methods for not only functional connectivity but also related fields, including task-based activation. Altogether, this work demonstrates that shifting the scale of inference and choosing FDR control are both immediately attainable and can help remedy the issues with statistical power plaguing typical studies in the field. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture of broad-scale effects distributed throughout the brain, suggesting that many focal reports may only reflect the tip of the iceberg of underlying effects. How focal versus broad-scale perspectives influence the inferences we make has not yet been comprehensively evaluated using real data. Here, we compare sensitivity and specificity across procedures representing multiple levels of inference using an empirical benchmarking procedure that resamples task-based connectomes from the Human Connectome Project dataset (∼1,000 subjects, 7 tasks, 3 resampling group sizes, 7 inferential procedures). Only broad-scale (network and whole brain) procedures obtained the traditional 80% statistical power level to detect an average effect, reflecting >20% more statistical power than focal (edge and cluster) procedures. Power also increased substantially for false discovery rate– compared with familywise error rate–controlling procedures. The downsides are fairly limited; the loss in specificity for broad-scale and FDR procedures was relatively modest compared to the gains in power. Furthermore, the broad-scale methods we introduce are simple, fast, and easy to use, providing a straightforward starting point for researchers. This also points to the promise of more sophisticated broad-scale methods for not only functional connectivity but also related fields, including task-based activation. Altogether, this work demonstrates that shifting the scale of inference and choosing FDR control are both immediately attainable and can help remedy the issues with statistical power plaguing typical studies in the field. Localizing cognitive function to distinct brain areas has been a mainstay of human brain research since early reports that focal injuries produce changes in behavior. Yet, accumulating evidence shows that areas do not act in isolation. Here, we evaluate the practical implications of the localizationist perspective by comparing the performance of localizing versus broad-scale statistical procedures in real connectome data (1,000 subjects performing 7 tasks). We find that popular localizing procedures miss substantially more true effects than simple broad-scale procedures. By highlighting the power of simple alternatives, we argue that moving beyond localization is viable and can help unlock opportunities for human neuroscience discovery. Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture of broad-scale effects distributed throughout the brain, suggesting that many focal reports may only reflect the tip of the iceberg of underlying effects. How focal versus broad-scale perspectives influence the inferences we make has not yet been comprehensively evaluated using real data. Here, we compare sensitivity and specificity across procedures representing multiple levels of inference using an empirical benchmarking procedure that resamples task-based connectomes from the Human Connectome Project dataset (∼1,000 subjects, 7 tasks, 3 resampling group sizes, 7 inferential procedures). Only broad-scale (network and whole brain) procedures obtained the traditional 80% statistical power level to detect an average effect, reflecting >20% more statistical power than focal (edge and cluster) procedures. Power also increased substantially for false discovery rate– compared with familywise error rate–controlling procedures. The downsides are fairly limited; the loss in specificity for broad-scale and FDR procedures was relatively modest compared to the gains in power. Furthermore, the broad-scale methods we introduce are simple, fast, and easy to use, providing a straightforward starting point for researchers. This also points to the promise of more sophisticated broad-scale methods for not only functional connectivity but also related fields, including task-based activation. Altogether, this work demonstrates that shifting the scale of inference and choosing FDR control are both immediately attainable and can help remedy the issues with statistical power plaguing typical studies in the field. Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture of broad-scale effects distributed throughout the brain, suggesting that many focal reports may only reflect the tip of the iceberg of underlying effects. How focal versus broad-scale perspectives influence the inferences we make has not yet been comprehensively evaluated using real data. Here, we compare sensitivity and specificity across procedures representing multiple levels of inference using an empirical benchmarking procedure that resamples taskbased connectomes from the Human Connectome Project dataset (~1,000 subjects, 7 tasks, 3 resampling group sizes, 7 inferential procedures). Only broad-scale (network and whole brain) procedures obtained the traditional 80% statistical power level to detect an average effect, reflecting >20% more statistical power than focal (edge and cluster) procedures. Power also increased substantially for false discovery rate– compared with familywise error rate–controlling procedures. The downsides are fairly limited; the loss in specificity for broad-scale and FDR procedures was relatively modest compared to the gains in power. Furthermore, the broad-scale methods we introduce are simple, fast, and easy to use, providing a straightforward starting point for researchers. This also points to the promise of more sophisticated broad-scale methods for not only functional connectivity but also related fields, including task-based activation. Altogether, this work demonstrates that shifting the scale of inference and choosing FDR control are both immediately attainable and can help remedy the issues with statistical power plaguing typical studies in the field. Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture of broad-scale effects distributed throughout the brain, suggesting that many focal reports may only reflect the tip of the iceberg of underlying effects. How focal versus broad-scale perspectives influence the inferences we make has not yet been comprehensively evaluated using real data. Here, we compare sensitivity and specificity across procedures representing multiple levels of inference using an empirical benchmarking procedure that resamples task-based connectomes from the Human Connectome Project dataset (∼1,000 subjects, 7 tasks, 3 resampling group sizes, 7 inferential procedures). Only broad-scale (network and whole brain) procedures obtained the traditional 80% statistical power level to detect an average effect, reflecting >20% more statistical power than focal (edge and cluster) procedures. Power also increased substantially for false discovery rate- compared with familywise error rate-controlling procedures. The downsides are fairly limited; the loss in specificity for broad-scale and FDR procedures was relatively modest compared to the gains in power. Furthermore, the broad-scale methods we introduce are simple, fast, and easy to use, providing a straightforward starting point for researchers. This also points to the promise of more sophisticated broad-scale methods for not only functional connectivity but also related fields, including task-based activation. Altogether, this work demonstrates that shifting the scale of inference and choosing FDR control are both immediately attainable and can help remedy the issues with statistical power plaguing typical studies in the field.Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture of broad-scale effects distributed throughout the brain, suggesting that many focal reports may only reflect the tip of the iceberg of underlying effects. How focal versus broad-scale perspectives influence the inferences we make has not yet been comprehensively evaluated using real data. Here, we compare sensitivity and specificity across procedures representing multiple levels of inference using an empirical benchmarking procedure that resamples task-based connectomes from the Human Connectome Project dataset (∼1,000 subjects, 7 tasks, 3 resampling group sizes, 7 inferential procedures). Only broad-scale (network and whole brain) procedures obtained the traditional 80% statistical power level to detect an average effect, reflecting >20% more statistical power than focal (edge and cluster) procedures. Power also increased substantially for false discovery rate- compared with familywise error rate-controlling procedures. The downsides are fairly limited; the loss in specificity for broad-scale and FDR procedures was relatively modest compared to the gains in power. Furthermore, the broad-scale methods we introduce are simple, fast, and easy to use, providing a straightforward starting point for researchers. This also points to the promise of more sophisticated broad-scale methods for not only functional connectivity but also related fields, including task-based activation. Altogether, this work demonstrates that shifting the scale of inference and choosing FDR control are both immediately attainable and can help remedy the issues with statistical power plaguing typical studies in the field. |
Author | Mejia, Amanda F. Zalesky, Andrew Noble, Stephanie Scheinost, Dustin |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Stephanie surname: Noble fullname: Noble, Stephanie – sequence: 2 givenname: Amanda F. surname: Mejia fullname: Mejia, Amanda F. – sequence: 3 givenname: Andrew surname: Zalesky fullname: Zalesky, Andrew – sequence: 4 givenname: Dustin surname: Scheinost fullname: Scheinost, Dustin |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35925887$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kUtv1DAUhS1URKeFNStQJDZs0voRx_YGCVU8KlViA2vLca4HjxI72MlU8-9xmDJAF6wsXX_n3GOfC3QWYgCEXhJ8RbBg11Mw-YpSzDDFhKgnaEOwInXbKHyGNhhTUcuGNufoIucdxlhxiZ-hc8YV5VKKDTK345Ti3odtNcV7SJUPlVuCnX0MZqhGsw0we1slyGUQLFS-zFa8O1TjUdjBIYa-ssOSZ0j1AHsYio-DBEXwHD11Zsjw4uG8RN8-fvh687m--_Lp9ub9XW2bhs11J6wE2mMnleG07du-hOedop1TTvWSO9cBNI1smXGYOGkd61vWOeo44VyyS_Tu6Dst3Qi9hTAnM-gplcDpoKPx-t-b4L_rbdxrxQRpG1oM3j4YpPhjgTzr0WcLw2ACxCVr2qqyHLe_dr15hO7iksqHFUpgLBSjZKVe_53oFOX37xfg-gjYFHNO4E4IwXrtV6_96j_9FgV_pLB-NmtZ5Ul--I_u1VG3y3NMpzVUEEEaQdhPTjG2IA |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1021_acs_chemrev_3c00401 crossref_primary_10_3233_BPL_240003 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroimage_2022_119742 crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0209_24_2024 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2024_105729 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2024_1508077 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_biopsych_2024_10_007 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ejon_2023_102499 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psychres_2024_116113 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bpsc_2023_09_002 crossref_primary_10_1038_s44271_024_00114_4 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_024_50248_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bpsc_2022_12_006 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jpsychires_2022_10_067 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bbr_2024_115272 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1010634 crossref_primary_10_1038_s44220_023_00101_4 crossref_primary_10_1214_24_AOAS1880 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15889 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41380_024_02767_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_nicl_2024_103630 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2022_12_011 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2023_05_006 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41586_024_08260_9 crossref_primary_10_1155_2024_3103115 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_patter_2023_100756 crossref_primary_10_1162_imag_a_00138 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2023_1206604 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41467_024_46150_w |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116468 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118648 10.1111/1467-9868.00346 10.1007/978-3-030-59728-3_44 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016 10.1073/pnas.1121049109 10.1038/s41467-020-17368-1 10.1038/s41593-020-00719-y 10.1038/nrn.2016.167 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00493 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:3<160::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-1 10.1073/pnas.0506580102 10.1093/biomet/73.3.751 10.1371/journal.pone.0184923 10.1093/cercor/bhaa290 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014 10.1093/cercor/bhx230 10.1109/MEMB.2006.1607672 10.1073/pnas.1602413113 10.1080/01621459.2019.1679638 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.090 10.1136/bmj.310.6973.170 10.1006/nimg.1996.0074 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117477 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.060 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117164 10.1002/hbm.25561 10.1002/hbm.20919 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108066 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.081 10.1073/pnas.1614502114 10.1002/hbm.24982 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.12.012 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116233 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041 10.1073/pnas.0905267106 10.1162/netn_a_00234 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x 10.1038/nn.3776 10.1101/2021.07.15.452548 10.1038/nrn3475 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x 10.1137/110832380 10.1007/s10548-019-00744-6 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118647 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118908 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.02.019 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.058 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.059 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2022 the Author(s) Copyright National Academy of Sciences Aug 9, 2022 Copyright © 2022 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2022 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2022 the Author(s) – notice: Copyright National Academy of Sciences Aug 9, 2022 – notice: Copyright © 2022 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2022 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD C1K FR3 H94 M7N P64 RC3 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1073/pnas.2203020119 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Entomology Abstracts (Full archive) Immunology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Virology and AIDS Abstracts Technology Research Database Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Engineering Research Database AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Virology and AIDS Abstracts Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts Technology Research Database Nucleic Acids Abstracts Ecology Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management Entomology Abstracts Genetics Abstracts Animal Behavior Abstracts Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B) Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C) AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Immunology Abstracts Engineering Research Database Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | CrossRef Virology and AIDS Abstracts MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Sciences (General) Statistics |
EISSN | 1091-6490 |
EndPage | 10 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC9371642 35925887 10_1073_pnas_2203020119 27171471 |
Genre | Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIMH NIH HHS grantid: U54 MH091657 – fundername: HHS | NIH | National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grantid: K00MH122372 – fundername: HHS | NIH | National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering (NIBIB) grantid: R01EB027119 – fundername: NIMH NIH HHS grantid: K00 MH122372 – fundername: NCATS NIH HHS grantid: UL1 TR001863 – fundername: NIBIB NIH HHS grantid: R01 EB027119 |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .55 0R~ 123 29P 2FS 2WC 4.4 53G 5RE 5VS 85S AACGO AAFWJ AANCE ABOCM ABPLY ABPPZ ABTLG ABZEH ACGOD ACIWK ACNCT ACPRK AENEX AFFNX AFOSN AFRAH ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BKOMP CS3 D0L DIK DU5 E3Z EBS F5P FRP GX1 H13 HH5 HYE JENOY JLS JSG JST KQ8 L7B LU7 N9A N~3 O9- OK1 PNE PQQKQ R.V RHI RNA RNS RPM RXW SJN TAE TN5 UKR W8F WH7 WOQ WOW X7M XSW Y6R YBH YKV YSK ZCA ~02 ~KM AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 2AX 7QG 7QL 7QP 7QR 7SN 7SS 7T5 7TK 7TM 7TO 7U9 8FD ABBHK AEUPB AEXZC C1K DCCCD FR3 H94 IPSME JAAYA JBMMH JHFFW JKQEH JLXEF JPM M7N P64 RC3 SA0 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c443t-b7c8e2d0f89a526d6d0275b92bf9f9d85ffbee44863af01f8cf3d63bf2f515583 |
ISSN | 0027-8424 1091-6490 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 17:42:28 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 04:48:46 EDT 2025 Wed Aug 13 07:07:38 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:09:27 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:57 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:03:21 EDT 2025 Thu May 29 08:51:22 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 32 |
Keywords | empirical fMRI power inference network |
Language | English |
License | This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC BY). |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c443t-b7c8e2d0f89a526d6d0275b92bf9f9d85ffbee44863af01f8cf3d63bf2f515583 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 Author contributions: S.N. conceived the study; S.N. and D.S. designed research; A.F.M. and A.Z. provided conceptual advice; S.N. performed research and contributed new analytic tools; A.F.M. proposed the inclusion of a multivariate inferential procedure; A.Z. proposed the inclusion of specificity benchmarking and community randomization tests; S.N. wrote the paper with contributions from A.F.M., A.Z., and D.S.; and D.S. provided supervision. Edited by Marcus Raichle, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO; received March 7, 2022; accepted June 23, 2022 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-6301-1167 0000-0002-4804-5553 0000-0002-4312-8974 |
OpenAccessLink | https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9371642 |
PMID | 35925887 |
PQID | 2700793218 |
PQPubID | 42026 |
PageCount | 10 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9371642 proquest_miscellaneous_2698630658 proquest_journals_2700793218 pubmed_primary_35925887 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2203020119 crossref_citationtrail_10_1073_pnas_2203020119 jstor_primary_27171471 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2022-08-09 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2022-08-09 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 08 year: 2022 text: 2022-08-09 day: 09 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: Washington |
PublicationTitle | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A |
PublicationYear | 2022 |
Publisher | National Academy of Sciences |
Publisher_xml | – name: National Academy of Sciences |
References | e_1_3_4_3_2 e_1_3_4_1_2 e_1_3_4_9_2 e_1_3_4_7_2 e_1_3_4_40_2 e_1_3_4_5_2 e_1_3_4_23_2 e_1_3_4_44_2 Raichle M. E. (e_1_3_4_20_2) 2019 e_1_3_4_21_2 e_1_3_4_42_2 e_1_3_4_27_2 e_1_3_4_48_2 e_1_3_4_25_2 e_1_3_4_46_2 e_1_3_4_29_2 e_1_3_4_30_2 e_1_3_4_51_2 e_1_3_4_34_2 e_1_3_4_55_2 e_1_3_4_32_2 e_1_3_4_53_2 e_1_3_4_15_2 e_1_3_4_38_2 e_1_3_4_13_2 e_1_3_4_36_2 e_1_3_4_19_2 e_1_3_4_17_2 e_1_3_4_2_2 e_1_3_4_8_2 e_1_3_4_41_2 e_1_3_4_6_2 e_1_3_4_4_2 e_1_3_4_22_2 e_1_3_4_45_2 e_1_3_4_43_2 e_1_3_4_26_2 e_1_3_4_49_2 e_1_3_4_24_2 e_1_3_4_47_2 e_1_3_4_28_2 Noble S. (e_1_3_4_11_2) 2020; 12267 e_1_3_4_52_2 e_1_3_4_50_2 e_1_3_4_12_2 e_1_3_4_33_2 e_1_3_4_54_2 e_1_3_4_10_2 e_1_3_4_31_2 e_1_3_4_16_2 e_1_3_4_37_2 e_1_3_4_14_2 e_1_3_4_35_2 e_1_3_4_56_2 e_1_3_4_18_2 e_1_3_4_39_2 |
References_xml | – ident: e_1_3_4_4_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116468 – ident: e_1_3_4_25_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118648 – ident: e_1_3_4_51_2 doi: 10.1111/1467-9868.00346 – ident: e_1_3_4_6_2 doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-59728-3_44 – ident: e_1_3_4_40_2 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016 – ident: e_1_3_4_5_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121049109 – ident: e_1_3_4_47_2 doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-17368-1 – ident: e_1_3_4_7_2 doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00719-y – ident: e_1_3_4_15_2 doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.167 – ident: e_1_3_4_39_2 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00493 – ident: e_1_3_4_9_2 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:3<160::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-1 – ident: e_1_3_4_48_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506580102 – ident: e_1_3_4_53_2 doi: 10.1093/biomet/73.3.751 – ident: e_1_3_4_3_2 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184923 – ident: e_1_3_4_12_2 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa290 – ident: e_1_3_4_38_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014 – ident: e_1_3_4_54_2 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx230 – ident: e_1_3_4_46_2 doi: 10.1109/MEMB.2006.1607672 – ident: e_1_3_4_17_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113 – ident: e_1_3_4_55_2 – ident: e_1_3_4_32_2 doi: 10.1080/01621459.2019.1679638 – ident: e_1_3_4_35_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.090 – ident: e_1_3_4_50_2 doi: 10.1136/bmj.310.6973.170 – ident: e_1_3_4_1_2 doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0074 – ident: e_1_3_4_30_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117477 – ident: e_1_3_4_28_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.060 – volume: 12267 start-page: 458 year: 2020 ident: e_1_3_4_11_2 article-title: The constrained network-based statistic: A new level of inference for neuroimaging publication-title: Med. Image Comput. Comput. Assist Interv. – ident: e_1_3_4_16_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117164 – ident: e_1_3_4_22_2 doi: 10.1002/hbm.25561 – ident: e_1_3_4_10_2 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20919 – ident: e_1_3_4_42_2 doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108066 – ident: e_1_3_4_56_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041 – ident: e_1_3_4_49_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.081 – ident: e_1_3_4_36_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1614502114 – ident: e_1_3_4_13_2 doi: 10.1002/hbm.24982 – ident: e_1_3_4_44_2 doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.12.012 – ident: e_1_3_4_23_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116233 – ident: e_1_3_4_2_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041 – ident: e_1_3_4_37_2 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106 – ident: e_1_3_4_41_2 doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00234 – ident: e_1_3_4_14_2 doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9 – ident: e_1_3_4_33_2 doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x – ident: e_1_3_4_21_2 doi: 10.1038/nn.3776 – ident: e_1_3_4_45_2 doi: 10.1101/2021.07.15.452548 – start-page: 97 volume-title: The Neocortex year: 2019 ident: e_1_3_4_20_2 – ident: e_1_3_4_34_2 doi: 10.1038/nrn3475 – ident: e_1_3_4_18_2 doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x – ident: e_1_3_4_29_2 doi: 10.1137/110832380 – ident: e_1_3_4_24_2 doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00744-6 – ident: e_1_3_4_43_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118647 – ident: e_1_3_4_31_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118908 – ident: e_1_3_4_26_2 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.02.019 – ident: e_1_3_4_52_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061 – ident: e_1_3_4_27_2 – ident: e_1_3_4_19_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.058 – ident: e_1_3_4_8_2 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.059 |
SSID | ssj0009580 |
Score | 2.5234675 |
Snippet | Inference in neuroimaging typically occurs at the level of focal brain areas or circuits. Yet, increasingly, well-powered studies paint a much richer picture... Localizing cognitive function to distinct brain areas has been a mainstay of human brain research since early reports that focal injuries produce changes in... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref jstor |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 1 |
SubjectTerms | Biological Sciences Brain Brain - physiology Brain mapping Clusters Connectome - methods Functional magnetic resonance imaging Humans Inference Magnetic resonance imaging Magnetic Resonance Imaging - methods Medical imaging Neural networks Neuroimaging Resampling Statistics |
Title | Improving power in functional magnetic resonance imaging by moving beyond cluster-level inference |
URI | https://www.jstor.org/stable/27171471 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35925887 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2700793218 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2698630658 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC9371642 |
Volume | 119 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3Pb9MwFLbKuHBBDBiEDWQkDkNVSus4sX2sgDFxqHbYpN6qOLFZpzabaHtgfwt_LO_FjpOWTRpcoipxrKTfl_een98PQj4Ya4uMwUoVTI8k5lqYOMcPLwVCaTC3S1jNYbTFJDu94N-n6bTX-92JWtqs9aC4vTOv5H9QhXOAK2bJ_gOyYVI4Ab8BXzgCwnB8EMatR-AGm52h7wL1lHfvLfMflXElmtHexi94vnRNicDmXLobtctgKRYbrJgQLzCGqI7QqrMAu6brWVB1qyawYNJ4EsdtXooXFqt-3D-btF2OJ9i3JoSVYV57gNpcuYDd8RK9Gv2TQXBmg_Lyvt1O3GW9a3Rp5pVPV_mC_ciqrvcCFr51tEVXIjPQktzlUQ-ME8Jgw8QZd21Eg5QeqQ4dnU_0L_EP8gp7Flf5asAYsA2tG9VqumZ3f0cBhrDEekNeJDOcYNZO8Ig8ZkLUQQDfpqNOSWfpEpz8GzSFo0TyaecJtmweF_Z614JmNy63Y-icPyNP_QqFjh3d9knPVM_JfgMrPfaFyj--IHngH635R-cVbflHG_7RwD_q-Uf1L-r4Rx3_6Bb_aODfS3Jx8vX882nsW3bEBefJOtaikIaVQytVnrKszErcFteKaausKmVqrTaGc5kluR2OrCxsUmaJtsxiryGZHJC96royrwmVxpicFaVWRcHZUCspSxjPjZRKGGkiMmj-1Fnh69ljW5XF7B4YI3IcbrhxpVzuH3pQoxTGMTESI7DjInLUwDbzggDuE0MsMwnGckTeh8sgpnHvLa_M9QbGZAreGe39iLxyKIfJk1SxFJR9RMQW_mEAloDfvlLNL-tS8FjNMuPszcNf7ZA8ab_DI7K3_rkxb8GuXut3Nb3_APGv0d8 |
linkProvider | Geneva Foundation for Medical Education and Research |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Improving+power+in+functional+magnetic+resonance+imaging+by+moving+beyond+cluster-level+inference&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+National+Academy+of+Sciences+-+PNAS&rft.au=Noble%2C+Stephanie&rft.au=Mejia%2C+Amanda+F.&rft.au=Zalesky%2C+Andrew&rft.au=Scheinost%2C+Dustin&rft.date=2022-08-09&rft.issn=0027-8424&rft.eissn=1091-6490&rft.volume=119&rft.issue=32&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073%2Fpnas.2203020119&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_1073_pnas_2203020119 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon |