Non-linear development of postural control and strategy use in young children: a longitudinal study
This longitudinal analysis confirmed a non-monotonic pattern of postural control development in children from age 5 to 8 years suggested by previous cross-sectional studies. Postural control was considered in terms of control strategy and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviatio...
Saved in:
Published in | Experimental brain research Vol. 140; no. 4; pp. 420 - 431 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Berlin
Springer
01.10.2001
Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
DOI | 10.1007/s002210100835 |
Cover
Loading…
Abstract | This longitudinal analysis confirmed a non-monotonic pattern of postural control development in children from age 5 to 8 years suggested by previous cross-sectional studies. Postural control was considered in terms of control strategy and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of center of pressure (COP) velocity; and of effectiveness and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of COP anteroposterior (a-p) excursion. Periods of significant variability were used to indicate behavioral transitions. Seventeen healthy children (nine males, eight females) aged 5-6 years (61.5-75 months) were tested at 3- to 4-month intervals up to age 8 years (83-97 months) in eyes-open quiet stance on a force platform for 30 s in each of ten trials. Data were reorganized into six developmental categories based on adjacent test dates prior to (-1) and after (+1, +2, +3, +4) a subject's trial with the lowest COP velocity (0). Developmental category is proposed to represent level of sensorimotor integrative skill. Within-subject ANCOVAs revealed a significant effect (P<0.0001) for developmental category with covariance due to height, weight and actual age removed. Post hoc tests showed a significant effect (P<0.0001) on measures of strategy. However, differences in COP velocity (type of strategy used) and differences in its variability (denoting a transition between strategies) were not always coincident. Performance outcome (COP a-p excursion) changed near linearly across categories. It was concluded that a non-monotonic change in control strategy as indicated by COP velocity describes the development of quiet stance equilibrium. A transition occurs from a primarily open-loop to incorporation of open- and closed-loop components of control. Honing of strategy used precedes and follows transitions. Constriction of velocity and excursion may typify the early stages of bimodal strategy. Developmental categories describe affiliation with the strategy employed and may represent differentiable levels of sensorimotor integrative skill. They may be more useful in assessing progression of equilibrium control than consecutive age in years. |
---|---|
AbstractList | This longitudinal analysis confirmed a non-monotonic pattern of postural control development in children from age 5 to 8 years suggested by previous cross-sectional studies. Postural control was considered in terms of control strategy and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of center of pressure (COP) velocity; and of effectiveness and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of COP anteroposterior (a-p) excursion. Periods of significant variability were used to indicate behavioral transitions. Seventeen healthy children (nine males, eight females) aged 5-6 years (61.5-75 months) were tested at 3- to 4-month intervals up to age 8 years (83-97 months) in eyes-open quiet stance on a force platform for 30 s in each of ten trials. Data were reorganized into six developmental categories based on adjacent test dates prior to (-1) and after (+1, +2, +3, +4) a subject's trial with the lowest COP velocity (0). Developmental category is proposed to represent level of sensorimotor integrative skill. Within-subject ANCOVAs revealed a significant effect (P<0.0001) for developmental category with covariance due to height, weight and actual age removed. Post hoc tests showed a significant effect (P<0.0001) on measures of strategy. However, differences in COP velocity (type of strategy used) and differences in its variability (denoting a transition between strategies) were not always coincident. Performance outcome (COP a-p excursion) changed near linearly across categories. It was concluded that a non-monotonic change in control strategy as indicated by COP velocity describes the development of quiet stance equilibrium. A transition occurs from a primarily open-loop to incorporation of open- and closed-loop components of control. Honing of strategy used precedes and follows transitions. Constriction of velocity and excursion may typify the early stages of bimodal strategy. Developmental categories describe affiliation with the strategy employed and may represent differentiable levels of sensorimotor integrative skill. They may be more useful in assessing progression of equilibrium control than consecutive age in years. This longitudinal analysis confirmed a non-monotonic pattern of postural control development in children from age 5 to 8 years suggested by previous cross-sectional studies. Postural control was considered in terms of control strategy and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of center of pressure (COP) velocity; and of effectiveness and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of COP anteroposterior (a-p) excursion. Periods of significant variability were used to indicate behavioral transitions. Seventeen healthy children (nine males, eight females) aged 5-6 years (61.5-75 months) were tested at 3- to 4-month intervals up to age 8 years (83-97 months) in eyes-open quiet stance on a force platform for 30 s in each of ten trials. Data were reorganized into six developmental categories based on adjacent test dates prior to (-1) and after (+1, +2, +3, +4) a subject's trial with the lowest COP velocity (0). Developmental category is proposed to represent level of sensorimotor integrative skill. Within-subject ANCOVAs revealed a significant effect (P<0.0001) for developmental category with covariance due to height, weight and actual age removed. Post hoc tests showed a significant effect (P<0.0001) on measures of strategy. However, differences in COP velocity (type of strategy used) and differences in its variability (denoting a transition between strategies) were not always coincident. Performance outcome (COP a-p excursion) changed near linearly across categories. It was concluded that a non-monotonic change in control strategy as indicated by COP velocity describes the development of quiet stance equilibrium. A transition occurs from a primarily open-loop to incorporation of open- and closed-loop components of control. Honing of strategy used precedes and follows transitions. Constriction of velocity and excursion may typify the early stages of bimodal strategy. Developmental categories describe affiliation with the strategy employed and may represent differentiable levels of sensorimotor integrative skill. They may be more useful in assessing progression of equilibrium control than consecutive age in years.This longitudinal analysis confirmed a non-monotonic pattern of postural control development in children from age 5 to 8 years suggested by previous cross-sectional studies. Postural control was considered in terms of control strategy and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of center of pressure (COP) velocity; and of effectiveness and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of COP anteroposterior (a-p) excursion. Periods of significant variability were used to indicate behavioral transitions. Seventeen healthy children (nine males, eight females) aged 5-6 years (61.5-75 months) were tested at 3- to 4-month intervals up to age 8 years (83-97 months) in eyes-open quiet stance on a force platform for 30 s in each of ten trials. Data were reorganized into six developmental categories based on adjacent test dates prior to (-1) and after (+1, +2, +3, +4) a subject's trial with the lowest COP velocity (0). Developmental category is proposed to represent level of sensorimotor integrative skill. Within-subject ANCOVAs revealed a significant effect (P<0.0001) for developmental category with covariance due to height, weight and actual age removed. Post hoc tests showed a significant effect (P<0.0001) on measures of strategy. However, differences in COP velocity (type of strategy used) and differences in its variability (denoting a transition between strategies) were not always coincident. Performance outcome (COP a-p excursion) changed near linearly across categories. It was concluded that a non-monotonic change in control strategy as indicated by COP velocity describes the development of quiet stance equilibrium. A transition occurs from a primarily open-loop to incorporation of open- and closed-loop components of control. Honing of strategy used precedes and follows transitions. Constriction of velocity and excursion may typify the early stages of bimodal strategy. Developmental categories describe affiliation with the strategy employed and may represent differentiable levels of sensorimotor integrative skill. They may be more useful in assessing progression of equilibrium control than consecutive age in years. This longitudinal analysis confirmed a non-monotonic pattern of postural control development in children from age 5 to 8 years suggested by previous cross-sectional studies. Postural control was considered in terms of control strategy and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of center of pressure (COP) velocity; and of effectiveness and its variability operationalized by mean and standard deviation of COP anteroposterior (a-p) excursion. Periods of significant variability were used to indicate behavioral transitions. Seventeen healthy children (nine males, eight females) aged 5-6 years (61.5-75 months) were tested at 3- to 4-month intervals up to age 8 years (83-97 months) in eyes-open quiet stance on a force platform for 30 s in each of ten trials. Data were reorganized into six developmental categories based on adjacent test dates prior to (-1) and after (+1, +2, +3, +4) a subject's trial with the lowest COP velocity (0). Developmental category is proposed to represent level of sensorimotor integrative skill. Within-subject ANCOVAs revealed a significant effect (P<0.0001) for developmental category with covariance due to height, weight and actual age removed. Post hoc tests showed a significant effect (P<0.0001) on measures of strategy. However, differences in COP velocity (type of strategy used) and differences in its variability (denoting a transition between strategies) were not always coincident. Performance outcome (COP a-p excursion) changed near linearly across categories. It was concluded that a non-monotonic change in control strategy as indicated by COP velocity describes the development of quiet stance equilibrium. A transition occurs from a primarily open-loop to incorporation of open- and closed-loop components of control. Honing of strategy used precedes and follows transitions. Constriction of velocity and excursion may typify the early stages of bimodal strategy. Developmental categories describe affiliation with the strategy employed and may represent differentiable levels of sensorimotor integrative skill. They may be more useful in assessing progression of equilibrium control than consecutive age in years.[PUBLICATION ABSTRACT] |
Author | Starkes, J. Kirshenbaum, N. Riach, C. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: N. surname: Kirshenbaum fullname: Kirshenbaum, N. – sequence: 2 givenname: C. surname: Riach fullname: Riach, C. – sequence: 3 givenname: J. surname: Starkes fullname: Starkes, J. |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=14072533$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11685395$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqF0c9rFDEUB_AglXZbPXqVINTbaH7MTBJvpfgLil7ac8gkb9aUbLJNMsL-983S1WIpeMkP-LwH7_tO0VFMERB6Q8kHSoj4WAhhjJL2lnx4gVa056yjlIxHaEUI7bteUnWCTku53X-5IMfohNJRDlwNK2R_pNgFH8Fk7OA3hLTdQKw4zXibSl2yCdimWHMK2ESHS82mwnqHlwLYR7xLS1xj-8sHlyF-wgaHFNe-Ls7HVto6uN0r9HI2ocDrw32Gbr58vr781l39_Pr98uKqs33Pa6cmOSkljaUDdQ6EoRMVwNgMhBFBlARplIJxsjBa3hvnppHMQsEkeuAD4Wfo_UPfbU53C5SqN75YCMFESEvRgjE-Du34H6SSCyaUavDdE3ibltwGK1rtY1dSjQ29PaBl2oDT2-w3Ju_0n5AbOD8AU6wJczbR-vLoeiLYwHlz_MHZnErJMGvrq6l-H7_xQVOi9yvX_6y8VXVPqv42ftbfA05Tq18 |
CODEN | EXBRAP |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2023_e17541 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_007_0882_7 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0140235 crossref_primary_10_3390_app12010113 crossref_primary_10_3390_jcm14051666 crossref_primary_10_5507_ag_2017_013 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1651_2227_2011_02322_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2020_e04541 crossref_primary_10_1142_S1013702519500136 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2018_11_024 crossref_primary_10_3389_fphys_2021_625553 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_bjpt_2017_10_006 crossref_primary_10_1136_bjsm_2010_079616 crossref_primary_10_3390_diagnostics11010008 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jns_2022_120516 crossref_primary_10_1097_PHM_0000000000000091 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_008_1271_6 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jmpt_2013_08_009 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jpurol_2016_05_003 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0079623 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2007_04_013 crossref_primary_10_1080_07853890_2021_1986638 crossref_primary_10_1123_jmld_2023_0030 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2010_08_051 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_clinbiomech_2010_08_007 crossref_primary_10_1080_00222895_2019_1643284 crossref_primary_10_1080_J006v26n01_07 crossref_primary_10_1080_00222895_2018_1454396 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2018_03_017 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ridd_2013_05_031 crossref_primary_10_1186_1743_0003_2_29 crossref_primary_10_1590_s1980_6574201600040005 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2017_04_031 crossref_primary_10_29328_journal_japch_1001024 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_014_3886_0 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_otohns_2010_01_028 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_009_1852_z crossref_primary_10_1177_0309364614560941 crossref_primary_10_1111_apa_14933 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2016_06_010 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2010_08_009 crossref_primary_10_3390_sym13101773 crossref_primary_10_1002_aur_70024 crossref_primary_10_1097_PEP_0b013e3181f940f3 crossref_primary_10_9759_hppt_1_39 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2004_11_042 crossref_primary_10_1123_jab_23_3_173 crossref_primary_10_2978_jsas_17_209 crossref_primary_10_3902_jnns_30_3 crossref_primary_10_2174_1874120701711010049 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_005_0109_8 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2018_11_016 crossref_primary_10_1080_23311908_2017_1413928 crossref_primary_10_3390_diagnostics11040637 crossref_primary_10_3390_diagnostics14050513 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jsams_2023_10_004 crossref_primary_10_1051_sm_2011106 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2024_2421220 crossref_primary_10_1097_PEP_0b013e31828e1e9d crossref_primary_10_1177_000348940711600905 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_007_1236_1 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1469_8749_2005_tb01170_x crossref_primary_10_1123_jmld_2018_0016 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2021_07_023 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neulet_2013_09_021 crossref_primary_10_1097_JPO_0000000000000355 crossref_primary_10_3390_children8121147 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_medengphy_2012_05_004 crossref_primary_10_4100_jhse_2011_64_05 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2014_02_002 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4592_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_gaitpost_2009_11_017 crossref_primary_10_3389_fspor_2022_760991 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0966_6362_03_00014_6 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2022_936680 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0966_6362_03_00064_X crossref_primary_10_1080_00207454_2024_2312981 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci10040216 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_humov_2020_102624 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2002 INIST-CNRS Springer-Verlag 2001 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2002 INIST-CNRS – notice: Springer-Verlag 2001 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 0-V 3V. 7QP 7QR 7RV 7TK 7TM 7X7 7XB 88E 88G 88J 8AO 8FD 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABUWG AFKRA ALSLI AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO FR3 FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ K9. KB0 M0S M1P M2M M2R NAPCQ P64 PHGZM PHGZT PJZUB PKEHL POGQB PPXIY PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PRQQA PSYQQ Q9U RC3 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1007/s002210100835 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】 ProQuest Central (Corporate) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Database Neurosciences Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) Psychology Database (Alumni) Social Science Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Pharma Collection Technology Research Database Hospital Premium Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Korea Engineering Research Database Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition) Health & Medical Collection (Alumni) Medical Database Psychology Database Social Science Database Nursing & Allied Health Premium Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Basic Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Central Essentials Nucleic Acids Abstracts Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest Central China Health Research Premium Collection Health & Medical Research Collection Chemoreception Abstracts ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Premium ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest Social Science Journals ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source (Alumni) Engineering Research Database ProQuest One Academic Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection Technology Research Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest Pharma Collection ProQuest Central ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection Genetics Abstracts Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni) ProQuest Medical Library ProQuest Psychology Journals ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest One Psychology |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology Psychology |
EISSN | 1432-1106 |
EndPage | 431 |
ExternalDocumentID | 2503262351 11685395 14072533 10_1007_s002210100835 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GeographicLocations | Canada |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: Canada |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -XW -Y2 -~C -~X .55 .86 .GJ .VR 0-V 06C 06D 0R~ 0VY 199 1N0 1SB 2.D 203 28- 29G 29~ 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 2P1 2VQ 2~H 30V 36B 3O- 3SX 4.4 406 408 409 40D 40E 53G 5GY 5QI 5RE 5VS 67N 67Z 6NX 78A 7RV 7X7 88E 8AO 8FI 8FJ 8TC 8UJ 95- 95. 95~ 96X AABHQ AACDK AAHNG AAIAL AAJBT AAJKR AANXM AANZL AAPKM AARHV AARTL AASML AATNV AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYJJ AAYQN AAYTO AAYXX AAYZH ABAKF ABBBX ABBRH ABBXA ABDBE ABDZT ABECU ABFSG ABFTV ABHLI ABHQN ABIVO ABJNI ABJOX ABKCH ABKTR ABLJU ABMNI ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABQBU ABQSL ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTMW ABULA ABUWG ABWNU ABXPI ACAOD ACBXY ACDTI ACGFS ACHSB ACHXU ACIWK ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACNCT ACOKC ACOMO ACPIV ACPRK ACSTC ACZOJ ADBBV ADHHG ADHIR ADHKG ADIMF ADKNI ADKPE ADRFC ADTPH ADURQ ADYFF ADYPR ADZKW AEBTG AEFIE AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEKMD AEMSY AENEX AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETLH AEVLU AEXYK AEZWR AFBBN AFDZB AFEXP AFFNX AFGCZ AFHIU AFKRA AFLOW AFOHR AFQWF AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGGDS AGJBK AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGQPQ AGRTI AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHAVH AHBYD AHKAY AHMBA AHPBZ AHSBF AHWEU AHYZX AIAKS AIGIU AIIXL AILAN AITGF AIXLP AJBLW AJRNO AJZVZ AKMHD ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ALWAN AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AMYQR AOCGG ARALO ARMRJ ASPBG ATHPR AVWKF AXYYD AYFIA AZFZN AZQEC B-. BA0 BBWZM BDATZ BENPR BGNMA BKEYQ BPHCQ BSONS BVXVI CAG CCPQU CITATION COF CS3 CSCUP DDRTE DL5 DNIVK DPUIP DU5 DWQXO EBD EBLON EBS EIOEI EJD EMB EMOBN EN4 EPAXT ESBYG EX3 FA8 FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FINBP FNLPD FRRFC FSGXE FWDCC FYUFA G-Y G-Z GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNUQQ GNWQR GQ7 GQ8 GXS H13 HF~ HG5 HG6 HMCUK HMJXF HQYDN HRMNR HVGLF HZ~ I09 IAO IHE IHR IHW IJ- IKXTQ INH INR IPY ISR ITC ITM IWAJR IXC IZIGR IZQ I~X I~Z J-C J0Z JBSCW JCJTX JZLTJ KDC KOV KOW KPH L7B LAS LLZTM M1P M2M M2R M4Y MA- N2Q NAPCQ NB0 NDZJH NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O9- O93 O9G O9I O9J OAM OHT OVD P19 P2P PF- PHGZM PHGZT PQQKQ PROAC PSQYO PSYQQ PT4 PT5 Q2X QOK QOR QOS R4E R89 R9I RHV RIG RNI ROL RPX RRX RSV RZK S16 S1Z S26 S27 S28 S3A S3B SAP SBL SBY SCLPG SDH SDM SHX SISQX SJYHP SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SV3 SZN T13 T16 TEORI TSG TSK TSV TUC U2A U9L UG4 UKHRP UOJIU UTJUX UZXMN VC2 VFIZW W23 W48 WH7 WJK WK6 WK8 WOW X7M YLTOR Z45 ZGI ZMTXR ZOVNA ZXP ~EX ~KM ABRTQ IQODW PJZUB PPXIY PRQQA -4W -56 -5G -BR -EM 3V. AAAVM ADINQ CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF GQ6 NPM PKN Z7R Z7U Z7W Z7X Z82 Z83 Z87 Z88 Z8M Z8O Z8Q Z8R Z8V Z8W Z91 Z92 7QP 7QR 7TK 7TM 7XB 8FD 8FK FR3 K9. P64 PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQUKI PRINS PUEGO Q9U RC3 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c443t-9b8b998ac151dde7a1b17e22fe0207098e8a99e6bce6c34addb60f79eb74e3503 |
IEDL.DBID | 7X7 |
ISSN | 0014-4819 |
IngestDate | Sun Aug 24 03:43:02 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 12:39:38 EDT 2025 Sat Aug 23 14:51:50 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 01:30:42 EST 2025 Mon Jul 21 09:17:15 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:50:19 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 04:11:14 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 4 |
Keywords | Human Follow up study Center of mass Strategy Equilibration Orthostatism Postural fitting Motor control Postnatal development Child Posture |
Language | English |
License | http://www.springer.com/tdm CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c443t-9b8b998ac151dde7a1b17e22fe0207098e8a99e6bce6c34addb60f79eb74e3503 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 14 content type line 23 ObjectType-Article-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 |
PMID | 11685395 |
PQID | 902219896 |
PQPubID | 47176 |
PageCount | 12 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_72236522 proquest_miscellaneous_18372799 proquest_journals_902219896 pubmed_primary_11685395 pascalfrancis_primary_14072533 crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s002210100835 crossref_primary_10_1007_s002210100835 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2001-10-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2001-10-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 10 year: 2001 text: 2001-10-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Berlin |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Berlin – name: Germany – name: Heidelberg |
PublicationTitle | Experimental brain research |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Exp Brain Res |
PublicationYear | 2001 |
Publisher | Springer Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer – name: Springer Nature B.V |
SSID | ssj0014370 |
Score | 1.9928924 |
Snippet | This longitudinal analysis confirmed a non-monotonic pattern of postural control development in children from age 5 to 8 years suggested by previous... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 420 |
SubjectTerms | Adaptation, Physiological - physiology Age Aging - physiology Biological and medical sciences Central Nervous System - growth & development Central Nervous System - physiology Child Child, Preschool Cross-sectional studies Equilibrium Feedback - physiology Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Longitudinal Studies Male Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration Movement - physiology Nonlinear Dynamics Observer Variation Postural Balance - physiology Posture - physiology Psychomotor Performance - physiology Velocity Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs |
Title | Non-linear development of postural control and strategy use in young children: a longitudinal study |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11685395 https://www.proquest.com/docview/902219896 https://www.proquest.com/docview/18372799 https://www.proquest.com/docview/72236522 |
Volume | 140 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1Na9wwEB3a5BIoIV9tnI-tDiWniqzXsrTKJaQlIRS6lNLA3owky1BI7E28e9h_nxlZ620O27MHDJrRzBvp6Q3Al1JYl7pMci1dzsXIpNz6UnBsh3InsGKJMT0U_jmR9w_ixzSfRm5OG2mVq5wYEnXZODojv9RYbIjfI69nz5yGRtHlapyg8R62SbmMGF1q2vdbiARU9wIlFVxg5YsSm-HlHNUujMaAQN6UpA8z0-LqVN1Yi824M9Sfuz3YjcCR3XSe3od3vj6Aw5sam-anJbtggcoZzsgPYKdPa8tDcJOm5gQmzQsr1xQh1lRs1rRBdoNFwjozdcnaTq92yRatZ39rtqR8wFaPvq-YYY8NDTlalDRQiwV92iN4uLv98_2ex9EKnBww59qOLTZaxmHBxwSnTGpT5UejyiN8VEM99mOjtZfWeekygUnQymGltLdK-CwfZh9hq25qfwyMBNlGmU9LIytRKYWG0tLtq9Vi6H2VwNfV6hYu6o7T-IvHoldM_tcZCVz05rNOcGOT4eCNq9bWpPmGKDaB05Xvirgx26IPowQ-919xR9E1ial9s2gLTHII6rTebKEQU0kErgl86kJi_e9UIv7R-cl__30KO4HKFjiBZ7A1f1n4c8Q2czsIETyA7W-3k1-_XwFGvPjR |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1LT9wwEB7R5VCkqmqhjxQKPrScanWTOPa6EqpoC1oKrKoKJG7BdhypEiQL2RXKj-I_duw8thy2N84ZxZJnPPONPfMNwIeMaROamFPJTUJZpEKqbcYopkOJYRix2Mg1Cp9O-Pic_bxILlbgvuuFcWWVnU_0jjorjbsj_ywx2Lj6Hv51ekPd0Cj3uNpN0Gis4tjWd5ixVXtHP1C9H6Po8ODs-5i2QwWoW3pGpR5pTDGUwVCHR1uoUIfCRlFuETiJoRzZkZLScm0sNzHD46_5MBfSasFsnAxj_O8TWGUxZjIDWP12MPn1u3-2YLFoel5CRhnG2pbU0_fquWiJ9u8xz4Mg-GyqKtRH3gzSWI50fcQ7fAHPW6hK9hvbegkrtliHjf0C0_TrmuwSXzzqb-XXYa13pPUGmElZUAdf1S3JFkVJpMzJtKw80QdpS-SJKjJSNQy5NZlXlvwpSO08EOnazL8QRa5KN1ZpnrkRXsQz4r6C80fZ99cwKMrCvgXiKOCi2IaZ4jnLhUBBrt17r5ZsaG0ewKdud1PTMp27gRtXac_R_K8yAtjtxacNxccywe0HqlpIO5Y5xM0BbHa6S1tXUKW94Qaw03_FM-weZlRhy3mVoltFGCnlcgmBKI4jVA7gTWMSi7VDjohLJu_-u_YOPB2fnZ6kJ0eT401Y84V0viJxCwaz27l9j8hqprdbeyZw-dhH6C-lAzaF |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9QwEB6VIqFKCEHLYym0PkBPWN0kjr1GQqiirFoKKw5U2luwHUdCKsnS7Arlp_HvmHEeSw_LreeMkijz-iae-QbgVS6si1wiuZYu5SI2Ebc-FxzLodQJzFhiQoPCX2by7FJ8mqfzLfjTz8JQW2UfE0OgzitH_8iPNSYb6u-Rx0XXFfH1dPp-8YvTAik6aO23abQWcuGb31i91e_OT1HVr-N4-vHbhzPeLRjg9BpLru3EYrlhHKY9dHNlIhspH8eFRxClxnriJ0ZrL63z0iUCQ4GV40Jpb5XwSTpO8L534K5K0ohcTM2HWg9RiGqnXyLBBWbdjt4zTO1R3kRPCOjnRjq8vzA1aqZoV2psxrwh900fwoMOtLKT1soewZYvd2HvpMSC_WfDjlhoIw3_53dhZwipzR64WVVyArLmmuXr9iRWFWxR1YHyg3XN8syUOatbrtyGrWrPfpSsoVjE-oHzt8ywq4oWLK1yWubFAjfuY7i8la_-BLbLqvTPgBEZXJz4KDeyEIVSKCgtnfxaLcbeFyN403_dzHWc57R64yob2Jr_VcYIjgbxRUv2sUnw4Iaq1tLEN4cIegT7ve6yLijU2WDCIzgcrqI30xGNKX21qjMMsAgotd4soRDPSQTNI3jamsT62ZFE7KXT5_999iHcQ8fJPp_PLvZhJ3TUhdbEF7C9vF75lwixlvYgGDOD77ftPX8BrQA5VQ |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Non-linear+development+of+postural+control+and+strategy+use+in+young+children%3A+a+longitudinal+study&rft.jtitle=Experimental+brain+research&rft.au=Kirshenbaum%2C+N.&rft.au=Riach%2C+C.&rft.au=Starkes%2C+J.&rft.date=2001-10-01&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft.volume=140&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=420&rft.epage=431&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs002210100835&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_1007_s002210100835 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |