The timing of control signals underlying fast point-to-point arm movements
It is known that proprioceptive feedback induces muscle activation when the facilitation of appropriate motoneurons exceeds their threshold. In the suprathreshold range, the muscle-reflex system produces torques depending on the position and velocity of the joint segment(s) that the muscle spans. Th...
Saved in:
Published in | Experimental brain research Vol. 137; no. 3-4; pp. 411 - 423 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Berlin
Springer
01.04.2001
Springer Nature B.V |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
DOI | 10.1007/s002210000643 |
Cover
Abstract | It is known that proprioceptive feedback induces muscle activation when the facilitation of appropriate motoneurons exceeds their threshold. In the suprathreshold range, the muscle-reflex system produces torques depending on the position and velocity of the joint segment(s) that the muscle spans. The static component of the torque-position relationship is referred to as the invariant characteristic (IC). According to the equilibrium-point (EP) hypothesis, control systems produce movements by changing the activation thresholds and thus shifting the IC of the appropriate muscles in joint space. This control process upsets the balance between muscle and external torques at the initial limb configuration and, to regain the balance, the limb is forced to establish a new configuration or, if the movement is prevented, a new level of static torques. Taken together, the joint angles and the muscle torques generated at an equilibrium configuration define a single variable called the EP. Thus by shifting the IC, control systems reset the EP. Muscle activation and movement emerge following the EP resetting because of the natural physical tendency of the system to reach equilibrium. Empirical and simulation studies support the notion that the control IC shifts and the resulting EP shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements are gradual rather than step-like. However, controversies exist about the duration of these shifts. Some studies suggest that the IC shifts cease with the movement offset. Other studies propose that the IC shifts end early in comparison to the movement duration (approximately, at peak velocity). The purpose of this study was to evaluate the duration of the IC shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements. Subjects made fast (hand peak velocity about 1.3 m/s) planar arm movements toward different targets while grasping a handle. Hand forces applied to the handle and shoulder/elbow torques were, respectively, measured from a force sensor placed on the handle, or computed with equations of motion. In some trials, an electromagnetic brake prevented movements. In such movements, the hand force and joint torques reached a steady state after a time that was much smaller than the movement duration in unobstructed movements and was approximately equal to the time to peak velocity (mean difference < 80 ms). In an additional experiment, subjects were instructed to rapidly initiate corrections of the pushing force in response to movement arrest. They were able to initiate such corrections only when the joint torques and the pushing force had practically reached a steady state. The latency of correction onset was, however, smaller than the duration of unobstructed movements. We concluded that during the time at which the steady state torques were reached, the control pattern of IC shifts remained the same despite the movement block. Thereby the duration of these shifts did not exceed the time of reaching the steady state torques. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that, in unobstructed movements, the IC shifts and resulting shifts in the EP end approximately at peak velocity. In other words, during the latter part of the movement, the control signals responsible for the equilibrium shift remained constant, and the movement was driven by the arm inertial, viscous and elastic forces produced by the muscle-reflex system. Fast movements may thus be completed without continuous control guidance. As a consequence, central corrections and sequential commands may be issued rapidly, without waiting for the end of kinematic responses to each command, which may be important for many motor behaviours including typing, piano playing and speech. Our study also illustrates that the timing of the control signals may be substantially different from that of the resulting motor output and that the same control pattern may produce different motor outputs depending on external conditions. |
---|---|
AbstractList | It is known that proprioceptive feedback induces muscle activation when the facilitation of appropriate motoneurons exceeds their threshold. In the suprathreshold range, the muscle-reflex system produces torques depending on the position and velocity of the joint segment(s) that the muscle spans. The static component of the torque-position relationship is referred to as the invariant characteristic (IC). According to the equilibrium-point (EP) hypothesis, control systems produce movements by changing the activation thresholds and thus shifting the IC of the appropriate muscles in joint space. This control process upsets the balance between muscle and external torques at the initial limb configuration and, to regain the balance, the limb is forced to establish a new configuration or, if the movement is prevented, a new level of static torques. Taken together, the joint angles and the muscle torques generated at an equilibrium configuration define a single variable called the EP. Thus by shifting the IC, control systems reset the EP. Muscle activation and movement emerge following the EP resetting because of the natural physical tendency of the system to reach equilibrium. Empirical and simulation studies support the notion that the control IC shifts and the resulting EP shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements are gradual rather than step-like. However, controversies exist about the duration of these shifts. Some studies suggest that the IC shifts cease with the movement offset. Other studies propose that the IC shifts end early in comparison to the movement duration (approximately, at peak velocity). The purpose of this study was to evaluate the duration of the IC shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements. Subjects made fast (hand peak velocity about 1.3 m/s) planar arm movements toward different targets while grasping a handle. Hand forces applied to the handle and shoulder/elbow torques were, respectively, measured from a force sensor placed on the handle, or computed with equations of motion. In some trials, an electromagnetic brake prevented movements. In such movements, the hand force and joint torques reached a steady state after a time that was much smaller than the movement duration in unobstructed movements and was approximately equal to the time to peak velocity (mean difference < 80 ms). In an additional experiment, subjects were instructed to rapidly initiate corrections of the pushing force in response to movement arrest. They were able to initiate such corrections only when the joint torques and the pushing force had practically reached a steady state. The latency of correction onset was, however, smaller than the duration of unobstructed movements. We concluded that during the time at which the steady state torques were reached, the control pattern of IC shifts remained the same despite the movement block. Thereby the duration of these shifts did not exceed the time of reaching the steady state torques. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that, in unobstructed movements, the IC shifts and resulting shifts in the EP end approximately at peak velocity. In other words, during the latter part of the movement, the control signals responsible for the equilibrium shift remained constant, and the movement was driven by the arm inertial, viscous and elastic forces produced by the muscle-reflex system. Fast movements may thus be completed without continuous control guidance. As a consequence, central corrections and sequential commands may be issued rapidly, without waiting for the end of kinematic responses to each command, which may be important for many motor behaviours including typing, piano playing and speech. Our study also illustrates that the timing of the control signals may be substantially different from that of the resulting motor output and that the same control pattern may produce different motor outputs depending on external conditions. It is known that proprioceptive feedback induces muscle activation when the facilitation of appropriate motoneurons exceeds their threshold. In the suprathreshold range, the muscle-reflex system produces torques depending on the position and velocity of the joint segment(s) that the muscle spans. The static component of the torque-position relationship is referred to as the invariant characteristic (IC). According to the equilibrium-point (EP) hypothesis, control systems produce movements by changing the activation thresholds and thus shifting the IC of the appropriate muscles in joint space. This control process upsets the balance between muscle and external torques at the initial limb configuration and, to regain the balance, the limb is forced to establish a new configuration or, if the movement is prevented, a new level of static torques. Taken together, the joint angles and the muscle torques generated at an equilibrium configuration define a single variable called the EP. Thus by shifting the IC, control systems reset the EP. Muscle activation and movement emerge following the EP resetting because of the natural physical tendency of the system to reach equilibrium. Empirical and simulation studies support the notion that the control IC shifts and the resulting EP shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements are gradual rather than step-like. However, controversies exist about the duration of these shifts. Some studies suggest that the IC shifts cease with the movement offset. Other studies propose that the IC shifts end early in comparison to the movement duration (approximately, at peak velocity). The purpose of this study was to evaluate the duration of the IC shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements. Subjects made fast (hand peak velocity about 1.3 m/s) planar arm movements toward different targets while grasping a handle. Hand forces applied to the handle and shoulder/elbow torques were, respectively, measured from a force sensor placed on the handle, or computed with equations of motion. In some trials, an electromagnetic brake prevented movements. In such movements, the hand force and joint torques reached a steady state after a time that was much smaller than the movement duration in unobstructed movements and was approximately equal to the time to peak velocity (mean difference < 80 ms). In an additional experiment, subjects were instructed to rapidly initiate corrections of the pushing force in response to movement arrest. They were able to initiate such corrections only when the joint torques and the pushing force had practically reached a steady state. The latency of correction onset was, however, smaller than the duration of unobstructed movements. We concluded that during the time at which the steady state torques were reached, the control pattern of IC shifts remained the same despite the movement block. Thereby the duration of these shifts did not exceed the time of reaching the steady state torques. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that, in unobstructed movements, the IC shifts and resulting shifts in the EP end approximately at peak velocity. In other words, during the latter part of the movement, the control signals responsible for the equilibrium shift remained constant, and the movement was driven by the arm inertial, viscous and elastic forces produced by the muscle-reflex system. Fast movements may thus be completed without continuous control guidance. As a consequence, central corrections and sequential commands may be issued rapidly, without waiting for the end of kinematic responses to each command, which may be important for many motor behaviours including typing, piano playing and speech. Our study also illustrates that the timing of the control signals may be substantially different from that of the resulting motor output and that the same control pattern may produce different motor outputs depending on external conditions.It is known that proprioceptive feedback induces muscle activation when the facilitation of appropriate motoneurons exceeds their threshold. In the suprathreshold range, the muscle-reflex system produces torques depending on the position and velocity of the joint segment(s) that the muscle spans. The static component of the torque-position relationship is referred to as the invariant characteristic (IC). According to the equilibrium-point (EP) hypothesis, control systems produce movements by changing the activation thresholds and thus shifting the IC of the appropriate muscles in joint space. This control process upsets the balance between muscle and external torques at the initial limb configuration and, to regain the balance, the limb is forced to establish a new configuration or, if the movement is prevented, a new level of static torques. Taken together, the joint angles and the muscle torques generated at an equilibrium configuration define a single variable called the EP. Thus by shifting the IC, control systems reset the EP. Muscle activation and movement emerge following the EP resetting because of the natural physical tendency of the system to reach equilibrium. Empirical and simulation studies support the notion that the control IC shifts and the resulting EP shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements are gradual rather than step-like. However, controversies exist about the duration of these shifts. Some studies suggest that the IC shifts cease with the movement offset. Other studies propose that the IC shifts end early in comparison to the movement duration (approximately, at peak velocity). The purpose of this study was to evaluate the duration of the IC shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements. Subjects made fast (hand peak velocity about 1.3 m/s) planar arm movements toward different targets while grasping a handle. Hand forces applied to the handle and shoulder/elbow torques were, respectively, measured from a force sensor placed on the handle, or computed with equations of motion. In some trials, an electromagnetic brake prevented movements. In such movements, the hand force and joint torques reached a steady state after a time that was much smaller than the movement duration in unobstructed movements and was approximately equal to the time to peak velocity (mean difference < 80 ms). In an additional experiment, subjects were instructed to rapidly initiate corrections of the pushing force in response to movement arrest. They were able to initiate such corrections only when the joint torques and the pushing force had practically reached a steady state. The latency of correction onset was, however, smaller than the duration of unobstructed movements. We concluded that during the time at which the steady state torques were reached, the control pattern of IC shifts remained the same despite the movement block. Thereby the duration of these shifts did not exceed the time of reaching the steady state torques. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that, in unobstructed movements, the IC shifts and resulting shifts in the EP end approximately at peak velocity. In other words, during the latter part of the movement, the control signals responsible for the equilibrium shift remained constant, and the movement was driven by the arm inertial, viscous and elastic forces produced by the muscle-reflex system. Fast movements may thus be completed without continuous control guidance. As a consequence, central corrections and sequential commands may be issued rapidly, without waiting for the end of kinematic responses to each command, which may be important for many motor behaviours including typing, piano playing and speech. Our study also illustrates that the timing of the control signals may be substantially different from that of the resulting motor output and that the same control pattern may produce different motor outputs depending on external conditions. |
Author | Feldman, A.G. Ghafouri, M. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: A.G. surname: Feldman fullname: Feldman, A.G. – sequence: 2 givenname: M. surname: Ghafouri fullname: Ghafouri, M. |
BackLink | http://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=1100080$$DView record in Pascal Francis https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11355386$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqF0UtLBSEUB3CJom6PZdsYKNpNedRRZxnRk6DN3Q-OV8uY0Zs6Qd--mbrRA6KVR_ydA8f_Nlr3wRuE9gGfAMbiNGFMyFhhzBldQzNglJQAmK-jGcbASiah3kLbKT1NVyrwJtoCoFVFJZ-h2_mjKbLrnX8ogi108DmGrkjuwasuFYNfmNi9Tq9WpVwsg_O5zKF8LwoV-6IPL6Y3PqddtGHHHrO3OnfQ_PJifn5d3t1f3Zyf3ZWaMZpLrmpiKmatpG3bAuNYW0WkYi2pOSGMWrXQnFBCtaaVJIrDggrOagtSSaA76Phj7DKG58Gk3PQuadN1ypswpEZgySvBq38hCElIXU8TD3_BpzDEaf-GCBBQA2ZiVAcrNbS9WTTL6HoVX5vPvxzB0QqopFVno_LapW9uzEjikZUfTMeQUjT2S-BmirT5Eeno6S-vXVbZTUkp1_3R9Qbc7qCI |
CODEN | EXBRAP |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_004_2149_x crossref_primary_10_1080_09593985_2017_1325958 crossref_primary_10_1113_jphysiol_2009_186858 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_108 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00292_2016 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_006_0591_7 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_006_0445_3 crossref_primary_10_1177_15459683241231528 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00183_2022 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00909_2006 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_017_5133_y crossref_primary_10_1113_jphysiol_2001_012809 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_022_06498_1 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00076_2020 crossref_primary_10_3389_fncom_2014_00144 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_plrev_2021_02_004 crossref_primary_10_3389_frvir_2022_943693 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00179_2023 crossref_primary_10_1523_JNEUROSCI_3147_06_2007 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00989_2004 crossref_primary_10_1177_1545968313510973 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00983_2005 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_009_2012_1 crossref_primary_10_1111_j_1469_8986_2009_00952_x crossref_primary_10_1016_S0959_4388_01_00265_3 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_01062_2002 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00483_2018 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00575_2018 crossref_primary_10_3389_fneur_2017_00227 crossref_primary_10_1016_S0006_8993_01_03332_7 crossref_primary_10_1177_15459683211023190 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2001 INIST-CNRS Springer-Verlag 2001. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2001 INIST-CNRS – notice: Springer-Verlag 2001. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION IQODW CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 0-V 3V. 7QP 7QR 7RV 7TK 7TM 7X7 7XB 88E 88G 88J 8AO 8FD 8FI 8FJ 8FK ABUWG AFKRA ALSLI AZQEC BENPR CCPQU DWQXO FR3 FYUFA GHDGH GNUQQ K9. KB0 M0S M1P M2M M2R NAPCQ P64 PHGZM PHGZT PJZUB PKEHL POGQB PPXIY PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI PRINS PRQQA PSYQQ Q9U RC3 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1007/s002210000643 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Pascal-Francis Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection【Remote access available】 ProQuest Central (Corporate) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Database (Proquest) Neurosciences Abstracts Nucleic Acids Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Collection ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) Medical Database (Alumni Edition) Psychology Database (Alumni) Social Science Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Pharma Collection Technology Research Database ProQuest Hospital Collection Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest Central Essentials - QC ProQuest Central ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Korea Engineering Research Database ProQuest Health Research Premium Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Health & Medical Collection Medical Database ProQuest Psychology Database Social Science Database (ProQuest) Nursing & Allied Health Premium Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts ProQuest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central China ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Basic Genetics Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) ProQuest One Psychology ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Central Essentials Nucleic Acids Abstracts Sociology & Social Sciences Collection ProQuest Central China Health Research Premium Collection Health & Medical Research Collection Chemoreception Abstracts ProQuest Central (New) ProQuest Medical Library (Alumni) Social Science Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Hospital Collection Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Hospital Collection (Alumni) Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts Nursing & Allied Health Premium ProQuest Health & Medical Complete ProQuest Social Science Journals ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source (Alumni) Engineering Research Database ProQuest One Academic Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Sociology & Social Sciences Collection Technology Research Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Social Science Journals (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College ProQuest One Health & Nursing ProQuest Pharma Collection ProQuest Central ProQuest Health & Medical Research Collection Genetics Abstracts Health and Medicine Complete (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest One Social Sciences ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest Nursing & Allied Health Source ProQuest Psychology Journals (Alumni) ProQuest Medical Library ProQuest Psychology Journals ProQuest Central (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic ProQuest One Psychology |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: https://www.proquest.com/central sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology Psychology |
EISSN | 1432-1106 |
EndPage | 423 |
ExternalDocumentID | 11355386 1100080 10_1007_s002210000643 |
Genre | Clinical Trial Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -XW -Y2 -~C -~X .55 .86 .GJ .VR 06C 06D 0R~ 0VY 199 1N0 1SB 203 29G 29~ 2J2 2JN 2JY 2KG 2KM 2LR 2P1 2VQ 2~H 30V 36B 3O- 4.4 406 408 409 40D 40E 53G 5GY 5RE 5VS 67N 67Z 6NX 78A 7RV 7X7 88E 8AO 8FI 8FJ 8TC 8UJ 95- 95. 95~ 96X AABHQ AACDK AAHNG AAIAL AAJBT AAJKR AANXM AANZL AAPKM AARHV AARTL AASML AATNV AATVU AAUYE AAWCG AAYIU AAYQN AAYTO AAYXX AAYZH ABAKF ABBBX ABBRH ABBXA ABDBE ABDZT ABECU ABFSG ABFTV ABHLI ABHQN ABIVO ABJNI ABJOX ABKCH ABKTR ABLJU ABMNI ABMQK ABNWP ABPLI ABQBU ABQSL ABSXP ABTEG ABTHY ABTKH ABTMW ABULA ABUWG ABWNU ABXPI ACAOD ACBXY ACDTI ACGFS ACHSB ACHXU ACIWK ACKNC ACMDZ ACMLO ACNCT ACOKC ACOMO ACPIV ACPRK ACSTC ACZOJ ADHHG ADHIR ADHKG ADIMF ADKNI ADKPE ADRFC ADTPH ADURQ ADYFF ADZKW AEBTG AEFQL AEGAL AEGNC AEJHL AEJRE AEKMD AEMSY AENEX AEOHA AEPYU AESKC AETLH AEVLU AEXYK AEZWR AFBBN AFDZB AFFNX AFGCZ AFHIU AFKRA AFLOW AFQWF AFWTZ AFZKB AGAYW AGDGC AGJBK AGMZJ AGQEE AGQMX AGQPQ AGRTI AGWIL AGWZB AGYKE AHBYD AHKAY AHMBA AHPBZ AHSBF AHWEU AHYZX AIAKS AIGIU AILAN AITGF AIXLP AJBLW AJRNO AJZVZ AKMHD ALIPV ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALSLI ALWAN AMKLP AMXSW AMYLF AMYQR AOCGG ARMRJ ASPBG ATHPR AVWKF AXYYD AYFIA AZFZN AZQEC B-. BA0 BDATZ BENPR BGNMA BPHCQ BSONS CAG CCPQU CITATION COF CS3 CSCUP DDRTE DL5 DNIVK DPUIP DU5 DWQXO EBD EBLON EBS EIOEI EJD EMB EMOBN EN4 EPAXT ESBYG FEDTE FERAY FFXSO FIGPU FINBP FNLPD FRRFC FSGXE FWDCC FYUFA G-Y G-Z GGCAI GGRSB GJIRD GNUQQ GNWQR GQ7 GQ8 GXS HF~ HG5 HG6 HMCUK HMJXF HQYDN HRMNR HVGLF HZ~ I09 IAO IHE IHR IHW IJ- IKXTQ INH INR IPY ISR ITM IWAJR IXC IZIGR IZQ I~X I~Z J-C J0Z JBSCW JCJTX JZLTJ KDC KOV KPH L7B LAS LLZTM M1P M2M M2R M4Y MA- N2Q NAPCQ NB0 NPVJJ NQJWS NU0 O9- O93 O9G O9I O9J OAM OVD P19 P2P PF- PHGZM PHGZT PSQYO PSYQQ PT4 PT5 QOK QOR QOS R89 R9I RHV RIG ROL RPX RRX RSV S16 S27 S3A S3B SAP SBL SBY SDH SDM SHX SISQX SJYHP SNE SNPRN SNX SOHCF SOJ SPISZ SRMVM SSLCW SSXJD STPWE SV3 SZN T13 TEORI TSG TSK TSV TUC U2A U9L UG4 UKHRP UOJIU UTJUX UZXMN VC2 VFIZW W23 W48 WH7 WJK WK8 X7M YLTOR Z45 ZGI ZMTXR ZOVNA ZXP ~EX ~KM 0-V 2.D 28- 3SX 5QI AAYJJ ABRTQ ADBBV ADYPR AEFIE AFEXP AFOHR AGGDS AHAVH AIIXL ARALO BBWZM BKEYQ BVXVI EX3 FA8 H13 IQODW ITC KOW NDZJH OHT PJZUB PPXIY PQQKQ PROAC PRQQA Q2X R4E RNI RZK S1Z S26 S28 SCLPG T16 WK6 WOW -4W -56 -5G -BR -EM 3V. AAAVM ADINQ CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF GQ6 NPM PKN Z7R Z7U Z7W Z7X Z82 Z83 Z87 Z88 Z8M Z8O Z8Q Z8R Z8V Z8W Z91 Z92 7QP 7QR 7TK 7TM 7XB 8FD 8FK FR3 K9. P64 PKEHL POGQB PQEST PQUKI PRINS Q9U RC3 PUEGO 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c443t-6a92e54ff83bbb1460cfa28a4b2962243fadc62323cc3582a61d37649f18a813 |
IEDL.DBID | 7X7 |
ISSN | 0014-4819 |
IngestDate | Fri Sep 05 07:29:41 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 00:41:05 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 15:43:45 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:32:39 EST 2025 Mon Jul 21 09:15:43 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 04:11:20 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:08:24 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 3-4 |
Keywords | Facilitation Human Reflex Gripping Proprioception Motor neuron Striated muscle Motor control Equilibrium point Posture Sensorimotor coordination Body movement Upper limb |
Language | English |
License | http://www.springer.com/tdm CC BY 4.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c443t-6a92e54ff83bbb1460cfa28a4b2962243fadc62323cc3582a61d37649f18a813 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 ObjectType-Article-2 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 |
PMID | 11355386 |
PQID | 2717191047 |
PQPubID | 47176 |
PageCount | 13 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_70865765 proquest_miscellaneous_17822991 proquest_journals_2717191047 pubmed_primary_11355386 pascalfrancis_primary_1100080 crossref_primary_10_1007_s002210000643 crossref_citationtrail_10_1007_s002210000643 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2001-04-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2001-04-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 04 year: 2001 text: 2001-04-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Berlin |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Berlin – name: Germany – name: Heidelberg |
PublicationTitle | Experimental brain research |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Exp Brain Res |
PublicationYear | 2001 |
Publisher | Springer Springer Nature B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Springer – name: Springer Nature B.V |
SSID | ssj0014370 |
Score | 1.8403406 |
Snippet | It is known that proprioceptive feedback induces muscle activation when the facilitation of appropriate motoneurons exceeds their threshold. In the... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed pascalfrancis crossref |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 411 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Algorithms Arm Arm - innervation Arm - physiology Balance Biological and medical sciences Elbow Equilibrium Feedback - physiology Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Hand Humans Hypotheses Isometric Contraction - physiology Isotonic Contraction - physiology Latency Models, Neurological Motor control and motor pathways. Reflexes. Control centers of vegetative functions. Vestibular system and equilibration Motor neurons Movement - physiology Muscle contraction Muscle, Skeletal - innervation Muscle, Skeletal - physiology Muscles Postural Balance - physiology Proprioception Proprioception - physiology Velocity Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs |
Title | The timing of control signals underlying fast point-to-point arm movements |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11355386 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2717191047 https://www.proquest.com/docview/17822991 https://www.proquest.com/docview/70865765 |
Volume | 137 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1La9wwEB7a5BIopU36cJukOpScKrq2tJJ9KmlJCIGGUlLYmxnJUilk7e3aOeTfdyRrneSwvRk8IJgZzXwzmgfAx1x63XgRWpQLwaUtJTdoNG-QNFpXWMxiu9j3K3XxS14u5ouUcOtTWeXGJkZD3XQ25Mg_FxR3UGwxk_rL6i8PW6PC62paofEUduPoMtJnvZgCLoICemxBySWX5PrSjM3YOhecV8xtKyke-aRnK-yJPX7ca7EdeEYHdP4CnifkyE5HUb-EJ67dh4PTlqLm5R07YbGWMybJ92Fvsmt3B3BJqsCGsL3rN-s8S8XpLFRukO6x0EW2vgndTsxjP7BV96cd-NDx-MFwvWTLLk4VH_pXcH1-dv3tgqcVCtxKKQausCrcXHpfCmMMWcWZ9ViUKE1RKfLewmNjCQEVwtrQM4sqb8jkyMrnJZa5eA07bde6t8C0a6SyTgvlnJz5EguSJlo0ytgKTZnBpw0Pa5vGi4ctFzf1NBj5IcszOJnIV-NcjW2ER48Eck-dR7ibweFGQHW6fn19rywZfJh-08UJryHYuu62r_OAjQgdb6fQFO5RODbP4M0o-AdnE0wTpXr3_8Pfw95YshYKfQ5hZ1jfuiPCMIM5jop6DLtfz65-_PwHxY_vTw |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1Lb9QwEB6V7YFKCJWWR6ClPkBPWGxsbx6Hqmqh1fa1QmiReotsx0ZI3WTZpEL7o_iPjJ1H28Ny6y1SrETyvL6x55sB-BAKG-eWO4oy41ToRFAlVUxziRodp5INPV3sahKNf4jz69H1GvztuDCurLLzid5R56V2Z-SfGeYdmFsMRXw4_03d1Ch3u9qN0GjU4sIs_2DKVh2cfUX5fmTs9GT6ZUzbqQJUC8FrGsmUmZGwNuFKKXQUQ20lS6RQLI0woHErc42ggHGtHY1URmGOVihSGyYyCTl-9gmsC0doHcD68cnk2_f-2kLwuOG8hIIKjLVtU0_P1XPR0h-mR4I_CILP5rJCedhmkMZqpOsj3ukmPG-hKjlqdOsFrJliC7aPCkzTZ0uyT3zxqD-V34KN3pEut-EcdY_UblzYT1Ja0lbDE1cqgspOHG1tcePoVcTKqibz8ldR07qk_oHIxYzMSt_GvK5ewvQxdvcVDIqyMG-AxCYXkTYxj4wRQ5tIhuojtVSR0qlUSQCfuj3MdNvP3I3VuMn6Tsz3tzyA_X75vGnksWrh7gOB3K0OPb4OYKcTUNbae5XdaWcAe_1rtFR3_SILU95WWejAGMLx1StizC8x_xsF8LoR_L1_Iy7kSfT2_z_fg6fj6dVldnk2uXgHG029nKsy2oFBvbg1uwigavW-VVsC2SMbyj-z0CqZ |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1La9wwEB7SFEqglDTpw23S6NDmVJG1pLXsQykhyZJHG3pIYW9GkqVSyNrbtUPZn9Z_15H8SHLY3HIzWFig-WbmG3keAB9j4WThuC9RZpwKkwqqlZa0UIhomSk2CuVi3y-T05_ifDqersG_vhbGp1X2NjEY6qIy_o78gGHcgbHFSMgD16VF_DiefJ3_oX6ClP_T2o_TaCFyYZd_MXyrv5wdo6w_MTY5uTo6pd2EAWqE4A1NVMbsWDiXcq01Go2RcYqlSmiWJejcuFOFQYLAuDG-pFQlcYEaKTIXpyqNOX72CTyVHEkVqpKcDrEeshDZVr_Eggr0ul17z1C15_1muFZPBL_nDp_PVY2Sce1IjdWcN_i-ySa86EgrOWxR9hLWbLkF24clBuyzJdknIY003M9vwcZgUpfbcI4oJI0fHPaLVI50efHEJ40g7IkvYFtc-0Ir4lTdkHn1u2xoU9HwQNRiRmZVaGje1K_g6jHO9jWsl1Vp3wKRthCJsZIn1oqRSxVDICmjdKJNpnQawef-DHPTdTb3Azau86En890jj2B_WD5vW3qsWrh7TyC3q-PAtCPY6QWUd5pf57c4jWBveI0663_EqNJWN3Uee1qGxHz1ComRJkaC4wjetIK_szcyRJ4m7x7efA-eoXrk384uL97DRps459ONdmC9WdzYXWRSjf4QMEsgf2Qd-Q9aaC1g |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+timing+of+control+signals+underlying+fast+point-to-point+arm+movements&rft.jtitle=Experimental+brain+research&rft.au=Ghafouri%2C+M&rft.au=Feldman%2C+A.G&rft.date=2001-04-01&rft.pub=Springer+Nature+B.V&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft.volume=137&rft.issue=3-4&rft.spage=411&rft.epage=423&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs002210000643&rft.externalDBID=HAS_PDF_LINK |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0014-4819&client=summon |