Cultural evolution of conformity and anticonformity

Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS Vol. 117; no. 24; pp. 13603 - 13614
Main Authors Denton, Kaleda Krebs, Ram, Yoav, Liberman, Uri, Feldman, Marcus W.
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States National Academy of Sciences 16.06.2020
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text

Cover

Loading…
Abstract Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed in many nonhuman animals. Boyd and Richerson used models of conformist and anticonformist bias to explain the evolution of large-scale cooperation, and subsequent research has extended these models. We revisit Boyd and Richerson’s original analysis and show that, with conformity based on more than three role models, the evolutionary dynamics can be more complex than previously assumed. For example, we show the presence of stable cycles and chaos under strong anticonformity and the presence of new equilibria when both conformity and anticonformity act at different variant frequencies, with and without selection. We also investigate the case of population subdivision with migration and find that the common claim that conformity can maintain between-group differences is not always true. Therefore, the effect of conformity on the evolution of cooperation by group selection may be more complicated than previously stated. Finally, using Feldman and Liberman’s modifier approach, we investigate the conditions under which a rare modifier of the extent of conformity or the number of role models can invade a population. Understanding the dynamics of conformistand anticonformist-biased transmission may have implications for research on human and nonhuman animal behavior, the evolution of cooperation, and frequency-dependent transmission in general.
AbstractList The evolutionary dynamics of cultural variants under conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission have implications for humans and nonhuman animals. Humans display both conformist and anticonformist biases, and models of conformist-biased transmission have been proposed to explain large-scale human cooperation. Nonhuman animals have been shown to display conformist biases in mating and foraging decisions. Here, we investigate established mathematical models of conformist and anticonformist bias with and without selection and find complex dynamics, including multiple stable polymorphic equilibria, stable cycles, and chaos. Using modifier theory, we show that evolution will reduce anticonformist bias against a culturally advantageous variant. Migration between subpopulations subject to different selection and conformity pressures can produce interesting polymorphisms or eliminate between-group differences. Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed in many nonhuman animals. Boyd and Richerson used models of conformist and anticonformist bias to explain the evolution of large-scale cooperation, and subsequent research has extended these models. We revisit Boyd and Richerson’s original analysis and show that, with conformity based on more than three role models, the evolutionary dynamics can be more complex than previously assumed. For example, we show the presence of stable cycles and chaos under strong anticonformity and the presence of new equilibria when both conformity and anticonformity act at different variant frequencies, with and without selection. We also investigate the case of population subdivision with migration and find that the common claim that conformity can maintain between-group differences is not always true. Therefore, the effect of conformity on the evolution of cooperation by group selection may be more complicated than previously stated. Finally, using Feldman and Liberman’s modifier approach, we investigate the conditions under which a rare modifier of the extent of conformity or the number of role models can invade a population. Understanding the dynamics of conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission may have implications for research on human and nonhuman animal behavior, the evolution of cooperation, and frequency-dependent transmission in general.
Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed in many nonhuman animals. Boyd and Richerson used models of conformist and anticonformist bias to explain the evolution of large-scale cooperation, and subsequent research has extended these models. We revisit Boyd and Richerson's original analysis and show that, with conformity based on more than three role models, the evolutionary dynamics can be more complex than previously assumed. For example, we show the presence of stable cycles and chaos under strong anticonformity and the presence of new equilibria when both conformity and anticonformity act at different variant frequencies, with and without selection. We also investigate the case of population subdivision with migration and find that the common claim that conformity can maintain between-group differences is not always true. Therefore, the effect of conformity on the evolution of cooperation by group selection may be more complicated than previously stated. Finally, using Feldman and Liberman's modifier approach, we investigate the conditions under which a rare modifier of the extent of conformity or the number of role models can invade a population. Understanding the dynamics of conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission may have implications for research on human and nonhuman animal behavior, the evolution of cooperation, and frequency-dependent transmission in general.Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed in many nonhuman animals. Boyd and Richerson used models of conformist and anticonformist bias to explain the evolution of large-scale cooperation, and subsequent research has extended these models. We revisit Boyd and Richerson's original analysis and show that, with conformity based on more than three role models, the evolutionary dynamics can be more complex than previously assumed. For example, we show the presence of stable cycles and chaos under strong anticonformity and the presence of new equilibria when both conformity and anticonformity act at different variant frequencies, with and without selection. We also investigate the case of population subdivision with migration and find that the common claim that conformity can maintain between-group differences is not always true. Therefore, the effect of conformity on the evolution of cooperation by group selection may be more complicated than previously stated. Finally, using Feldman and Liberman's modifier approach, we investigate the conditions under which a rare modifier of the extent of conformity or the number of role models can invade a population. Understanding the dynamics of conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission may have implications for research on human and nonhuman animal behavior, the evolution of cooperation, and frequency-dependent transmission in general.
Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed in many nonhuman animals. Boyd and Richerson used models of conformist and anticonformist bias to explain the evolution of large-scale cooperation, and subsequent research has extended these models. We revisit Boyd and Richerson's original analysis and show that, with conformity based on more than three role models, the evolutionary dynamics can be more complex than previously assumed. For example, we show the presence of stable cycles and chaos under strong anticonformity and the presence of new equilibria when both conformity and anticonformity act at different variant frequencies, with and without selection. We also investigate the case of population subdivision with migration and find that the common claim that conformity can maintain between-group differences is not always true. Therefore, the effect of conformity on the evolution of cooperation by group selection may be more complicated than previously stated. Finally, using Feldman and Liberman's modifier approach, we investigate the conditions under which a rare modifier of the extent of conformity or the number of role models can invade a population. Understanding the dynamics of conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission may have implications for research on human and nonhuman animal behavior, the evolution of cooperation, and frequency-dependent transmission in general.
Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs when the reverse is true. Conformist and anticonformist bias have been widely documented in humans, and conformist bias has also been observed in many nonhuman animals. Boyd and Richerson used models of conformist and anticonformist bias to explain the evolution of large-scale cooperation, and subsequent research has extended these models. We revisit Boyd and Richerson’s original analysis and show that, with conformity based on more than three role models, the evolutionary dynamics can be more complex than previously assumed. For example, we show the presence of stable cycles and chaos under strong anticonformity and the presence of new equilibria when both conformity and anticonformity act at different variant frequencies, with and without selection. We also investigate the case of population subdivision with migration and find that the common claim that conformity can maintain between-group differences is not always true. Therefore, the effect of conformity on the evolution of cooperation by group selection may be more complicated than previously stated. Finally, using Feldman and Liberman’s modifier approach, we investigate the conditions under which a rare modifier of the extent of conformity or the number of role models can invade a population. Understanding the dynamics of conformistand anticonformist-biased transmission may have implications for research on human and nonhuman animal behavior, the evolution of cooperation, and frequency-dependent transmission in general.
Author Denton, Kaleda Krebs
Ram, Yoav
Feldman, Marcus W.
Liberman, Uri
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Kaleda Krebs
  surname: Denton
  fullname: Denton, Kaleda Krebs
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Yoav
  surname: Ram
  fullname: Ram, Yoav
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Uri
  surname: Liberman
  fullname: Liberman, Uri
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Marcus W.
  surname: Feldman
  fullname: Feldman, Marcus W.
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32461360$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kc1PGzEQxa0KVALtuSdQpF56CYw_13tBqqK2ICFxgbPl9c62jjZ2sL1I_Pd1FBoKBw6WLfv3Zt74HZODEAMS8oXCOYWGX2yCzecMQFBglDYfyIxCSxdKtHBAZgCsWWjBxBE5znkFAK3U8JEccSYU5QpmhC-nsUzJjnN8jONUfAzzOMxdDENMa1-e5jb0dRX_cvWJHA52zPj5eT8h9z9_3C2vFje3v66X328WTgheqolOoZYMmkGgdLJF4azQLTYDlcgQdU-lHrhwjmGvwXbMqq7r67kTA6P8hFzu6m6mbo29w1CqUbNJfm3Tk4nWm9cvwf8xv-OjaTgoTbcFvj0XSPFhwlzM2meH42gDxikbJqCRmoFsKvr1DbqKUwp1vEpRAUJxyip19r-jvZV__1mBix3gUsw54bBHKJhtYmabmHlJrCrkG4XzxW5zqCP58R3d6U63yiWmfRumWqVlnf0vFduk9w
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2418364121
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_47835_w
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2107204118
crossref_primary_10_1111_ele_14184
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tpb_2023_12_003
crossref_primary_10_1093_ej_ueae085
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2412380122
crossref_primary_10_1098_rstb_2021_0400
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jtbi_2023_111429
crossref_primary_10_1177_1354067X241236722
crossref_primary_10_1080_14790718_2022_2144868
crossref_primary_10_1086_724491
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_024_02035_y
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2020_3107
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chaos_2024_114742
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsif_2022_0570
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2022_1951
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pcbi_1012619
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2205914119
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12110_022_09435_x
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2322874121
crossref_primary_10_1103_PhysRevResearch_2_043352
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tpb_2021_10_004
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tpb_2023_06_001
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_021_02677_2
crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_022_03399_y
crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2023_1634
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2414291121
crossref_primary_10_1017_ehs_2021_29
crossref_primary_10_1017_ehs_2024_2
crossref_primary_10_1017_ehs_2024_37
crossref_primary_10_1063_5_0210787
crossref_primary_10_1002_ecm_1483
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_230650
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10816_022_09548_8
crossref_primary_10_1057_s41599_022_01095_7
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2322885121
crossref_primary_10_1111_brv_12899
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2417078122
Cites_doi 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.015
10.1098/rsbl.2009.1014
10.1007/s12110-016-9275-6
10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00018-X
10.1371/journal.pone.0205573
10.1007/s00265-010-1100-3
10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.05.004
10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.07.030
10.1098/rspb.2011.1172
10.1016/j.crhy.2019.05.002
10.3390/e21050521
10.1016/j.tpb.2019.12.004
10.1038/nature13998
10.1016/j.tpb.2007.03.003
10.1073/pnas.83.13.4824
10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.11.012
10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.992
10.1038/nature04047
10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00101-0
10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.003
10.1007/BF01531189
10.1093/beheco/arh008
10.1007/BF02980577
10.1126/science.aat1590
10.1371/journal.pone.0166323
10.1016/0040-5809(82)90029-6
10.2307/1943165
10.1016/j.tpb.2007.04.003
10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.04.023
10.1016/j.tpb.2013.09.010
10.1111/desc.12231
10.1007/s10955-013-0701-4
10.1126/science.7123211
10.1006/jtbi.2000.2202
10.1073/pnas.1619655114
10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.05.029
10.1073/pnas.1621067114
10.1016/S0167-2681(03)00094-5
10.1098/rstb.2010.0046
10.1126/science.1232769
10.1163/156853702320281836
10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.02.003
10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.07.005
10.1006/anbe.1998.0760
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.
Copyright National Academy of Sciences Jun 16, 2020
Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2020
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.
– notice: Copyright National Academy of Sciences Jun 16, 2020
– notice: Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2020
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QL
7QP
7QR
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TK
7TM
7TO
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
M7N
P64
RC3
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1073/pnas.2004102117
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)
Immunology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
Engineering Research Database
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
Entomology Abstracts
Genetics Abstracts
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Immunology Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList CrossRef
MEDLINE - Academic
Virology and AIDS Abstracts


MEDLINE
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Sciences (General)
EISSN 1091-6490
EndPage 13614
ExternalDocumentID PMC7306811
32461360
10_1073_pnas_2004102117
26968511
Genre Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-~X
.55
0R~
123
29P
2AX
2FS
2WC
4.4
53G
5RE
5VS
85S
AACGO
AAFWJ
AANCE
ABBHK
ABOCM
ABPLY
ABPPZ
ABTLG
ABXSQ
ABZEH
ACGOD
ACHIC
ACIWK
ACNCT
ACPRK
ADQXQ
AENEX
AEUPB
AEXZC
AFFNX
AFOSN
AFRAH
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AQVQM
BKOMP
CS3
D0L
DCCCD
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EBS
F5P
FRP
GX1
H13
HH5
HYE
IPSME
JAAYA
JBMMH
JENOY
JHFFW
JKQEH
JLS
JLXEF
JPM
JSG
JST
KQ8
L7B
LU7
N9A
N~3
O9-
OK1
PNE
PQQKQ
R.V
RHI
RNA
RNS
RPM
RXW
SA0
SJN
TAE
TN5
UKR
W8F
WH7
WOQ
WOW
X7M
XSW
Y6R
YBH
YKV
YSK
ZCA
~02
~KM
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QL
7QP
7QR
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TK
7TM
7TO
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
M7N
P64
RC3
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c443t-64b6e85207f4e5c59e4ca489e7f15e2ee8d158f34cc2ed80ab2a6bbded8b4f213
ISSN 0027-8424
1091-6490
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 13:14:24 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 07:37:13 EDT 2025
Mon Jun 30 09:58:08 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 03 06:59:14 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:01:12 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 03:40:22 EDT 2025
Thu May 29 09:12:22 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 24
Keywords migration
multiple equilibria
conformity
modifiers
population differentiation
Language English
License Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.
This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License 4.0 (CC BY-NC-ND).
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c443t-64b6e85207f4e5c59e4ca489e7f15e2ee8d158f34cc2ed80ab2a6bbded8b4f213
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
Contributed by Marcus W. Feldman, April 10, 2020 (sent for review March 4, 2020; reviewed by Jeremy R. Kendal and Mark M. Tanaka)
Reviewers: J.R.K., University of Durham; and M.M.T., University of New South Wales.
Author contributions: U.L. and M.W.F. designed research; K.K.D., Y.R., U.L., and M.W.F. performed research; and K.K.D., Y.R., U.L., and M.W.F. wrote the paper.
ORCID 0000-0002-9653-4458
0000-0001-9779-529X
OpenAccessLink https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC7306811
PMID 32461360
PQID 2414046312
PQPubID 42026
PageCount 12
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_7306811
proquest_miscellaneous_2407582057
proquest_journals_2414046312
pubmed_primary_32461360
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2004102117
crossref_citationtrail_10_1073_pnas_2004102117
jstor_primary_26968511
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2020-06-16
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2020-06-16
PublicationDate_xml – month: 06
  year: 2020
  text: 2020-06-16
  day: 16
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: Washington
PublicationTitle Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS
PublicationTitleAlternate Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
PublicationYear 2020
Publisher National Academy of Sciences
Publisher_xml – name: National Academy of Sciences
References Karlin S. (e_1_3_3_37_2) 1982; 14
Rogers E. M. (e_1_3_3_44_2) 2010
e_1_3_3_17_2
e_1_3_3_16_2
e_1_3_3_19_2
e_1_3_3_38_2
e_1_3_3_18_2
e_1_3_3_39_2
e_1_3_3_13_2
e_1_3_3_36_2
e_1_3_3_12_2
e_1_3_3_15_2
e_1_3_3_34_2
e_1_3_3_14_2
e_1_3_3_35_2
e_1_3_3_32_2
Cavalli-Sforza L. L. (e_1_3_3_1_2) 1981
e_1_3_3_33_2
e_1_3_3_11_2
e_1_3_3_30_2
e_1_3_3_10_2
e_1_3_3_31_2
e_1_3_3_40_2
Boyd R. (e_1_3_3_3_2) 1985
e_1_3_3_6_2
e_1_3_3_5_2
e_1_3_3_8_2
e_1_3_3_7_2
e_1_3_3_28_2
e_1_3_3_9_2
e_1_3_3_27_2
e_1_3_3_29_2
e_1_3_3_24_2
e_1_3_3_47_2
e_1_3_3_23_2
e_1_3_3_48_2
e_1_3_3_26_2
e_1_3_3_45_2
e_1_3_3_25_2
e_1_3_3_46_2
e_1_3_3_2_2
e_1_3_3_20_2
e_1_3_3_43_2
e_1_3_3_4_2
e_1_3_3_22_2
e_1_3_3_41_2
e_1_3_3_21_2
e_1_3_3_42_2
References_xml – ident: e_1_3_3_12_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.015
– volume: 14
  start-page: 204
  year: 1982
  ident: e_1_3_3_37_2
  article-title: Classifications of selection-migration structures and conditions for a protected polymorphism
  publication-title: Evol. Biol.
– ident: e_1_3_3_16_2
  doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.1014
– ident: e_1_3_3_28_2
  doi: 10.1007/s12110-016-9275-6
– ident: e_1_3_3_4_2
  doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00018-X
– ident: e_1_3_3_48_2
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205573
– ident: e_1_3_3_39_2
  doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1100-3
– volume-title: Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach
  year: 1981
  ident: e_1_3_3_1_2
– ident: e_1_3_3_6_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.05.004
– ident: e_1_3_3_19_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.07.030
– ident: e_1_3_3_5_2
  doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1172
– ident: e_1_3_3_33_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.crhy.2019.05.002
– ident: e_1_3_3_34_2
  doi: 10.3390/e21050521
– ident: e_1_3_3_45_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.tpb.2019.12.004
– ident: e_1_3_3_17_2
  doi: 10.1038/nature13998
– ident: e_1_3_3_25_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.tpb.2007.03.003
– ident: e_1_3_3_36_2
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.83.13.4824
– ident: e_1_3_3_14_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.11.012
– ident: e_1_3_3_26_2
  doi: 10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.992
– ident: e_1_3_3_9_2
  doi: 10.1038/nature04047
– ident: e_1_3_3_22_2
  doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00101-0
– ident: e_1_3_3_23_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.003
– volume-title: Diffusion of Innovations
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_3_3_44_2
– ident: e_1_3_3_38_2
  doi: 10.1007/BF01531189
– ident: e_1_3_3_13_2
  doi: 10.1093/beheco/arh008
– ident: e_1_3_3_32_2
  doi: 10.1007/BF02980577
– volume-title: Culture and the Evolutionary Process
  year: 1985
  ident: e_1_3_3_3_2
– ident: e_1_3_3_20_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.aat1590
– ident: e_1_3_3_35_2
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166323
– ident: e_1_3_3_46_2
  doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(82)90029-6
– ident: e_1_3_3_11_2
  doi: 10.2307/1943165
– ident: e_1_3_3_21_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.tpb.2007.04.003
– ident: e_1_3_3_43_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2013.04.023
– ident: e_1_3_3_30_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.tpb.2013.09.010
– ident: e_1_3_3_7_2
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12231
– ident: e_1_3_3_31_2
  doi: 10.1007/s10955-013-0701-4
– ident: e_1_3_3_2_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.7123211
– ident: e_1_3_3_29_2
  doi: 10.1006/jtbi.2000.2202
– ident: e_1_3_3_47_2
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1619655114
– ident: e_1_3_3_24_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.05.029
– ident: e_1_3_3_18_2
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.1621067114
– ident: e_1_3_3_40_2
  doi: 10.1016/S0167-2681(03)00094-5
– ident: e_1_3_3_41_2
  doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0046
– ident: e_1_3_3_10_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.1232769
– ident: e_1_3_3_27_2
  doi: 10.1163/156853702320281836
– ident: e_1_3_3_8_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.02.003
– ident: e_1_3_3_15_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.07.005
– ident: e_1_3_3_42_2
  doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0760
SSID ssj0009580
Score 2.497126
Snippet Conformist bias occurs when the probability of adopting a more common cultural variant in a population exceeds its frequency, and anticonformist bias occurs...
The evolutionary dynamics of cultural variants under conformist- and anticonformist-biased transmission have implications for humans and nonhuman animals....
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
jstor
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 13603
SubjectTerms Animal behavior
Bias
Biological Sciences
Conformity
Cooperation
Cultural Evolution
Evolution
Frequency dependence
Group selection
Humans
Models, Psychological
Social Behavior
Social Conformity
Title Cultural evolution of conformity and anticonformity
URI https://www.jstor.org/stable/26968511
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32461360
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2414046312
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2407582057
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC7306811
Volume 117
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1NT9wwELVaeumlKm1pQ2mVSj1QoWzxV5I9IgpCLd1y2JW2p8hOHIGEsggWDvx6ZmzHSWCRaC9R5NhO1jMej71v3hDyVQnYdOmKJmMtOJ5WpTDnuExqxTJ0iHmuLdpikh7NxM-5nPcirjG6ZKlH5e3KuJL_kSqUgVwxSvYfJBs6hQK4B_nCFSQM1yfJeL-lzTA3_j0OJW5DjNC9dkys0C4U9Z3Rk7B4XbVQgUl7NrjXRZr46X-1k-ycTLq8xT9M46H3v2CRqRTYC6M75LxTtL8LdRMwPwhO8Qeus8uzzgk9r3zpb5h1iMjtH0UwzFiTuEhJbz3B-UhS4fJ_jsyKstbkunhNr1tM9Cwo5aklPXho28EYYULiRlmWdYE5yX03AxbtyZ_icHZ8XEwP5tPn5AWD7QOzBrtPxpw7lgr_aS3lU8a_3-t-4K04wOqqrch9RG3PRZm-Jq_83iLec4qyTp6Z5g1Zb8UXb3uK8W9vCW81Jw6aEy_quFOTGDQnHmrOOzI7PJjuHyU-fUZSCsGX8Mt0anLJdrNaGFnKsRFIYT82WU2lYcbkFZV5zUVZMlPlu0ozlWpdwb0WNaN8g6w1i8Z8ILHileAKGSsoF4KVCrxazSteS1FLPU4jMmqHqSg9tzymODkvLMYh4wWOa9GNa0S2Q4MLR6vyeNUNO-6hHkM-J9gnRGSrFUThJyW0E8gXlXLKIvIlPAaTif-DqcYsrrEO-Mng-Uro-72TW-icI78iqGBEsoFEQwWkYx8-ac5OLS07rJVpTunmE977kbzs5s8WWVteXptP4Nwu9WerqXf1AqQe
linkProvider ABC ChemistRy
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Cultural+evolution+of+conformity+and+anticonformity&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+National+Academy+of+Sciences+-+PNAS&rft.au=Denton%2C+Kaleda+Krebs&rft.au=Ram%2C+Yoav&rft.au=Liberman%2C+Uri&rft.au=Feldman%2C+Marcus+W&rft.date=2020-06-16&rft.issn=1091-6490&rft.eissn=1091-6490&rft.volume=117&rft.issue=24&rft.spage=13603&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073%2Fpnas.2004102117&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon