Eighteen-month-old infants represent nonlocal syntactic dependencies

The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode po...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS Vol. 118; no. 41; pp. 1 - 7
Main Authors Perkins, Laurel, Lidz, Jeffrey
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published United States National Academy of Sciences 12.10.2021
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0027-8424
1091-6490
1091-6490
DOI10.1073/pnas.2026469118

Cover

Loading…
Abstract The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode potentially unbounded dependencies over abstract structural configurations. How does such a system develop in human minds? We show that 18-mo-old infants are capable of representing abstract nonlocal dependencies, suggesting that a core property of syntax emerges early in development. Our test case is English wh-questions, in which a fronted wh-phrase can act as the argument of a verb at a distance (e.g., W h a t ¯ did the chef burn?). Whereas prior work has focused on infants’ interpretations of these questions, we introduce a test to probe their underlying syntactic representations, independent of meaning. We ask when infants know that an object wh-phrase and a local object of a verb cannot co-occur because they both express the same argument relation (e.g., * W h a t ¯ did the chef burn t h e   p i z z a ¯ ). We find that 1) 18 mo olds demonstrate awareness of this complementary distribution pattern and thus represent the nonlocal grammatical dependency between the wh-phrase and the verb, but 2) younger infants do not. These results suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development, during which the computational capacities for representing nonlocal syntactic dependencies become evident.
AbstractList The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode potentially unbounded dependencies over abstract structural configurations. How does such a system develop in human minds? We show that 18-mo-old infants are capable of representing abstract nonlocal dependencies, suggesting that a core property of syntax emerges early in development. Our test case is English wh-questions, in which a fronted wh-phrase can act as the argument of a verb at a distance (e.g., W h a t ¯ did the chef burn?). Whereas prior work has focused on infants’ interpretations of these questions, we introduce a test to probe their underlying syntactic representations, independent of meaning. We ask when infants know that an object wh-phrase and a local object of a verb cannot co-occur because they both express the same argument relation (e.g., * W h a t ¯ did the chef burn t h e   p i z z a ¯ ). We find that 1) 18 mo olds demonstrate awareness of this complementary distribution pattern and thus represent the nonlocal grammatical dependency between the wh-phrase and the verb, but 2) younger infants do not. These results suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development, during which the computational capacities for representing nonlocal syntactic dependencies become evident.
The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode potentially unbounded dependencies over abstract structural configurations. How does such a system develop in human minds? We show that 18-mo-old infants are capable of representing abstract nonlocal dependencies, suggesting that a core property of syntax emerges early in development. Our test case is English wh-questions, in which a fronted wh-phrase can act as the argument of a verb at a distance (e.g., What did the chef burn?). Whereas prior work has focused on infants' interpretations of these questions, we introduce a test to probe their underlying syntactic representations, independent of meaning. We ask when infants know that an object wh-phrase and a local object of a verb cannot co-occur because they both express the same argument relation (e.g., * What did the chef burn the pizza ). We find that 1) 18 mo olds demonstrate awareness of this complementary distribution pattern and thus represent the nonlocal grammatical dependency between the wh-phrase and the verb, but 2) younger infants do not. These results suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development, during which the computational capacities for representing nonlocal syntactic dependencies become evident.The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode potentially unbounded dependencies over abstract structural configurations. How does such a system develop in human minds? We show that 18-mo-old infants are capable of representing abstract nonlocal dependencies, suggesting that a core property of syntax emerges early in development. Our test case is English wh-questions, in which a fronted wh-phrase can act as the argument of a verb at a distance (e.g., What did the chef burn?). Whereas prior work has focused on infants' interpretations of these questions, we introduce a test to probe their underlying syntactic representations, independent of meaning. We ask when infants know that an object wh-phrase and a local object of a verb cannot co-occur because they both express the same argument relation (e.g., * What did the chef burn the pizza ). We find that 1) 18 mo olds demonstrate awareness of this complementary distribution pattern and thus represent the nonlocal grammatical dependency between the wh-phrase and the verb, but 2) younger infants do not. These results suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development, during which the computational capacities for representing nonlocal syntactic dependencies become evident.
The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode potentially unbounded dependencies over abstract structural configurations. How does such a system develop in human minds? We show that 18-mo-old infants are capable of representing abstract nonlocal dependencies, suggesting that a core property of syntax emerges early in development. Our test case is English -questions, in which a fronted -phrase can act as the argument of a verb at a distance (e.g., ). Whereas prior work has focused on infants' interpretations of these questions, we introduce a test to probe their underlying syntactic representations, independent of meaning. We ask when infants know that an object -phrase and a local object of a verb cannot co-occur because they both express the same argument relation (e.g., * ). We find that 1) 18 mo olds demonstrate awareness of this complementary distribution pattern and thus represent the nonlocal grammatical dependency between the -phrase and the verb, but 2) younger infants do not. These results suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development, during which the computational capacities for representing nonlocal syntactic dependencies become evident.
As mature speakers of a language, we are able to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences. This ability comes from a powerful cognitive system—syntax—whose properties reveal the types of computations that human minds can engage in. One core property is the capacity to encode abstract grammatical dependencies that can hold at a distance. When does this property emerge in development? We identify a nonlocal dependency that 18-mo-old infants, but not younger infants, represent syntactically. These findings suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development and that even before they regularly produce full sentences of their own, infants demonstrate some of the core computational capacities that syntax relies on. The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode potentially unbounded dependencies over abstract structural configurations. How does such a system develop in human minds? We show that 18-mo-old infants are capable of representing abstract nonlocal dependencies, suggesting that a core property of syntax emerges early in development. Our test case is English wh -questions, in which a fronted wh -phrase can act as the argument of a verb at a distance (e.g., What did the chef burn? ). Whereas prior work has focused on infants’ interpretations of these questions, we introduce a test to probe their underlying syntactic representations, independent of meaning. We ask when infants know that an object wh -phrase and a local object of a verb cannot co-occur because they both express the same argument relation (e.g., * What did the chef burn the pizza ). We find that 1) 18 mo olds demonstrate awareness of this complementary distribution pattern and thus represent the nonlocal grammatical dependency between the wh -phrase and the verb, but 2) younger infants do not. These results suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development, during which the computational capacities for representing nonlocal syntactic dependencies become evident.
The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of primitive linguistic elements to build arbitrarily complex expressions. The expressive power of syntax comes in part from its ability to encode potentially unbounded dependencies over abstract structural configurations. How does such a system develop in human minds? We show that 18-mo-old infants are capable of representing abstract nonlocal dependencies, suggesting that a core property of syntax emerges early in development. Our test case is English wh-questions, in which a fronted wh-phrase can act as the argument of a verb at a distance (e.g., What did the chef burn?). Whereas prior work has focused on infants' interpretations of these questions, we introduce a test to probe their underlying syntactic representations, independent of meaning. We ask when infants know that an object wh-phrase and a local object of a verb cannot co-occur because they both express the same argument relation (e.g., *What did the chef burn the pizza). We find that 1) 18 mo olds demonstrate awareness of this complementary distribution pattern and thus represent the nonlocal grammatical dependency between the wh-phrase and the verb, but 2) younger infants do not. These results suggest that the second year of life is a period of active syntactic development, during which the computational capacities for representing nonlocal syntactic dependencies become evident.
Author Perkins, Laurel
Lidz, Jeffrey
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Laurel
  surname: Perkins
  fullname: Perkins, Laurel
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Jeffrey
  surname: Lidz
  fullname: Lidz, Jeffrey
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34607945$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kTtrHDEUhYVxsNdOalcJA2ncjK3njNQEgp8BQ5qkFhrpjlfLrDSRtAH_-2hZZ2O7CAhU6Dvnnqtzgg5DDIDQGcEXBPfscg4mX1BMO94pQuQBWhCsSNtxhQ_RAmPat5JTfoxOcl5hjJWQ-AgdM97hXnGxQNc3_nFZAEK7jqEs2zi5xofRhJKbBHOCDKE0deoUrZma_BSKscXbxsEMwUGwHvJ79G40U4YPz_cp-nl78-Pqvn34fvft6utDazlnpWWDwFRZxxXDozCYC-VcPZaPbhigk0oIKhxTxBkYDLga1nEjCfRGMmbYKfqy8503wxqcrdGSmfSc_NqkJx2N169fgl_qx_hbS0FJ14tqcP5skOKvDeSi1z5bmCYTIG6ypqJXTGLV0Yp-foOu4iaFul6lJBNSVaxSn14m2kf5-8EVuNwBNsWcE4x7hGC9rVBvK9T_KqwK8UZhfTHFx-1KfvqP7uNOt8olpv0Y2mPFmJLsD01Wq6I
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2026469118
crossref_primary_10_1177_01427237241252873
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_024_79531_8
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_langsci_2023_101585
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13080
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ssaho_2025_101332
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_linguistics_030521_045816
crossref_primary_10_1007_s10339_023_01140_6
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2201651119
crossref_primary_10_1080_10489223_2022_2147840
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_05122_0
crossref_primary_10_1017_S0305000924000588
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ssaho_2023_100648
crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2024_2372663
Cites_doi 10.1126/science.1196404
10.1126/science.aaa3799
10.3758/s13428-016-0809-y
10.18637/jss.v067.i01
10.1073/pnas.2026469118
10.1353/lan.0.0107
10.1017/S0305000903005695
10.7551/mitpress/5929.001.0001
10.1086/431525
10.1515/9783112316009
10.1207/s15327078in0702_4
10.1371/journal.pone.0036399
10.1121/1.3486197
10.1038/ncomms9537
10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00157-3
10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.704
10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-125236
10.1007/978-94-009-2727-8_4
10.1080/10489223.2015.1115048
10.1353/lan.2018.0019
10.1017/S0305000916000209
10.1207/s15327078in1002_5
10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00060-2
10.1080/10489223.2020.1776010
10.1109/TIT.1956.1056813
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.06.002
10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00069-4
10.1207/S15327078IN0104_9
10.1126/science.298.5598.1569
10.1080/10489223.1995.9671658
10.1111/1467-9280.00476
10.1207/s15473341lld0204_3
10.1080/10489223.2019.1659274
10.1016/j.cognition.2011.03.004
10.1207/S15327078IN0403_06
10.18637/jss.v082.i13
10.1023/A:1023255705029
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2021 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.
Copyright National Academy of Sciences Oct 12, 2021
Copyright © 2021 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2021
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2021 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.
– notice: Copyright National Academy of Sciences Oct 12, 2021
– notice: Copyright © 2021 the Author(s). Published by PNAS. 2021
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QG
7QL
7QP
7QR
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TK
7TM
7TO
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
M7N
P64
RC3
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1073/pnas.2026469118
DatabaseName CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)
Immunology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
Engineering Research Database
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Genetics Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Nucleic Acids Abstracts
Ecology Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management
Entomology Abstracts
Genetics Abstracts
Animal Behavior Abstracts
Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)
Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)
AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Immunology Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList
MEDLINE - Academic
MEDLINE
CrossRef

Virology and AIDS Abstracts
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Sciences (General)
EISSN 1091-6490
EndPage 7
ExternalDocumentID PMC8521675
34607945
10_1073_pnas_2026469118
27093398
Genre Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S
Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: National Science Foundation (NSF)
  grantid: #BCS-1827709
– fundername: National Science Foundation (NSF)
  grantid: #BCS-1551629
– fundername: National Science Foundation (NSF)
  grantid: #DGE-1449815
GroupedDBID ---
-DZ
-~X
.55
0R~
123
29P
2AX
2FS
2WC
4.4
53G
5RE
5VS
85S
AACGO
AAFWJ
AANCE
ABBHK
ABOCM
ABPLY
ABPPZ
ABTLG
ABXSQ
ABZEH
ACGOD
ACIWK
ACNCT
ACPRK
AENEX
AEUPB
AEXZC
AFFNX
AFOSN
AFRAH
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
BKOMP
CS3
D0L
DCCCD
DIK
DU5
E3Z
EBS
F5P
FRP
GX1
H13
HH5
HYE
IPSME
JAAYA
JBMMH
JENOY
JHFFW
JKQEH
JLS
JLXEF
JPM
JSG
JST
KQ8
L7B
LU7
N9A
N~3
O9-
OK1
PNE
PQQKQ
R.V
RHI
RNA
RNS
RPM
RXW
SA0
SJN
TAE
TN5
UKR
W8F
WH7
WOQ
WOW
X7M
XSW
Y6R
YBH
YKV
YSK
ZCA
~02
~KM
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
RHF
VQA
YIF
YIN
7QG
7QL
7QP
7QR
7SN
7SS
7T5
7TK
7TM
7TO
7U9
8FD
C1K
FR3
H94
M7N
P64
RC3
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c443t-3b5029cd4930f5a0459dd9ddc4fdbbe6895525d391daebaed580d4a81e7a833a3
ISSN 0027-8424
1091-6490
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 14:30:36 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 11:41:25 EDT 2025
Mon Jun 30 08:13:03 EDT 2025
Wed Feb 19 02:27:47 EST 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:03:04 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:55:33 EDT 2025
Thu May 29 08:53:13 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 41
Keywords nonlocal dependencies
syntax
wh-questions
language acquisition
Language English
License Copyright © 2021 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.
This open access article is distributed under Creative Commons Attribution License 4.0 (CC BY).
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c443t-3b5029cd4930f5a0459dd9ddc4fdbbe6895525d391daebaed580d4a81e7a833a3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
Edited by Toben H. Mintz, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, and accepted by Editorial Board Member Renée Baillargeon August 18, 2021 (received for review December 23, 2020)
Author contributions: L.P. and J.L. designed research; L.P. performed research; L.P. and J.L. analyzed data; and L.P. and J.L. wrote the paper.
ORCID 0000-0001-8829-1495
0000-0003-0719-9510
OpenAccessLink https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC8521675
PMID 34607945
PQID 2583589096
PQPubID 42026
PageCount 7
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_8521675
proquest_miscellaneous_2579380962
proquest_journals_2583589096
pubmed_primary_34607945
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_2026469118
crossref_citationtrail_10_1073_pnas_2026469118
jstor_primary_27093398
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2021-10-12
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2021-10-12
PublicationDate_xml – month: 10
  year: 2021
  text: 2021-10-12
  day: 12
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: Washington
PublicationTitle Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS
PublicationTitleAlternate Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
PublicationYear 2021
Publisher National Academy of Sciences
Publisher_xml – name: National Academy of Sciences
References e_1_3_4_7_2
e_1_3_4_40_2
Chomsky N. (e_1_3_4_13_2) 2010
e_1_3_4_23_2
e_1_3_4_21_2
e_1_3_4_42_2
e_1_3_4_48_2
Hirsh-Pasek K. (e_1_3_4_19_2) 1996
e_1_3_4_25_2
e_1_3_4_29_2
Hunter M. A. (e_1_3_4_35_2) 1988; 5
Pinker S. (e_1_3_4_4_2) 1984
Goldberg A. E. (e_1_3_4_47_2) 2006
Denison S. (e_1_3_4_39_2) 2012
e_1_3_4_30_2
e_1_3_4_51_2
e_1_3_4_34_2
e_1_3_4_57_2
e_1_3_4_32_2
e_1_3_4_59_2
e_1_3_4_38_2
e_1_3_4_36_2
e_1_3_4_17_2
Elman J. L. (e_1_3_4_46_2) 1996
Valian V. (e_1_3_4_54_2) 1990
Bresnan J. (e_1_3_4_5_2) 1982; 13
e_1_3_4_41_2
e_1_3_4_6_2
Everett D. L. (e_1_3_4_15_2) 2012
Tincoff R. (e_1_3_4_27_2) 2000
e_1_3_4_22_2
e_1_3_4_45_2
Chomsky N. (e_1_3_4_1_2) 1965
e_1_3_4_20_2
e_1_3_4_26_2
Ross J. R. (e_1_3_4_10_2) 1967
e_1_3_4_24_2
e_1_3_4_28_2
Cohen L. B. (e_1_3_4_55_2) 2004
Shieber S. M. (e_1_3_4_11_2) 1985
Jin K.-S. (e_1_3_4_44_2) 2014
Perkins L. (e_1_3_4_43_2) 2019
Chomsky N. (e_1_3_4_2_2) 1975
Tomasello M. (e_1_3_4_49_2) 2003
e_1_3_4_52_2
Chomsky N. (e_1_3_4_9_2) 1977
Fodor J. A. (e_1_3_4_3_2) 1966
e_1_3_4_12_2
e_1_3_4_33_2
e_1_3_4_58_2
e_1_3_4_31_2
Chomsky N. (e_1_3_4_8_2) 1963
e_1_3_4_16_2
e_1_3_4_37_2
e_1_3_4_14_2
e_1_3_4_56_2
Lightfoot D. (e_1_3_4_53_2) 1991
e_1_3_4_18_2
Pinker S. (e_1_3_4_50_2) 1989
References_xml – ident: e_1_3_4_42_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.1196404
– start-page: 71
  volume-title: Formal Syntax
  year: 1977
  ident: e_1_3_4_9_2
– volume-title: Reflections on Language
  year: 1975
  ident: e_1_3_4_2_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_41_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.aaa3799
– ident: e_1_3_4_58_2
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0809-y
– start-page: 54
  volume-title: The Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic Perspectives
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_3_4_13_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_56_2
  doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
– ident: e_1_3_4_59_2
  doi: 10.1073/pnas.2026469118
– ident: e_1_3_4_16_2
  doi: 10.1353/lan.0.0107
– ident: e_1_3_4_29_2
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000903005695
– volume-title: Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development
  year: 1996
  ident: e_1_3_4_46_2
  doi: 10.7551/mitpress/5929.001.0001
– ident: e_1_3_4_14_2
  doi: 10.1086/431525
– ident: e_1_3_4_7_2
  doi: 10.1515/9783112316009
– volume: 5
  start-page: 69
  year: 1988
  ident: e_1_3_4_35_2
  article-title: A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli
  publication-title: Adv. Infancy Res.
– volume-title: A New Program for Obtaining and Organizing Data in Infant Perception and Cognition Studies (Version 1.0)
  year: 2004
  ident: e_1_3_4_55_2
– volume-title: Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure
  year: 1989
  ident: e_1_3_4_50_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_23_2
  doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0702_4
– ident: e_1_3_4_34_2
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036399
– volume: 13
  start-page: 613
  year: 1982
  ident: e_1_3_4_5_2
  article-title: Cross-serial dependencies in Dutch
  publication-title: Linguist. Inq.
– ident: e_1_3_4_28_2
  doi: 10.1121/1.3486197
– volume-title: How Grammars Grow: Argument Structure and the Acquisition of Non-basic Syntax
  year: 2019
  ident: e_1_3_4_43_2
– volume-title: Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language
  year: 2006
  ident: e_1_3_4_47_2
– start-page: 269
  volume-title: Handbook of Mathematical Psychology
  year: 1963
  ident: e_1_3_4_8_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_40_2
  doi: 10.1038/ncomms9537
– ident: e_1_3_4_32_2
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00157-3
– ident: e_1_3_4_37_2
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.704
– start-page: 105
  volume-title: The Genesis of Language: A Psycholinguistic Approach
  year: 1966
  ident: e_1_3_4_3_2
– volume-title: Language Learnability and Language Development
  year: 1984
  ident: e_1_3_4_4_2
– volume-title: Language: The Cultural Tool
  year: 2012
  ident: e_1_3_4_15_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_52_2
  doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-125236
– volume-title: Aspects of the Theory of Syntax
  year: 1965
  ident: e_1_3_4_1_2
– start-page: 79
  volume-title: Philosophy, Language, and Artificial Intelligence
  year: 1985
  ident: e_1_3_4_11_2
  doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-2727-8_4
– ident: e_1_3_4_18_2
  doi: 10.1080/10489223.2015.1115048
– volume-title: Language Processing and Language Acquisition
  year: 1990
  ident: e_1_3_4_54_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_17_2
  doi: 10.1353/lan.2018.0019
– ident: e_1_3_4_31_2
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000916000209
– ident: e_1_3_4_33_2
  doi: 10.1207/s15327078in1002_5
– ident: e_1_3_4_26_2
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00060-2
– ident: e_1_3_4_38_2
  doi: 10.1080/10489223.2020.1776010
– start-page: 105
  volume-title: Methods for Assessing Children’s Syntax
  year: 1996
  ident: e_1_3_4_19_2
– start-page: 726
  volume-title: Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development
  year: 2000
  ident: e_1_3_4_27_2
– volume-title: How to Set Parameters: Arguments from Language Change
  year: 1991
  ident: e_1_3_4_53_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_6_2
  doi: 10.1109/TIT.1956.1056813
– ident: e_1_3_4_45_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.06.002
– ident: e_1_3_4_48_2
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00069-4
– ident: e_1_3_4_36_2
  doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0104_9
– ident: e_1_3_4_12_2
  doi: 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569
– start-page: 27
  volume-title: Advances in Child Development and Behavior
  year: 2012
  ident: e_1_3_4_39_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_30_2
  doi: 10.1080/10489223.1995.9671658
– volume-title: Proceedings of the 38th Boston University Conference on Language Development
  year: 2014
  ident: e_1_3_4_44_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_22_2
  doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00476
– ident: e_1_3_4_24_2
  doi: 10.1207/s15473341lld0204_3
– ident: e_1_3_4_20_2
  doi: 10.1080/10489223.2019.1659274
– volume-title: Constraints on Variables in Syntax
  year: 1967
  ident: e_1_3_4_10_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_25_2
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.03.004
– volume-title: Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition
  year: 2003
  ident: e_1_3_4_49_2
– ident: e_1_3_4_21_2
  doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0403_06
– ident: e_1_3_4_57_2
  doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13
– ident: e_1_3_4_51_2
  doi: 10.1023/A:1023255705029
SSID ssj0009580
Score 2.4634194
Snippet The human ability to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences is driven by syntax, a cognitive system that can combine a finite number of...
As mature speakers of a language, we are able to produce and understand an indefinite number of sentences. This ability comes from a powerful cognitive...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
jstor
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 1
SubjectTerms Cognition - physiology
Cognitive ability
Comprehension
Computer applications
Female
Human performance
Humans
Infant
Infants
Language Development
Male
Questions
Sentences
Social Sciences
Speech - physiology
Syntax
Title Eighteen-month-old infants represent nonlocal syntactic dependencies
URI https://www.jstor.org/stable/27093398
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34607945
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2583589096
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2579380962
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC8521675
Volume 118
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Rb9MwELbKeOEFMWAQGChIPAxVLklsx_bjBEMTgqoPm9QnIid2tElTN9H0Zb-es2M76egQIFVRlTh25Lucv3PuvkPoPSCOTPGa46xmBNOiFVhJqbAhhUXnrdKu1uH3eXl6Tr8u2XIy-TGKWtp09ay53ZlX8j9ShXMgV5sl-w-SjZ3CCfgP8oUjSBiOfyXjE-tZgyOKYcTuAl9fWSKl1kW2OLJKm1g0Bf_eLViWnKBzKVHTUPm2CRGEHp0u4mq2DrED87BZeDyknnh7sJ7i6WI-FDJeGLvvvg7Z1ibGbny71LejrLHxRkPhIt3ywS3903hjC1vAqkf7vOiZ6Y0qYBJc0r4saLS6g9ndBPIrb0Tz0WrMd5p5sEu2NvFKWcJ1gHSlDN1tc2cX3G7YSPEAPSzAjXClPZb5iJRZ9ClK_pkD9RMnH-_0vYVa-sDVXS7J3cjaEVQ5e4Ieex8jPe4VZh9NzOop2g-zmB55qvEPz9Dn3zUo9RqURg1KgwalUYPSsQY9R-dfTs4-nWJfVwM3lJIOk5plhWw0lSRrmQJQL7WGX0NbXdemFJKxgmkic61MrYyGadJUidxwJQhR5ADtwcjmJUpbyqhsGy4A6FOhWilKWQPCBJDLOYDRBM3CvFWNJ523tU-uKhf8wEllJ7oaJjpBR_GGm55v5f6mB04QsV2QdoIOg2Qq_7bCfQx8DSHBY0_Qu3gZbKn9QKZW5npj28BqJaBJkaAXvSBj54SWGaxdLEF8S8SxgeVp376yurxwfO0CIDL45a_ue97X6NHwvh2ive7nxrwBqNvVb52-_gKnw6tM
linkProvider Geneva Foundation for Medical Education and Research
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Eighteen-month-old+infants+represent+nonlocal+syntactic+dependencies&rft.jtitle=Proceedings+of+the+National+Academy+of+Sciences+-+PNAS&rft.au=Perkins%2C+Laurel&rft.au=Lidz%2C+Jeffrey&rft.date=2021-10-12&rft.pub=National+Academy+of+Sciences&rft.issn=0027-8424&rft.eissn=1091-6490&rft.volume=118&rft.issue=41&rft.spage=1&rft.epage=7&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073%2Fpnas.2026469118&rft.externalDocID=27093398
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0027-8424&client=summon